Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The perceived size of the implicit representation of the dorsum and palm of the hand

Abstract

The perception of the body and its parts has traditionally been studied using the conscious body image. Here, we determine the implicit representation of the hand. Participants were sequentially shown two life-size images of either the dorsal or palmar surface of their hand. In one interval either the horizontal or vertical dimension of the image was varied using an adaptive staircase, while the other interval contained the full-size, undistorted image. Participants reported which image most closely matched their hand. The staircase honed in on the distorted image that was equally likely to be judged as matching their own hand as the accurate image. The implicit representation was taken as midway between these two images. The experiment was repeated with different hand orientations. Perceived width depended on the orientation, with differences found between the upright and right orientations. Interestingly, the perceived length of the dorsum and palm were different from each other—length of the dorsum was overestimated whereas palm length was perceived accurately. This study reveals distortions of the implicit representation of the hands in healthy individuals.

The perception of our hand size is critical for our interactions with the world—especially for finer work. The relative lengths of our fingers and the proportions of our hands are part of defining ourselves. The hand is also our primary organ for exploring the world by touch. Although the palmar side of the hand (specifically the finger tips) is the part used for touching things (with its covering of glabrous skin and notorious concentration of tactile sensors), it is the relatively insensitive dorsum of the hand (covered largely with hairy skin) that is generally visible.

The hand has been the main focus for studying various aspects of perception and action such as touch, haptics, motor skills, reaching, grasping, pointing, tool use, position sense, size perception, etc. [114] and so it is important to get a complete picture of how the hand is represented in the brain. All these sensory processes and tasks require knowledge about how the brain represents the hand and therefore can be impacted by any size or shape distortions that may occur. We recently developed a novel psychophysical method for determining perceived body size that provides an objective, unbiased, implicit measure of the body representation in the brain [15,16]. Here, we examine the perceived size of the dorsum and palm of the hand to determine and compare baseline size accuracy when the hand is viewed in various orientations and to compare the representation of the two sides of the hand.

Previous studies looking at how the hand is represented have used methods that involve making judgements based on the perceived location of landmarks, such as the knuckles and fingertips [1,2,714,1720]. These judgments require participants to point to landmarks on their occluded hand, from which maps of perceived size are produced by comparing differences between judged and actual position. This type of task is effective at determining a body model that is based on position sense and postural information of the hand but is potentially complicated by inaccuracies in proprioception, reaching, etc. Here we obtain a measure of the representation of hand size that does not rely on judgements of landmarks or other perceptual tasks but rather looks at the hand as a whole and requires making only size judgements.

Previous studies that have explored hand representation usually focused on one side—either the dorsum or the palm [1,2023]–but have not considered how the size perception of the two sides of the same body part might correspond. While the perceived sizes of the dorsum and palm have not previously been compared within individuals, previous studies have shown differences between the dorsum and palm for tasks involving localization of cutaneous stimuli [23], mental rotation of the hands after experiencing the rubber hand illusion [24], tactile size perception [10,25], tactile distance [26], position sense [11], pointing to locations of hand and finger landmarks—such as knuckles and fingertips [11] and distances between two fingers [27].

Longo and Haggard [11], in what they referred to as a 2.5-D representation of the human hand (inspired by David Marr [28]), found clear overestimation of the perceived size of both sides of the hand but the palmar side was significantly less overestimated than the dorsal surface. This was despite the fact that the palm, with its larger number of receptors has a larger representation in the primary somatosensory cortical map than the dorsum [29]. Higher accuracy for the palm may be due to the more orderly, somatotopic representation of glabrous skin (palm) compared with hairy skin (dorsum) which is represented irregularly in islands of cortex intermixed with the map of glabrous skin [30]. Tactile skin perception and position sense [10] both show that the implicit representation of the palm is less distorted than that of the dorsum; one might expect that the more accurate representation of the palm would be transferred or extruded to the dorsum if the brain used a single, coherent model of the hand as a volumetric object.

Our study has two motivations. Firstly, to determine how accurate healthy populations are at judging the size of the dorsum and palm of their hand. Based on previous research [1,21,22] we predicted that the dorsum would overall be perceived as shorter and wider than it actually was. Secondly, to assess how perception of hand size might be affected when viewing the hand in familiar and unfamiliar perspectives. The hand is more likely to be identified as belonging to the self when viewed in a familiar orientation [31,32]. Our previous rationale for varying the orientation of the face while assessing its perceived size [15] had to do with unique properties that the face lacks compared to much of the rest of the body especially the hands, properties such as interaction, movement, and visual perspective. We used the opposite line of thought in the current study. The hand has all these attributes and uses them to the greatest extent of any body part, thus making it worthwhile to explore how orientation impacts its perceived size. With all the different positions and forms that the hand can take, it seems natural to consider how perception may change with these varying viewpoint. Why might one assume that measuring it in only a “typical” or “canonical” position would be correct? Life-long experiences of directly interacting with and seeing the hands from multiple different viewpoints may result in greater flexibility and plasticity in how the brain represents and perceives the hand. Thus, unlike for the face, we did not expect that varying orientation would affect the hand’s perceived size.

