Figures
Abstract
Several studies have recently suggested that an abnormal processing of respiratory interoceptive and nociceptive (painful) stimuli may contribute to eating disorder (ED) pathophysiology. Mood and anxiety disorders (MA) are also characterized by abnormal respiratory symptoms, and show substantial comorbidity with ED. However, no studies have examined both respiratory and pain processing simultaneously within ED and MA. The present study systematically evaluated responses to perturbations of respiratory and nociceptive signals across the levels of physiology, behavior, and symptom report in a transdiagnostic ED sample (n = 51) that was individually matched to MA individuals (n = 51) and healthy comparisons (HC; n = 51). Participants underwent an inspiratory breath-holding challenge as a probe of respiratory interoception and a cold pressor challenge as a probe of pain processing. We expected both clinical groups to report greater stress and fear in response to respiratory and nociceptive perturbation than HCs, in the absence of differential physiological and behavioral responses. During breath-holding, both the ED and MA groups reported significantly more stress, feelings of suffocation, and suffocation fear than HC, with the ED group reporting the most severe symptoms. Moreover, anxiety sensitivity was related to suffocation fear only in the ED group. The heightened affective responses in the current study occurred in the absence of group differences in behavioral (breath hold duration, cold pressor duration) and physiological (end-tidal carbon dioxide, end-tidal oxygen, heart rate, skin conductance) responses. Against our expectations, there were no group differences in the response to cold pain stimulation. A matched-subgroup analysis focusing on individuals with anorexia nervosa (n = 30) produced similar results. These findings underscore the presence of abnormal respiratory interoception in MA and suggest that hyperreactivity to respiratory signals may be a potentially overlooked clinical feature of ED.
Citation: Lapidus RC, Puhl M, Kuplicki R, Stewart JL, Paulus MP, Rhudy JL, et al. (2020) Heightened affective response to perturbation of respiratory but not pain signals in eating, mood, and anxiety disorders. PLoS ONE 15(7): e0235346. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346
Editor: Kristin Vickers, Ryerson University, CANADA
Received: January 9, 2020; Accepted: June 14, 2020; Published: July 15, 2020
Copyright: © 2020 Lapidus et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; https://www.nimh.nih.gov/index.shtml) grant number K23MH112949 (SSK), (National Institute of General Medical Sciences NIGMS; https://www.nigms.nih.gov) grant number P20GM121312 (SSK, MPP, JSF, and JLS), the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (https://www.bbrfoundation.org) (SSK), and The William K. Warren Foundation (https://www.williamkwarrenfoundation.org). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Eating disorders (ED) are deadly illnesses that often begin in adolescence or young adulthood, and maintain a chronic course associated with severe impairments of emotional, social, cognitive, and physical functioning, and a low quality of life [1–4]. Despite the devastation caused by these disorders, many of the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying the development and maintenance of ED remain poorly understood.
Interoceptive dysfunction is one component that has been conceptually implicated in ED expression [5]. For example, it has been suggested that the symptoms of certain EDs may be influenced by a perceptual amplification of physiological signals such that changes in the internal body state induce a hyperreactivity characterized by fearful responses to food related stimuli [6]. Supporting this notion are findings that individuals with bulimia nervosa (BN) experience heightened stomach fullness after a standardized non-nutrient water load [7] and individuals with anorexia nervosa (AN) exhibit increased activation of the insular cortex during interoceptive attention to stomach sensations [8]. BN individuals report higher symptoms of anxiety and panic than healthy comparisons (HC) in response to breathing 35% carbon dioxide (CO2)-enriched air, but at similar levels as individuals with panic disorder, suggesting that amplifications of interoceptive processing might also extend to respiratory signals [9]. In previous interoceptive processing using the adrenaline analogue isoproterenol, we found that individuals with AN reported a greater intensity of cardiorespiratory sensations relative to HC during meal anticipation, an effect that was particularly pronounced for sensations of dyspnea [10]. These individuals were less accurate at localizing cardiovascular sensations [11], suggesting an inability to accurately discriminate interoceptive signals from different regions in the body despite a tendency to perceive them more intensely.
Nociceptive signals are often considered a component of interoceptive processing [12], and while studies have investigated differences in pain processing in ED, the available evidence is mixed. Acutely ill individuals with AN and BN demonstrate higher pain thresholds to heat stimuli [13–17], and BN show higher pain thresholds to pressure stimuli [15, 16]. However, recovered AN report more unpleasantness during heat pain stimulation than HC in the context of decreased insula responses [18]. These findings suggest that perceptions of heat and pressure pain intensity may be dampened despite affectively experiencing these signals as more aversive. In contrast to studies of heat pain, studies assessing cold pain have not found any evidence of elevated pain thresholds in acute AN or BN patients [19, 20]. Some of these studies were distinguished by methodological limitations (e.g. using suboptimal cold pain stimulation via placing a back of ice on the hand instead of the standard hand immersion in circulating icy water [19]) suggesting that further investigation of cold nociception is warranted.
Over 70% of individuals with EDs exhibit at least one comorbid lifetime mood or anxiety disorder (MA) [21], suggesting that disentangling the interoceptive pathophysiology of ED will require an effort to account for the routinely high rates of comorbidity that are observed with MA. This is especially important as interoceptive dysfunction has also been implicated in the pathophysiology of these disorders [22]. For example, similarly to individuals with EDs, individuals with MA exhibit higher self-reported negative affect and panic symptoms than HC in response to cardiorespiratory physiological challenges, largely in the absence of differential physiological responding [23–25]. There is also mixed evidence for abnormal pain processing in MA disorders [see 26–29]. Therefore, in the current study we planned on the inclusion of a MA control group (as recommended previously [30]), using a standardized set of interoceptive and nociceptive stimulation protocols.
Anxiety sensitivity (AS) [31] is a transdiagnostic trait that may explain abnormal interoception across both ED and MA disorders, based on a tendency towards hyperreactivity to anxiety-inducing cues. For example, when compared to low-AS counterparts, non-clinical individuals with high AS are more likely to report panic symptoms during respiratory challenges [32–34]. Heightened AS is also strongly associated with fearful appraisals of pain, but modestly associated with pain tolerance [35], suggesting the possibility that AS influences the processing of sensory signals beyond those involved in anxiety. AS and responses to respiratory perturbations have been studied extensively in individuals with anxiety disorders (see [36–38] for meta-analytic reviews). However, although existing evidence supports a role for AS in anxiety expression, it has received limited study in ED [39–41].
In the current study we examined physiological, sensory and affective responses to the modulation of two different aversive physiological states (inspiratory breath holding or hand immersion in cold water), in ED and MA participants relative to demographically matched HCs. During both tasks we measured biological reactivity in the form of physiological signal changes (HR, RR, SCL), subjective responses via self-reported intensity and valence of the associated emotional feeling, and objective behavioral responses (exhalation or hand withdrawal). Although interoceptive accuracy is a common experimental measure relating the closeness of a self-report of physiological signal with the actual signal, we did not measure this in the current study as it is not typically measured in these tasks. We selected these probes on the basis that they are minimally invasive, quickly administered, perturb physiological, behavioral and symptom parameters, and can be readily translated to clinical settings. Our goal was to examine whether the acute modulation of respiratory or nociceptive signals elicits abnormal processing in a transdiagnostic sample of ED (spanning the ED diagnoses of AN, BN, or an unspecified eating disorder) in relation to a demographically matched transdiagnostic sample of individuals with mood and/or anxiety disorders (MA) or to demographically matched HCs. We were interested in whether individuals with EDs would demonstrate similar responses to individuals with MA on both tasks, potentially suggesting the possibility of shared underlying etiological processes. Given the inherent heterogeneity of transdiagnostic samples, and the preponderance of studies focusing exclusively on AN, we planned to conduct a sub-group analysis in individuals with this diagnosis to examine whether abnormalities of respiratory and pain processing would be restricted to this category.
For the current study, we tested: 1) whether physiological responses to breath holding and cold pain (indexed via heart rate (HR), electrodermal activity (EDA), respiration rate (RR), or concentrations of CO2 or oxygen (O2) would differentiate the ED, MA, or HC groups; 2) whether negatively-valenced subjective experiences of breath holding and cold pain (indexed by ratings of stress, suffocation feelings, suffocation fear for breath hold, and pain intensity for cold pressor) would be greater than HC for either the ED group, MA group, or both; and 3) whether affective responses to interoceptive perturbations would relate to anxiety sensitivity within each group.
Materials and methods
Participants
Participants were drawn from two studies at the Laureate Institute for Brain Research, one of which included the Tulsa 1000, a naturalistic study focused on assessment and prediction of psychiatric outcomes in 1000 treatment-seeking individuals with MA, substance use, ED, or HC [42]. We examined data from a total of 153 participants: 51 individuals (49 female) with current or lifetime ED who were individually matched using age, sex, gender, and body mass index (BMI) to 51 individuals with current MA, and 51 HC. Individuals with lifetime EDs were allowed to be included in the ED group, as previous findings have suggested that abnormal characteristics associated with ED diagnosis, such as perfectionism, cognitive rigidity, and extreme food preferences can persist after recovery [43–45]. These characteristics in the recovery period are paralleled by maintenance of altered neural responding [46–48], suggesting the residual presence of an ‘illness scar’ that can elucidate pathophysiological processes underlying ED. All participants were required to have a BMI >17 in order to avoid acute physiological-related confounds due to severe malnutrition [49].
Participants were diagnostically grouped based on a structured diagnostic clinical interview (MINI 6.0 or 7.0) [50]. To be included in the ED group, participants were required to meet current/lifetime diagnostic criteria for AN, BN, or an eating disorder not otherwise specified. ED participants were permitted to have co-occurring MA disorders given the high comorbidity rates of these disorders [51, 52]. Inclusion in the MA group required the current diagnosis of a mood or anxiety disorder via the MINI interview, and a self-rated screening scale score of ≥ 10 on the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) [53] and/or ≥ 8 on the Overall Anxiety Severity and Impairment Scale (OASIS) [54]. A score of < 2 on the Sick, Control, One, Fat, Food (SCOFF) Questionnaire [55] and/or absence of a current or prior history of an ED, as determined through a structured life chart interview [56] was also required to be in the MA group. Individuals in the HC group were negative for any psychiatric disorder per the MINI interview, and reported symptom scores beneath the clinical cutoffs on all of the self-rated screening scales. Psychosis, bipolar disorder, moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury or other neurocognitive disorder, and pregnancy were exclusionary for all participants. Additionally, individuals reporting active suicidal ideation with intent or plan were excluded for safety reasons and were provided with appropriate clinical resources. Obsessive compulsive disorder was exclusionary for the Tulsa 1000 study. Patients taking psychotropic medications were allowed, provided the dose had been stable for at least two weeks. Written informed consent was obtained for all participants as approved by the Western Institutional Review Board screening protocol #20101611, and all subjects received compensation for their participation.
Procedure
Participants were asked to abstain from central nervous system stimulants and depressants such as caffeine, nicotine, amphetamines, or alcohol the day of their appointment. Participants were instructed by computer as they completed a series of behavioral tasks, including the inspiratory breath hold and cold pressor tasks. The breath hold and cold pressor tasks were separated by a heartbeat tapping task that was not reported on in this study. In the heartbeat tapping task, individuals were asked to focus on the sensation of their heartbeat and to press a key on a keyboard when they felt their heartbeat. During this trial, participants were also asked to hold their breath while tapping to amplify interoceptive sensations. This task lasted for a maximum of one minute followed by approximately 30 seconds of time in which participants entered subjective ratings of the task using a keyboard. The cold pressor task began next, with approximately three minutes of instruction and equipment set-up occurring prior to the participant submerging their hand in the water. Altogether, this resulted in a three-and-a-half minute delay between the final breath hold in the heartbeat tapping task and the cold pressor task, allowing ample time for recovery following a breath hold in the previous task [57]. For a minority of participants, the breath hold and cold pressor tasks were conducted on different days due to technical difficulties. In these instances, participants were rescheduled to complete the cold pressor task at their earliest convenience (typically less than a week later).
Physiological signals measured during these tasks were collected continuously at 1000 Hz and included the electrocardiogram (ECG; lead II configuration), electrodermal activity (electrodes placed on the middle phalanx of index and middle fingers), and respiration (via transducer belt), (MP510, Biopac Systems Inc., Lehigh, Pennsylvania). Percent oxygen (O2) and carbon dioxide (CO2) concentrations were collected prior to and following breath hold trials using an Oxigraf capnometer (Sunnyvale, CA).
Inspiratory breath hold (BH) challenge.
The BH challenge consisted of two trials (BH1 and BH2), each limited to a two-minute maximum duration breath hold [58]. Two trials were used as previous work has shown increased breath hold times following repeated trials [59]. To avoid biased responding, participants were not informed of the maximum trial duration. Participants were provided with a nose clip to prevent inadvertent respirations. They were then instructed to exhale all the air out of their lungs, take a deep breath in, hold it for as long as they could tolerate, and then exhale into the capnometer. BH trials were separated by approximately a two-minute recovery period. Immediately afterwards participants rated the subjective effort, unpleasantness, intensity, difficulty, stress, breathlessness, urge to breathe, feelings of suffocation, and suffocation fear experienced during the task using a visual analogue scale (VAS) that ranged from 0 (“not at all”) to 100 (“extremely”).
Cold pressor (CP) challenge.