As in our previous papers [15,16], we included sex and body satisfaction as variables to assess how they might impact body size perception. Since the hand is a relatively neutral body part for weight, shape, and size concerns, we did not have any set predictions about differences that might occur but instead used sex and body satisfaction as exploratory factors to add and extend the existing literature.

Methods

Participants

40 participants (20 females, mean age 24.43 years, SD = 9.37) took part in the experiment. Handedness was assessed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [33] to ensure that only those with a strong preference were included (38 right-handers). They were recruited from the York University Undergraduate Research Participant Pool and received course credit for taking part in the study. The experiment was approved by the York Ethics Board and all participants signed informed consent forms. The study was performed in accordance with the Treaty of Helsinki.

Materials/Stimuli

Body dissatisfaction.

The Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ) [34] is a 34-item self-report questionnaire that was developed to assess concerns about body shape and experiences of feeling fat that participants may have experienced within the previous month. Each question uses a scale from 1 (never) to 6 (always) that are added together to obtain a total BSQ score ranging from 34–204, with lower scores indicating lower levels of body dissatisfaction and higher scores indicating higher levels of body dissatisfaction. The test was administered before the experiment began to obtain a measure of body dissatisfaction. The total BSQ score was used for correlations. To create categorical groups, participants were divided into high and low groups according to whether their score was above or below the overall mean.

Photographs.

Color photographs of the dorsal and palmar surfaces of the participant’s dominant hand placed flat against a wall were taken using a digital camera (Canon EOS 10D). Photographs were taken under standardized conditions with the flash on, from midarm to the fingers, from a distance of approximately four feet (122 cm) with no zoom function. The images were then cropped to include only the dorsum or palm and formatted with a white background (Adobe Photoshop CC 2014). These images served as the undistorted “real” images and were used for composing the distorted images. Using a soft tape measure, hand size was measured straight down from the tip of the middle finger to the bottom of the hand, just before the start of the wrist. The size of the image was presented life-size by adjusting the magnification of the image until it matched the participant’s actual hand size. Optical distortions of the lens were confirmed as being less than 0.3% of the width or length of the photograph (given that the other length was set to veridical) using Matlab to assess the amount of distortion.

Distorting the images.

Images were presented and distorted using MATLAB (version 2011b) and Psychophysics Toolbox [35] running on a MacBook Pro. One dimension of the image (either width or length) was distorted (made either bigger or smaller) using a QUEST adaptive staircase psychometric procedure [36]. The image was viewed in the center of a computer monitor (27-inch Apple display) with the dorsum or palm in one of the four orientations shown in Fig 1: (1) upright (0°), (2) to the right (90°), (3) upside down (180°), and (4) to the left (270°). Perceived width and length dimensions were separately measured for the palm and dorsum in each of the four orientations, resulting in a total of 16 conditions.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Sample images for the dorsum and palm in each orientation used.

The four orientations are shown, from left to right, in the order of (1) upright (0°), (2) to the right (90°), (3) upside down (180°), and (4) to the left (270°) for the dorsum (top row) and palm (bottom row).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230624.g001

Procedure

Participants sat in a chair approximately 45 cm in front of the computer screen. To measure perceived size, a two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) design was used. Each trial consisted of two 1.5s intervals—one interval containing the undistorted image and one interval containing the distorted image—separated by a blank white screen for 1.5s. Participants identified which interval contained the image that most closely matched the perception of their own hand and responded using a two-button computer mouse (left button for first interval, right button for second interval). Two interleaved QUEST staircases (25 trials per staircase) were used for each condition (50 trials total), with one starting with the manipulated dimension 40% larger than the undistorted image and the other starting with that dimension 40% smaller than the undistorted image. The standard deviation of the guess was set to 60% as the authors [36] suggest a liberal standard deviation to improve accuracy. Each of the 16 conditions were run in a single block and took approximately six minutes to complete. Block order was determined by a Latin square and was counterbalanced across participants.