The CP challenge consisted of one trial with a two-minute maximum duration. Participants were instructed to keep their dominant hand submerged up to the wrist in cold water continuously circulating at a constant temperature of five degrees Celsius (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA) for as long as they could tolerate. To avoid biased responding, participants were not informed of the maximum trial duration. Throughout the challenge, participants made continuous real-time ratings of their pain intensity as it increased and decreased on a scale ranging from 0 (no pain) to 100 (worst pain imaginable). These ratings were used to calculate each individual’s peak pain rating, as well as the time to reach mild (25/100), moderate (50/100), and peak pain. Immediately afterwards they rated the degree of unpleasantness, stress, and difficulty experienced on a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 (none) to 100 (extremely).
Clinical symptoms.
Self-report scales were used to quantify ED pathology including the Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale (EDDS) [60] and the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire-6 (EDE-Q) [61]. All participants completed the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI), a transdiagnostic trait measure of the fear of experiencing anxiety-related symptoms and sensations [31].
Physiological data processing
Physiological data were imported into R version 5.3.1 (R core team, 2013) for analysis. Outliers (e.g., signal interference or movement associated artifacts) were identified through a manual quality-control process and corrected by using a two-sided moving average linear filter. To determine how the breath hold and cold pressor tasks affected physiology, changes in heart rate, skin conductance, O2, and CO2 were calculated. For the heart rate, maximum increase/decrease change scores were calculated by subtracting the baseline heart rate (defined as the average heart rate during the 20 seconds prior to trial onset) from the maximum and minimum observed heart rate using a five second moving average window. A similar calculation was done with skin conductance. Average O2 and CO2 concentrations were examined before and immediately after each BH trial, and O2 and CO2 change scores were calculated by subtracting post-trial from pre-trial concentrations. Average heart rate and electrodermal activity (via skin conductance level, SCL) was also calculated across the entire window within each BH trial, as well as across the CP task.
Statistical analysis
To examine differences in BH responding, a mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was computed with group (ED, MA, and HC) as the between-subjects factor and trial (BH1, BH2) as the within-subjects factor for the following dependent variables: average HR, maximum HR increase, maximum HR decrease, average SCL, maximum SCL increase, maximum SCL decrease, pre/post CO2 change, pre/post O2 change, BH duration, and VAS ratings for effort, unpleasantness, intensity, difficulty, stress, urge to breathe, feelings of suffocation, and suffocation fear. For CP responses, one-way ANOVAs were conducted with group as the between-subjects factor for each of the following dependent variables: average HR, maximum HR increase, maximum HR decrease, average SCL, maximum SCL increase, maximum SCL decrease, trial duration, time to mild pain, time to moderate pain, time to peak pain, peak pain, and VAS ratings for unpleasantness, difficulty, and stress. Standard listwise deletion was used in the analysis to address missing data. Missing values were not imputed due to the large number of variables relative to the number of participants in each group. All results were corrected for multiple-comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. Significant ANOVA group main effects and interactions were evaluated via post-hoc two-sided t-tests. As no group by trial interactions were identified on the breath hold trials 1 and 2, average scores were calculated for use in t-tests. In the subgroup analysis, the same tests were conducted using only AN (n = 30) and individually AN-matched MA participants (n = 30) and HC (n = 30) participants. The small BN sample size (n = 14) prevented a comparable subgroup analysis. All analyses were conducted using R version 5.3.1 (R core team, 2013).
Results
Participant characteristics
The groups did not differ in age, sex, or BMI, but they differed with respect to measures of psychopathology (Table 1). The ED group endorsed significantly higher SCOFF scores than the MA group, who in turn endorsed significantly higher scores than HC. The ED and MA groups reported significantly higher OASIS, PHQ-9, ASI total, ASI cognitive concern, ASI physical concern, and ASI social concern scores than HC, but these groups did not differ from each other. The ED and MA groups exhibited generally similar amounts of comorbid disorders (Table 1), while the ED group had a significantly higher percentage of medicated individuals than the MA group (Table 1). Average ED severity scores were above the minimum cutoffs for both the EDDS and EDE-Q, indicative of a clinical ED sample (EDDS: average score = 33.9, 16.5 cutoff, [62]; EDE-Q: average score = 2.68, 2.30 cutoff [63]).
Missing data
There was data missing for several of the measures used within our sample. The two primary reasons related to differences in the ED symptom severity scales employed in the two studies, and to equipment malfunction in a subset of cases (e.g. capnometer not accurately recording O2 and CO2 levels during a BH trial). Regarding ED symptom severity self-report measures, 17% of data were missing for the EDDS and 69% of data were missing for the EDE-Q6. ED symptom severity was not used as an outcome measure of this study. For the BH task, less than 1% of the self-report data were missing across each variable and approximately 3–4% of the behavioral data were missing across each variable. For the physiological data in the breath hold task, respiration rate data were missing for between 6–7% for each variable, skin conductance level data were missing for between 2–4% for each variable, heartrate data was missing for between 6%-7% for each variable, with the exception of “heartrate change from baseline to minimum heartrate” for BH1 and BH2, which was missing at 18% and 19% respectively. For both BH1 and BH2, data for O2 and CO2 levels were missing at rates of 3–4% of each variable, with the exception of the following: baseline O2 and CO2 (16–21% missing per variable), post O2 and CO2 (10% missing per variable), and change in O2 and CO2 from baseline to post breath hold for BH1 and BH2 (19–23% per variable). For the CP task, less than 1% of data were missing across each self-report level variable and between 3–5% of data were missing across each behavioral level variable, with the exception of time to moderate pain, which was missing at 12%. At the physiological level, respiration rate and skin conductance level variables were missing at 3% each, and heartrate data was missing at 8–9% for each variable, with the exception of the variable heartrate change from baseline to minimum heartrate which was missing at 30%. Less than 1% of data were missing for each of the ASI-3 subscales. See S3 File for a detailed description of missing data for each variable sorted by participant group.
Physiology and behavior
Analysis of the available data revealed that both the BH and the CP tasks successfully modulated autonomic physiology (Fig 1) and behavior (Fig 2), with the majority of participants terminating each task before the maximum trial duration. Across all groups, both the BH and CP probes elicited initial elevations in heart rate followed by a slowing and reduction below baseline levels. However, despite the observed task-induced changes in physiology and behavior, there were no significant group main effects or interactions for either BH or CP (Table 2).
Black line = mean. Grey shaded area = SE. *** indicates significant difference at p < 0.001. Time 0 (s) = start of trial (inhale for BH, hand enters water for CP). Mean heart rate (beats per minute, BPM) or skin conductance level (microsiemens, μS) with shaded standard error for the entire sample (n = 144) for each task. Red dashed line indicates task onset. Late fluctuations may be related to subject dropout, as such BH1 was only graphed for 120 seconds. Red circles indicate time at which peak heart rate or SCL was achieved for each individual. Blue circles indicate time at which minimum heart rate or SCL was reached for each individual. Color saturation of circles indicates greater frequency. Individual groups were not plotted because no significant differences were observed between groups (see Table 2).
Exhalation ended the breath hold task. Hand removal from water ended the cold pressor task.
Symptom report
Although no between group differences in physiology and behavior were detected on the BH task, there were significant between group differences for reports of stress, F(2,149) = 6.34, p = 0.01, feelings of suffocation, F(2,149) = 7.51, p = 0.01, and suffocation fear, F(2,149) = 8.86, p = 0.002 (Table 2, Fig 3). Reports of effort, unpleasantness, intensity, difficulty, and breathlessness were not significantly different between groups. The MA and the ED groups both reported higher stress and higher suffocation fear than HC in response to breath holding, but they were not significantly different from each other (Table 3). The ED group reported the highest feelings of suffocation, at levels that were significantly greater than both MA and HC, while the MA group reported higher feelings of suffocation than the HC group (Table 3). The ED group showed larger effect sizes than MA for stress (0.69 vs 0.41 Cohen’s d) and suffocation fear (0.82 vs 0.68 Cohen’s d), although both groups did not significantly differ from each other on these ratings. Finally, despite a clear elicitation of pain ratings within each group, there were no significant between group differences in any of the reported pain symptoms during the CP task (Table 2).
Ratings could range from 0 ‘not at all’ to 100 ‘extremely’. HC = Healthy Comparison. MA = Mood/Anxiety. ED = Eating Disorder. * indicates significant difference at p < 0.05.
AN subgroup analysis
There were no between group differences in age, F(2,87) = 0.38, p = 0.69, or BMI, F(2,87) = 0.60, p = 0.55 (S1 Table). Both the AN and MA groups endorsed significantly higher OASIS, PHQ-9, and ASI total/subscale scores than HC but these did not differ from each other. The AN exhibited the highest SCOFF scores, followed by MA and then HC. Similar to the pattern of findings in the larger transdiagnostic sample, there were no group differences in physiology or behavior for BH, and no group differences in any of the CP variables (Table 4). At the symptom level, we observed group differences in feelings of suffocation, F(2,87) = 6.16, p = 0.027, and suffocation fear F(2,87) = 4.88, p = 0.045, (Table 4, S1 Fig). Within these, both the AN and MA groups reported higher feelings of suffocation and suffocation fear than HC, but they were not different from each other (S2 Table, S1 Fig). Again, although the AN and the MA groups did not significantly differ, the AN group demonstrated larger effect sizes for both feelings of suffocation (0.91 vs 0.56 Cohen’s d) and suffocation fear (0.80 vs 0.60 Cohen’s d).
Correlational analysis
To examine whether anxiety sensitivity was differentially related to suffocation fear in each group, we calculated Spearman’s rank-order correlations between ASI Total Score and each ASI subscale (cognitive concerns, physical concerns, social concerns) and average suffocation fear (S3 Table). After applying the Benjamini-Hochberg correction for false discovery rates, this analysis revealed that suffocation fears were positively correlated with ASI scores, but only in the ED group for the physical concerns subscale (rs = 0.50, p<0.0001; Fig 4).
Suffocation fear ratings could range from 0 ‘not at all’ to 100 ‘extremely’. Insets depict relationships between suffocation fear and anxiety sensitivity index subscales. HC = Healthy Comparison. MA = Mood/Anxiety. ED = Eating Disorder. pcorr = p value with Benjamini-Hochberg correction applied. *** indicates significance at p < 0.001.
Discussion
The present study examined physiological, behavioral, and subjective responses to the acute modulation of respiratory interoceptive and cold pain signals across demographically matched samples of participants with ED or MA, relative to HC. We observed significant task-related changes in physiological and behavioral responses during both tasks, but without group-level differences in these parameters. However, during inspiratory breath holding, we observed heightened levels of stress, feelings of suffocation, and suffocation fear in both ED and MA relative to healthy individuals, with the ED group reporting significantly higher feelings of suffocation than the MA group after correcting for multiple comparisons. The AN-only subgroup analysis revealed findings similar to the larger transdiagnostic ED sample, suggesting the possibility that heightened suffocation fear may be common across EDs. We did not identify any evidence of abnormal nociceptive responding to cold pain, at any level, for MA, ED, or the AN-only subgroup. Finally, we found that suffocation fears were correlated with anxiety sensitivity physical concerns, but only in the ED group.
Previous studies examining responses to homeostatic perturbations have shown that physiological responses do not differ between ED individuals and HC [10, 64–67], similar to the observations of the current study. While it would be premature to assume that there are no differences in peripheral (autonomic) physiology between these groups, the presence of group differences in symptom reports in the absence of differences in any of the physiological measures recorded in the current study suggests that affective response to breath holding may be localized to neural circuits within the central nervous system responsible for processing interoceptive threat. Confirming this possibility would require subsequent studies employing measurement of neural signals via functional neuroimaging or electroencephalography methods. Alternative processes such as attentional biases, or temperamental traits (e.g. interoceptive sensibility) could also play a role, although we would expect these processes to also be driven by central nervous system mechanisms. Supporting evidence for these arguments comes from prior studies comparing women recovered from AN to HC that observed aberrant anterior and dorsal-mid insular activity during interoceptive attention [8], and increased activation in the striatum, cingulate, and prefrontal cortices during loaded breathing [68] in the absence of clear differences in physiological responses. These findings also suggest some possible cortical loci of abnormal respiratory interoceptive sensation (i.e., insula, striatum, cingulate, frontal cortices). Taken together, these findings and the data from the current study emphasize two points: 1) the importance of measuring responses to physiological challenges across various levels of the nervous system to localize dysfunction, and 2) concomitant measurement of the associated central neural circuits may be necessary to adequately identify anatomical substrates responsible for generating abnormal interoceptive symptom reports.
The observation of heightened suffocation feelings and fear across both ED and MA in the absence of group differences in behavioral or physiological responses to breath holding is evocative of Klein's suffocation false alarm hypothesis [69]. This hypothesis proposed that a hypersensitivity to fluctuations in CO2 levels preferentially triggers misperceptions of suffocation in individuals with panic disorder. In congruence with this notion, previous studies have observed heightened anxiety and panic symptoms in various anxiety disorders in response to voluntary breath holding [58] and the 35% CO2 inhalation challenge [23, 24, 70]. The fact that a voluntary breath hold could elicit a fear of suffocation seems paradoxical, as subjects are always free to exhale and continue breathing. Yet, the repeated observation of such fears in breath hold studies speaks to the robustness of the phenomena. Our observation in the current study that CO2 changes and suffocation fear were not significantly correlated following the breath hold provides intriguing evidence for an argument that the task may have elicited a false suffocation alarm in both patient groups. To this point, in the current study the ED group actually demonstrated the lowest changes in CO2 from pre to post breath hold (though not significantly different), despite reporting the highest fears and feelings of suffocation (S4 Table). These suffocation fears also occurred despite the ED group’s higher rate of psychotropic medication usage, something that has been reported to reduce behavioral responses to CO2 exposure [71]. Although preliminary until replicated, we believe that the current findings could be interpreted as evidence in support of a hypersensitivity to CO2 in ED. Importantly, these findings suggest that suffocation feelings and fears are not exclusive to anxiety disorders, but are relevant to ED as well.