Data analysis

The QUEST program returned an estimate of the distortion at which the participant reported that the distorted image was equally like their perceived body size as the undistorted image. The QUEST algorithm assumes the observer's psychometric function follows a Weibull distribution and adaptively determines the amount of distortion to be presented based on the participant's response to the previous trials. As the experiment goes on, knowledge on the observer's psychometric accumulates. To visualize and confirm the QUEST’s performance, the participant’s decision was plotted against the distortion used for each trial and fitted with a logistic (Eq 1) using the curve fitting toolbox in MATLAB. (1) where x0 is the actual size, x is the distorted value that was equally likely to be judged as matching the observer’s size, and b is an estimate of the slope of the function. The size of the internal body representation was taken as the point halfway between x and x0.

All of the participants’ data for each condition were examined to validate the accuracy and efficiency of the QUEST procedure. The QUEST’s performance was also checked to determine whether reliable estimates were obtained within 50 trials. All data were inspected for outliers. Outliers were defined as points that fell outside ± 3 standard deviations from the mean. Data that failed to meet these criteria were discarded from the analyses (0 participants).

Estimates were converted to percentages relative to the accurate reference photograph. For the two left-hand participants, the data for the right and left orientations were switched to correspond to the correct position. Data analyses were conducted on these values using SPSS 23. Correlations and mixed measures analyses of variances (ANOVAs) were used for statistical analyses, with alpha set at p < .05 and post-hoc multiple comparisons were made using Bonferroni corrections.

Results

Data in only one out of 16 of the conditions were significantly correlated with BSQ score so BSQ groups were not included in the analyses.

Dorsum and palm size accuracy: Width dimension

Fig 2 shows the results for perceived width of the dorsum and palm. A three-way mixed ANOVA was conducted to test for the within-subjects effects of body part (dorsum or palm), orientation (upright, right, upside down, and left), and the between-subjects effect of sex (male and female). The only significant effect found was for orientation, F(3, 114) = 3.44, p = .018, ηp2 = .083, with pairwise comparisons showing that upright (M = .97, SE = .95) and right (M = -1.82, SE = .67) orientations differed by 2.79% ± .98, p = .044, 95% CI = [.05, 5.53].

thumbnail
Fig 2. Perceived width of the dorsum and palm.

Mean differences from accurate when dorsum of the hand (left panel) or palm (right panel) was distorted in the width dimension for each viewing orientation. Positive and negative scores represent overestimation and underestimation respectively. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230624.g002

Dorsum and palm size accuracy: Length dimension

A second ANOVA was conducted using the same variables as above but for the length dimension (Fig 3). A significant main effect was found for body part, F(1, 38) = 8.68, p = .005, ηp2 = .186, with greater amounts of distortion observed for the dorsum of the hand (M = 2.26, SE = .94) compared to the palm (M = .30, SE = .74). This means that participants were less accurate at judging the length of their dorsum but were not significantly different from perfect accuracy for the palm.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Perceived dorsum and palm length.

Mean differences from accurate when dorsum (left panel) or palm (right panel) was distorted in the length dimension for each viewing orientation. Positive and negative scores represent overestimation and underestimation respectively. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230624.g003

Relationship between dorsum and palm

Correlations between errors in the perceived size of the dorsum and palm (Table 1) were conducted for width and length (averaged across the four orientations in Fig 4). The width of the dorsum and palm were significantly correlated, r(39) = .550, 95% CI = [.29, .74], R2 = .302, p < .001, as were the length of the dorsum and palm, r(39) = .708, 95% CI = [.51, .84], R2 = .502, p < .001. These results show that there was a strong relationship between the perceived size of the dorsum and palm–if someone overestimates their dorsum size, they will likely overestimate their palm size as well.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Dorsum vs palm.

Relationship between dorsum and palm distortions for width (pink triangles) and length (blue circles). The lines plotted through the data are the goodness-of-fit lines for the width and length.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230624.g004

thumbnail
Table 1. Correlations between the perceived size of the dorsum and palm (N = 40).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230624.t001

Actual vs perceived size

To see how the distortion percentages would look when calculated relative to actual size, we calculated the perceived lengths of the dorsum and palm (averaged across the four orientations) to make our results more clearly understandable and relatable to previous studies. Fig 5 shows the perceived length of (Fig 5A) the dorsum (M = 7.31 inches, SE = .12) and (Fig 5B) palm (M = 7.27 inches, SE = .11) plotted as a function of actual length (dorsum: M = 7.14 inches, SE = .07; palm: M = 7.24 inches, SE = .08). The perceived length was significantly difference from the actual length for the dorsum, t(39) = 2.37, p = .023, ηp2 = .125, with people perceiving their dorsum to be longer than it actually was by .17 inches ± .07, 95% CI = [.02, .32]. There was no significant difference between the perceived and actual length of the palm, t(39) = .57, p = .573, ηp2 = .008.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Actual vs perceived length.

Relationship between actual and perceived lengths for (A) dorsum length and (B) palm length. The line plotted through the data represents accurate perception.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230624.g005

Discussion

The method we used here reveals distortions of the implicit body representation independent of the conscious body image. The length of the representation of the dorsum and palm of the hand were significantly different from each other but only the perceived width of the dorsum depended on orientation with overestimation found when the dorsum was viewed upright and an underestimation when viewed in the right orientation. The representations of the dorsum was longer than its actual size. The dorsum and palm did not show any differences for width judgements. Accuracy differed between the dorsum and the palm in the length dimension, meaning that the perceived length of the dorsum and palm were not the same although there was a strong correlation between them.

Perception of the size of the dorsum and palm reveals distortions in how the body is represented in the brain. The enormous distortions in the homunculus of the primary somatosensory cortex are largely corrected [37] but for every orientation, dorsum length was overestimated whereas the length of the palm was perceived accurately. This is curious as it is the dorsum that is more usually seen (look down at your hands as they type on your computer right now). However, this makes sense in terms of function, as it is the location of the fine-tuned receptors on the palm side of your fingertips [38] that it is important that you place in the correct positions.

Comparisons to previous findings

Overestimations of hand width, underestimation of finger length, and a radial-ulnar gradient of magnification, with finger length underestimation increasing from thumb to little finger were found by Longo & Haggard [1]. Longo & Haggard [10] confirmed these findings and also showed that the width of both the dorsal and palmar surfaces were overestimated whereas only finger length was underestimated for the dorsal surface. These findings differ from our current findings but there are many explanations for why this might be– 1) our methods are extremely different including the design, task, and measures obtained; 2) they measured hand width using the distance between the bases of the index and little fingers; 3) they only looked at finger length rather than the length of the entire hand and palm. In fact, no distortions were found when they tried using a template matching task to see how it would compare to their original findings [1,12,39].

Sex and body satisfaction scores

Since sex [4043] and body satisfaction [4447] are known to be factors that influence body perception, we looked at both variables in our current study but surprisingly did not find effects of either.

Our findings that there are no differences between how males and females perceive their hand size is not compatible with what has been found in previous studies that looked at sex differences in hand representation [6,20]. Longo [6] recently conducted a meta-analysis of nineteen experiments that used his ‘psychomorphometric paradigm’ method of mapping the body [1] to examine whether sex differences exist for perceptual hand maps. He concluded that distortions occurred for both males and females but that sex differences exist in the magnitude of distortions in perceptual hand maps, with women showing greater overestimation of hand width and men showing greater underestimation of finger length. This differs from what Coelho & Gonzalez [20] found. They found that finger length was underestimated in both sexes but hand width was only overestimated in women. Longo [6] speculates that these differences could be explained by how actual hand size (used as a reference) was measured—using a photograph of the hand next to a ruler in his studies compared to Coelho and Gonzalez’s method of pointing to landmarks on a non-occluded hand. The latter may not provide an accurate reference due to potential motor control or visual biases with which such localization tasks may be associated. Again, the fundamental differences between our experimental methods and how the hand was measured could be the cause (e.g., hand width and finger length) since our method asks people to access their internal representation.

Conclusions

Knowledge about body size is important for many aspects of perception and action that rely on this information for executing and interpreting tasks, especially for the hands that are required for so many fundamental behaviours (reaching, grasping, touching, holding, etc.,). Distortions in how the brain represents the hand may impact these processes and result in errors that have far-reaching consequences.