While this is the first study to explicitly examine suffocation fear in transdiagnostic EDs as well as in AN, previous studies documenting abnormal respiratory processing in ED can be interpreted as providing evidence of suffocation alarm misfires. For example, there are heightened reports of choking following loaded breathing in AN as compared to HC [68], elevated panic attack rates in BN similar to individuals with panic disorder after breathing 35% CO2 enriched air [9], and heightened ratings of dyspnea intensity in AN vs. HC during administration of isoproterenol, an adrenaline analogue [10]. In contrast, Perna et al., 2004 [72] found that in response to 35% CO2 inhalation, individuals with transdiagnostic EDs responded more similarly to HCs than to panic disorder patients with respect to panic-related sensations (e.g. trembling, palpitations) and subjective ratings. However, this study excluded ED individuals with comorbid diagnoses, and had a substantially smaller sample size than the current study (n = 15 per group), which may have contributed to the differences in findings. With this consideration in mind, our study points to interoceptive abnormalities in EDs that are not specific only to the gastrointestinal system, and thus have the potential to expand research and clinical attention to the examination of respiratory processes in ED patients.
Another aim of the current study was to examine whether individuals with EDs display exaggerated reactions to negatively-valenced cutaneous stimuli beyond the respiratory interoceptive domain (i.e., cold pain). In the current study we did not find any evidence of differential responding to cold pain. The lack of elevated self-report ratings during cold pain elicitation indicates that the heightened reports of stress and suffocation in ED cannot be attributable to a general amplification of distress across all domains of aversive sensory processing. Simultaneously, the lack of group differences with cold pain in the context of other studies finding elevated pain thresholds [13, 17] and heightened unpleasant experience in ED [18] to pain modalities (i.e. heat and pressure), suggests there is a complex relationship between nociceptive processing in ED. Perhaps, as in depression [27], the experience of pain is modality specific (e.g., heat pain eliciting different response from cold pain). Although speculative, responses to cold pain stimulation may also be blunted by chronic peripheral vasoconstriction in ED, an allostatic adjustment which is thought to occur in an effort to conserve heat [73]. In other words, we cannot rule out the possibility of a different outcome had we tested cold pain processing in the body trunk (e.g., via whole body cold plunge). Alternatively, as illness acuity influences nociceptive responding to heat pain and remits with unrestrained eating [13, 74], thermal pain thresholds may be a more relevant state factor for understanding pain processing in ED. Our study also cannot offer conclusions about nociceptive processing across each illness phase, as we did not focus our efforts on acutely ill ED individuals. However, this study represents the largest sample to date of cold pain processing in ED, and provides further evidence (i.e., beyond [19, 20]) that abnormal response to cold pain is not a characteristic of EDs and specifically, AN.
Lastly, we sought to examine the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and affective responses to interoceptive and nociceptive perturbations in ED and MA. While anxiety sensitivity is already identified as a transdiagnostic predictor of symptom response during interoceptive and nociceptive challenges in both HC [75] and MA [76, 77], little research has used behavioral measures to examine the relevance of anxiety sensitivity to ED despite the high comorbidity between ED and MA. Our observation that suffocation fears were significantly related to anxiety sensitivity only in the ED group reinforces the notion that the pathophysiology of EDs partially involves a process in which the perceptual amplification of certain physiological cues induces hyperreactivity and fearful responses. If such responses were generalized, individuals with EDs might be expected to show exaggerated interoceptive reactivity to changes not only in gut signals, but also to other interoceptive signals such as the breath and heartbeat, that contribute to emotional experience.
The results of this study point to a heightened sensitivity to dyspnea sensations at the self-report level in individuals with eating disorders, which may more broadly represent a difficulty adapting to internal shifts from homeostatic set points (as discussed in [68]). From this perspective, although speculative, we suspect that targeting heightened affective reactivity to the perturbation of interoceptive signals through interventions such as interoceptive exposure (as is being increasingly suggested [78, 79]) might help individuals with EDs to increase the perceived tolerance of discomforting body states. Whether such interventions might reduce pre-meal anxiety [80] and ultimately influence the avoidance of food consumption awaits empirical study.
Limitations
Several limitations of this study warrant acknowledgments. First, individuals in the ED sample had comorbid mood and anxiety disorders according to the structured clinical interviews employed. Allowing comorbid depression into the samples might be seen as limiting the ability to determine whether heightened aversive experience of respiratory perturbation is related primarily to eating or anxiety-related pathology. However, this limitation is tempered by 1) the fact that the majority of individuals with eating disorders have mood related diagnoses when assessed under structured conditions [81, 82], 2) the fact that ED group was shown to have both heightened suffocation feelings (relative to the MA group), and 3) the fact that the ED group was the only group to show a correlation between suffocation fear and AS physical concerns. Second, the majority of individuals in this study were female, and thus further study may be need to determine whether these findings apply to males with ED. Third, although the lack of physiological differences in several of our physiological variables (including “heartrate change from baseline to minimum heartrate” and “change in pre/post O2/CO2 saturation”) could be limited by higher percentages of missing data (16–30% missing and 19–23% missing, respectively), our suffocation feeling/fear symptom-level measures had the lowest levels of missing data (<1%) for each variable. Lastly, the current study design is unable to distinguish whether heightened suffocation fear in ED is due to a premorbid process or whether it is a residual “scar” of the disorder [83]. Future research could address this limitation through longitudinal assessments of adolescents to see whether abnormal respiratory interoception precedes ED onset, or alternatively, evaluate the relationship between suffocation fear and clinical severity across periods of relapse, remission, and recovery [84].
Conclusion
Individuals with ED and MA show evidence of heightened suffocation fear during inspiratory breath holding. Abnormal respiratory interoceptive processing may be an overlooked feature of ED psychopathology that warrants further investigation.
Supporting information
S1 Fig. AN subgroup analysis: Average feelings of suffocation and suffocation fear.
Ratings Range from 0 (not at all)– 100 (extremely). HC = Healthy Comparison. MA = Mood/Anxiety. AN = Anorexia Nervosa. * indicates significant difference at p < 0.05.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s001
(PDF)
S1 Table. Anorexia nervosa subgroup analysis: Demographics and screening scores at study entry.
HC = Healthy Comparison, MA = Mood/Anxiety, ED = Eating Disorder, BMI = Body Mass Index, SCOFF = Sick, Control, One Stone, Fat, Food Eating Disorder Screener, OASIS = Overall Anxiety Severity and Impairment Scale, PHQ-9 = Patient Health Questionnaire, ASI = Anxiety Sensitivity Index. aED>MA>HC, bED and MA > HC. Bolded values indicate significance at p < 0.05.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s002
(PDF)
S2 Table. AN symptom level subgroup analysis: t-tests to discern where group differences lie.
HC = Healthy Comparison, MA = Mood/Anxiety. AN = Anorexia Nervosa. d = Cohen’s d effect size. 95% CI = 95% confidence interval for effect size. Symptom ratings for each individual were averaged over Breath Hold Trials 1 and 2. Bolded values indicate significance at p < 0.05.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s003
(PDF)
S3 Table. Spearman correlations between suffocation fear and anxiety sensitivity across groups.
HC = Healthy Comparison. MA = Mood/Anxiety, ED = Eating Disorder. p = p value uncorrected. pcorr = p value with Benjamini-Hochberg False Discovery Rate Correction applied. Bolded values indicate significance at p < 0.05.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s004
(PDF)
S4 Table. Means and standard deviations (SD) for carbon dioxide percentage at baseline and pre/post carbon dioxide percent change for each breath hold trial.
BH1 = Breath Hold Trial 1. BH2 = Breath Hold Trial 2. HC = Healthy Comparison. MA = Mood/Anxiety. ED = Eating Disorder.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s005
(PDF)
S2 File. Variable names.
contains an explanation of each variable name in the dataset S1 File.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s007
(CSV)
S3 File. Missing data.
A table detailing missing data for each variable by participant group.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235346.s008
(CSV)
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Valerie Upshaw and members of the LIBR assessment team for their help with diagnostic assessment and data collection, to W. Kyle Simmons for helpful discussions during task development, and to Danielle Deville and Kristen Gray for useful comments during manuscript writing. Results presented at the International Conference on Eating Disorders, Chicago, IL, April 2018. The Tulsa 1000 Authors include the following contributors: Robin L. Aupperle, Jerzy Bodurka, Yoon-Hee Cha, Justin S. Feinstein, Sahib S. Khalsa, Rayus Kuplicki, Martin P. Paulus, Jonathan B. Savitz, Jennifer L. Stewart, and Teresa A. Victor. Teresa A. Victor is the lead author for this group and can be reached at TVictor@laureateinstitute.org.
References
- 1. Nagl M, Jacobi C, Paul M, Beesdo-Baum K, Höfler M, Lieb R, et al. Prevalence, incidence, and natural course of anorexia and bulimia nervosa among adolescents and young adults. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2016;25(8):903–18.
- 2. Harrison A, Sullivan S, Tchanturia K, Treasure J. Emotional functioning in eating disorders: attentional bias, emotion recognition and emotion regulation. Psychological Medicine. 2010;40(11):1887–97. pmid:20102669
- 3. Mitchell JE, Crow S. Medical complications of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2006;19(4):438–43. pmid:16721178
- 4. Jenkins PE, Hoste RR, Meyer C, Blissett JM. Eating disorders and quality of life: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31(1):113–21. pmid:20817335
- 5. Kaye WH, Fudge JL, Paulus M. New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009 Aug;10(8):573. pmid:19603056
- 6. Klabunde M, Collado D, Bohon C. An interoceptive model of bulimia nervosa: A neurobiological systematic review. Journal of psychiatric research. 2017;94:36–46. pmid:28651098
- 7. Koch KL, Bingaman S, Tan L, Stern RM. Visceral perceptions and gastric myoelectrical activity in healthy women and in patients with bulimia nervosa. Neurogastroenterology and Motility. 1998;10(1):3–10. pmid:9507247
- 8. Kerr KL, Moseman SE, Avery JA, Bodurka J, Zucker NL, Simmons WK. Altered insula activity during visceral interoception in weight-restored patients with anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(2):521. pmid:26084229
- 9. Woznica A, Vickers K, Koerner N, Fracalanza K. Reactivity to 35% carbon dioxide in bulimia nervosa and panic disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2015;228(3):571–5. pmid:26141602
- 10. Khalsa SS, Craske MG, Li W, Vangala S, Strober M, Feusner JD. Altered interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa: effects of meal anticipation, consumption and bodily arousal. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2015;48(7):889–97. pmid:25712775
- 11. Khalsa SS, Hassanpour MS, Strober M, Craske MG, Arevian AC, Feusner JD. Interoceptive anxiety and body representation in anorexia nervosa. Frontiers in psychiatry. 2018;9:444. pmid:30298026
- 12. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS, et al. Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2018 Jun 1;3(6):501–13.
- 13. Bär K-J, Boettger S, Wagner G, Wilsdorf C, Gerhard UJ, Boettger MK, et al. Changes of pain perception, autonomic function, and endocrine parameters during treatment of anorectic adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45(9):1068–76. pmid:16926614
- 14. Lautenbacher S, Pauls AM, Strian F, Pirke K-M, Krieg J-C. Pain sensitivity in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry. 1991;29(11):1073–8. pmid:1873371
- 15. Faris PL, Raymond NC, De Zwaan M, Howard LA, Eckert ED, Mitchell JE. Nociceptive, but not tactile, thresholds are elevated in bulimia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry. 1992;32(5):462–6. pmid:1486152
- 16. de Zwaan M, Biener D, Bach M, Wiesnagrotzki S, Stacher G. Pain sensitivity, alexithymia, and depression in patients with eating disorders: are they related? Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1996;41(1):65–70. pmid:8887820
- 17. Stein D, Kaye WH, Matsunaga H, Myers D, Orbach I, Har‐Even D, et al. Pain perception in recovered bulimia nervosa patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2003;34(3):331–6. pmid:12949924
- 18. Strigo IA, Matthews SC, Simmons AN, Oberndorfer T, Klabunde M, Reinhardt LE, et al. Altered insula activation during pain anticipation in individuals recovered from anorexia nervosa: evidence of interoceptive dysregulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(1):23–33. pmid:22836447
- 19. Goldzak-Kunik G, Friedman R, Spitz M, Sandler L, Leshem M. Intact sensory function in anorexia nervosa. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2011;95(2):272–82. pmid:22205316
- 20. Schmahl C, Meinzer M, Zeuch A, Fichter M, Cebulla M, Kleindienst N, et al. Pain sensitivity is reduced in borderline personality disorder, but not in posttraumatic stress disorder and bulimia nervosa. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2010;11(2–2):364–71.
- 21. Paulus MP, Stein MB. Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain structure and Function. 2010 Jun 1;214(5–6):451–63. pmid:20490545
- 22. Keski-Rahkonen A, Mustelin L. Epidemiology of eating disorders in Europe: prevalence, incidence, comorbidity, course, consequences, and risk factors. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2016;29(6):340–5. pmid:27662598
- 23. Perna G, Barbini B, Cocchi S, Bertani A, Gasperini M. 35% CO2 challenge in panic and mood disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1995;33(3):189–94. pmid:7790671
- 24. Caldirola D, Perna G, Arancio C, Bertani A, Bellodi L. The 35% CO2 challenge test in patients with social phobia. Psychiatry Research. 1997;71(1):41–8. pmid:9247980
- 25. Kaplan JS, Arnkoff DB, Glass CR, Tinsley R, Geraci M, Hernandez E, Luckenbaugh D, Drevets WC, Carlson PJ. Avoidant coping in panic disorder: a yohimbine biological challenge study. Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 2012;25(4):425–42.
- 26. Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Arthur D, Paulus MP. Association of major depressive disorder with altered functional brain response during anticipation and processing of heat pain. Archives of general psychiatry. 2008;65(11):1275–84. pmid:18981339
- 27. Bär KJ, Brehm S, Boettger MK, Boettger S, Wagner G, Sauer H. Pain perception in major depression depends on pain modality. Pain. 2005;117(1–2):97–103. pmid:16061323
- 28. Lautenbacher S, Spernal J, Schreiber W, Krieg JC. Relationship between clinical pain complaints and pain sensitivity in patients with depression and panic disorder. Psychosomatic medicine. 1999;61(6):822–7. pmid:10593634
- 29. Schwier C, Kliem A, Boettger MK, Bär KJ. Increased cold-pain thresholds in major depression. The journal of pain. 2010;11(3):287–90. pmid:19944649
- 30. Godart NT, Flament MF, Curt F, Perdereau F, Lang F, Venisse JL, et al. Anxiety disorders in subjects seeking treatment for eating disorders: a DSM-IV controlled study. Psychiatry research. 2003 Mar 25;117(3):245–58. pmid:12686367
- 31. Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, et al. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychological assessment. 2007;19(2):176. pmid:17563199
- 32. Asmundson GJ, Norton GR, Wilson KG, Sandler LS. Subjective symptoms and cardiac reactivity to brief hyperventilation in individuals with high anxiety sensitivity. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1994;32(2):237–41. pmid:8155062
- 33. Eke M, McNally RJ. Anxiety sensitivity, suffocation fear, trait anxiety, and breath-holding duration as predictors of response to carbon dioxide challenge. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1996 Aug 1;34(8):603–7. pmid:8870286
- 34. Alius MG, Pané‐Farré CA, Von Leupoldt A, Hamm AO. Induction of dyspnea evokes increased anxiety and maladaptive breathing in individuals with high anxiety sensitivity and suffocation fear. Psychophysiology. 2013;50(5):488–97. pmid:23421426
- 35. Ocañez KL, Kathryn McHugh R, Otto MW. A meta‐analytic review of the association between anxiety sensitivity and pain. Depression and anxiety. 2010;27(8):760–7. pmid:20336798
- 36. Olatunji BO, Wolitzky-Taylor KB. Anxiety sensitivity and the anxiety disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Psychological bulletin. 2009;135(6):974. pmid:19883144
- 37. Naragon-Gainey K. Meta-analysis of the relations of anxiety sensitivity to the depressive and anxiety disorders. Psychological bulletin. 2010;136(1):128. pmid:20063929
- 38. Domschke K, Stevens S, Pfleiderer B, Gerlach AL. Interoceptive sensitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: an overview and integration of neurobiological findings. Clinical psychology review. 2010;30(1):1–1. pmid:19751958
- 39. Anestis MD, Holm-Denoma JM, Gordon KH, Schmidt NB, Joiner TE. The role of anxiety sensitivity in eating pathology. Cognitive therapy and research. 2008;1;32(3):370–85.
- 40. Davey GC, Chapman L. Disgust and eating disorder symptomatology in a non‐clinical population: The role of trait anxiety and anxiety sensitivity. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy: An International Journal of Theory & Practice. 2009;16(4):268–75.
- 41. Fulton JJ, Lavender JM, Tull MT, Klein AS, Muehlenkamp JJ, Gratz KL. The relationship between anxiety sensitivity and disordered eating: The mediating role of experiential avoidance. Eating behaviors. 2012;13(2):166–9. pmid:22365805
- 42. Victor TA, Khalsa SS, Simmons WK, Feinstein JS, Savitz J, Aupperle RL, et al. Tulsa 1000: A naturalistic study protocol for multilevel assessment and outcome prediction in a large psychiatric sample. BMJ open. 2018;8(1):e016620. pmid:29371263
- 43. Klump KL, Strober M, Bulik CM, Thornton L, Johnson C, Devlin B, et al. Personality characteristics of women before and after recovery from an eating disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34(8):1407–18. pmid:15724872
- 44. Sunday SR, Halmi KA. Comparison of the Yale‐Brown‐Cornell eating disorders scale in recovered eating disorder patients, restrained dieters, and nondieting controls. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2000;28(4):455–9. pmid:11054794
- 45. Holtkamp K, Müller B, Heussen N, Remschmidt H, Herpertz-Dahlmann BJEc, psychiatry a. Depression, anxiety, and obsessionality in long-term recovered patients with adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa. 2005;14(2):106–10.
- 46. Frank GK, Wagner A, Achenbach S, McConaha C, Skovira K, Aizenstein H, et al. Altered brain activity in women recovered from bulimictype eating disorders after a glucose challenge: a pilot study. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2006;39(1):76–9. pmid:16254868
- 47. Cowdrey FA, Park RJ, Harmer CJ, McCabe C. Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70(8):736–43. pmid:21714958
- 48. Frank GK, Shott ME, Hagman JO, Mittal VA. Alterations in brain structures related to taste reward circuitry in ill and recovered anorexia nervosa and in bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;170(10):1152–60. pmid:23680873
- 49. Frank GK, Favaro A, Marsh R, Ehrlich S, Lawson EA. Toward valid and reliable brain imaging results in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2018;51(3):250–61. pmid:29405338
- 50. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 20:22–33;quiz 4–57.
- 51. Al-Asadi AM, Klein B, Meyer D. Multiple comorbidities of 21 psychological disorders and relationships with psychosocial variables: a study of the online assessment and diagnostic system within a web-based population. Journal of medical Internet research. 2015;17(3).
- 52. Blinder BJ, Cumella EJ, Sanathara VA. Psychiatric comorbidities of female inpatients with eating disorders. Psychosomatic medicine. 2006;68(3):454–62. pmid:16738079
- 53. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ‐9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2001;16(9):606–13. pmid:11556941
- 54. Norman SB, Hami Cissell S, Means‐Christensen AJ, Stein MB. Development and validation of an overall anxiety severity and impairment scale (OASIS). Depression and Anxiety. 2006;23(4):245–9. pmid:16688739
- 55. Morgan JF, Reid F, Lacey JH. The SCOFF questionnaire: Assessment of a new screening tool for eating disorders. BMJ Open. 1999;319(7223):1467–8.
- 56. Aupperle RL, Paulus MP, Kuplicki R, Touthang J, Victor T, Yeh HW, et al. Tulsa 1000 Investigators Tulsa 1000 Investigators. Web-Based Graphic Representation of the Life Course of Mental Health: Cross-Sectional Study Across the Spectrum of Mood, Anxiety, Eating, and Substance Use Disorders. JMIR Mental Health. 2020;7(1):e16919. pmid:32012081
- 57. Virtanen J, Noponen T, Ilmoniemi RJ. Properties of end-expiratory breath hold responses measured with near-infrared spectroscopy. InOptical Tomography and Spectroscopy of Tissue IX 2011 Feb 17 (Vol. 7896, p. 78960D). International Society for Optics and Photonics.
- 58. Asmundson GJ, Stein MB. Triggering the false suffocation alarm in panic disorder patients by using a voluntary breath-holding procedure. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151(2):264–6. pmid:8296901
- 59. Van Der Does AW. Voluntary breath holding: Not a suitable probe of the suffocation alarm in panic disorder. Behaviour research and therapy. 1997;35(8):779–84. pmid:9256521
- 60. Stice E, Telch CF, Rizvi SL. Development and validation of the Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale: A brief self-report measure of anorexia, bulimia, and binge-eating disorder. Psychological Assessment. 2000;12(2):123–31. pmid:10887758
- 61. Luce KH, Crowther JH. The reliability of the eating disorder examination—Self report questionnaire version (EDE‐Q). International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1999;25(3):349–51. pmid:10192002
- 62. Krabbenborg MA, Danner UN, Larsen JK, van der Veer N, van Elburg AA, de Ridder DT, et al. The Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale: Psychometric features within a clinical population and a cut‐off point to differentiate clinical patients from healthy controls. European Eating Disorders Review. 2012;20(4):315–20. pmid:21805535
- 63. Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B, Owen C, Beumont PJ. Validity of the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) in screening for eating disorders in community samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2004;42(5):551–67. pmid:15033501
- 64. Tuschen-Caffier B, Vögele C. Psychological and physiological reactivity to stress: An experimental study on bulimic patients, restrained eaters and controls. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 1999;68(6):333–40. pmid:10559714
- 65. Vocks S, Legenbauer T, Wächter A, Wucherer M, Kosfelder J. What happens in the course of body exposure?: Emotional, cognitive, and physiological reactions to mirror confrontation in eating disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2007;62(2):231–9. pmid:17270582
- 66. Hilbert A, Vögele C, Tuschen-Caffier B, Hartmann AS. Psychophysiological responses to idiosyncratic stress in bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. Physiology and Behavior. 2011;104(5):770–7. pmid:21839762
- 67. Kaye W, George D, Gwirtsman H, Jimerson D, Goldstein D, Ebert M, et al. Isoproterenol infusion test in anorexia nervosa: Assessment of pre-and post-beta-noradrenergic receptor activity. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1990;26(3):355–9. pmid:2274636
- 68. Berner LA, Simmons AN, Wierenga CE, Bischoff-Grethe A, Paulus MP, Bailer U, et al. Altered interoceptive activation before, during, and after aversive breathing load in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine. 2018;48(1):142–54. pmid:28714434
- 69. Klein DF. False suffocation alarms, spontaneous panics, and related conditions: an integrative hypothesis. Archives of general psychiatry. 1993;50(4):306–17. pmid:8466392
- 70. Vickers K, Jafarpour S, Mofidi A, Rafat B, Woznica A. The 35% carbon dioxide test in stress and panic research: Overview of effects and integration of findings. Clinical psychology review. 2012;32(3):153–64. pmid:22322014
- 71. McMurray KM, Strawn JR, Sah R. Fluoxetine Modulates Spontaneous and Conditioned Behaviors to Carbon Dioxide (CO2) Inhalation and Alters Forebrain–Midbrain Neuronal Activation. Neuroscience. 2019;396:108–18. pmid:30439538
- 72. Perna G, Casolari A, Bussi R, Cucchi M, Arancio C, Bellodi L. Comparison of 35% carbon dioxide reactivity between panic disorder and eating disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2004;125(3):277–83. pmid:15051188
- 73. Sachs KV, Harnke B, Mehler PS, Krantz MJ. Cardiovascular complications of anorexia nervosa: a systematic review. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2016;49(3):238–48. pmid:26710932
- 74. Krieg J-C, Roscher S, Strian F, Pirke K-M, Lautenbacher S. Pain sensitivity in recovered anorexics, restrained and unrestrained eaters. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1993;37(6):595–601. pmid:8410745
- 75. Holtz K, Hamm AO, Pané-Farré CA. Repeated Interoceptive Exposure in Individuals With High and Low Anxiety Sensitivity. Behavior modification. 2019;43(4):467–89. pmid:29690770
- 76. Schmidt NB, Cook JH. Effects of anxiety sensitivity on anxiety and pain during a cold pressor challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1999;37(4):313–23. pmid:10204277
- 77. Schmidt NB, Zvolensky MJ, Maner JK. Anxiety sensitivity: Prospective prediction of panic attacks and Axis I pathology. Journal of psychiatric research. 2006;40(8):691–9. pmid:16956622
- 78. Boswell JF, Anderson LM, Oswald JM, Reilly EE, Gorrell S, Anderson DA. A preliminary naturalistic clinical case series study of the feasibility and impact of interoceptive exposure for eating disorders. Behav Res Ther. 2019.
- 79. Zucker NL, LaVia MC, Craske MG, Foukal M, Harris AA, Datta N, et al. Feeling and body investigators (FBI): ARFID division-An acceptance-based interoceptive exposure treatment for children with ARFID. The International journal of eating disorders. 2019;52(4):466–72. pmid:30597590
- 80. Steinglass JE, Sysko R, Mayer L, Berner LA, Schebendach J, Wang Y, et al. Pre-meal anxiety and food intake in anorexia nervosa. Appetite. 2010;1;55(2):214–8. pmid:20570701
- 81. Kaye WH, Bulik CM, Thornton L, Barbarich N, Masters K, Group PFC. Comorbidity of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(12):2215–21. pmid:15569892
- 82. Mischoulon D, Eddy KT, Keshaviah A, Dinescu D, Ross SL, Kass AE, et al. Depression and eating disorders: treatment and course. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;130(3):470–7. pmid:21109307
- 83. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Venkatraman VK, Fudge J, May JC, Mazurkewicz L, et al. Altered reward processing in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(12):1842–9. pmid:18056239
- 84. Khalsa SS, Portnoff LC, McCurdy-McKinnon D, Feusner JD. What happens after treatment? A systematic review of relapse, remission, and recovery in anorexia nervosa. J Eat Disord. 2017;5:20. pmid:28630708