References

  1. 1. Longo MR, Haggard P. An implicit body representation underlying human position sense. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107: 11727–11732. pmid:20547858
  2. 2. Coelho LA, Zaninelli G, Gonzalez CLR. A kinematic examination of hand perception. Psychol Res. 2017;81: 1224–1231. pmid:27738751
  3. 3. Cardinali L, Frassinetti F, Brozzoli C, Urquizar C, Roy AC, Farnè A. Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Curr Biol. 2009;19: R478–9. pmid:19549491
  4. 4. Coelho LA, Schacher JP, Scammel C, Doan JB, Gonzalez CLR. Long- but not short-term tool-use changes hand representation. Exp Brain Res. 2019;237: 137–146. pmid:30353213
  5. 5. Peviani V, Bottini G. The distorted hand metric representation serves both perception and action. J Cogn Psychol. 2018;30: 880–893.
  6. 6. Longo MR. Sex differences in perceptual hand maps: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2019;196: 1–10. pmid:30933684
  7. 7. Coelho LA, Gonzalez CL. The visual and haptic contributions to hand perception. Psychol Res. 2018;82: 866875. pmid:28502021
  8. 8. Longo MR. The effects of immediate vision on implicit hand maps. Exp Brain Res. 2014;232: 1241–1247. pmid:24449015
  9. 9. Longo MR. Posture modulates implicit hand maps. Conscious Cogn. 2015;36: 96–102. pmid:26117153
  10. 10. Longo MR, Haggard P. Weber’s illusion and body shape: Anisotropy of tactile size perception on the hand. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2011;37: 720–726. pmid:21480744
  11. 11. Longo MR, Haggard P. A 2.5-D representation of the human hand. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2012;38: 9–13. pmid:21895388
  12. 12. Longo MR, Haggard P. Implicit body representations and the conscious body image. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2012;141: 164–168. pmid:22964057
  13. 13. Longo MR, Mattioni S, Ganea N. Perceptual and conceptual distortions of implicit hand maps. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9: 1–12.
  14. 14. Longo MR, Mancini F, Haggard P. Implicit body representations and tactile spatial remapping. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2015;160: 77–87. pmid:26196650
  15. 15. D’Amour S, Harris LR. Perceived face size in healthy adults. PLoS One. 2017; pmid:28481928
  16. 16. D’Amour S, Harris LR. The Representation of Body Size: Variations With Viewpoint and Sex. Front Psychol. 2019;10: 1–9.
  17. 17. Ambroziak KB, Tamè L, Longo MR. Conceptual distortions of hand structure are robust to changes in stimulus information. Conscious Cogn. 2018;61: 107–116. pmid:29373301
  18. 18. Saulton A, Dodds TJ, Bülthoff HH, de la Rosa S. Objects exhibit body model like shape distortions. Exp Brain Res. 2015;233: 1471–1479. pmid:25678309
  19. 19. Saulton A, Longo MR, Wong HY, Bülthoff HH, de la Rosa S. The role of visual similarity and memory in body model distortions. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2016;164: 103–111. pmid:26783695
  20. 20. Coelho LA, Gonzalez CLR. Chubby hands or little fingers: sex differences in hand representation. Psychol Res. 2018; 1–8. pmid:29616336
  21. 21. Longo MR. Intuitive anatomy: Distortions of conceptual knowledge of hand structure. Cognition. 2015;142: 230–235. pmid:26056746
  22. 22. Longo MR, Long C, Haggard P. Mapping the invisible hand: a body model of a phantom limb. Psychol Sci. 2012;23: 740–2. pmid:22653797
  23. 23. Mancini F, Longo MR, Iannetti GD, Haggard P. A supramodal representation of the body surface. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49: 1194–1201. pmid:21199662
  24. 24. Ionta S, Sforza A, Funato M, Blanke O. Anatomically plausible illusory posture affects mental rotation of body parts. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2013;13: 197–209. pmid:22993114
  25. 25. Longo MR, Ghosh A, Yahya T. Bilateral Symmetry of Distortions of Tactile Size Perception. Perception. 2015;44: 1251–1262. pmid:26562894
  26. 26. Knight FLC, Longo MR, Bremner AJ. Categorical perception of tactile distance. Cognition. 2014;131: 254–262. pmid:24561189
  27. 27. Cocchini G, Galligan T, Mora L, Kuhn G. The magic hand: Plasticity of mental hand representation. Q J Exp Psychol. 2018;71: 2314–2324. pmid:30362401
  28. 28. Marr D. Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Henry Holt and Co., Inc.nc. 1982.
  29. 29. Johansson RS, Vallbo ÅB. Tactile sensibility in the human hand: relative and absolute densities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in glabrous skin. J Physiol. 1979;286: 283–300. Available: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1281571/pdf/jphysiol00755-0288.pdf%0Ahttp://jp.physoc.org/content/286/1/283.short pmid:439026
  30. 30. Powell TP, Mountcastle VB. Some aspects of the functional organization of the cortex of the postcentral gyrus of the monkey: A correlation of findings obtained in a single unit analysis with cytoarchitecture. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp. 1959;105: 133–162. pmid:14434571
  31. 31. Hoover AEN, Harris LR. The role of the viewpoint on body ownership. Exp Brain Res. 2015;233: 1053–1060. pmid:25537470
  32. 32. Hoover AEN, Harris LR. Inducing ownership over an “other” perspective with a visuo-tactile manipulation. Exp Brain Res. 2016;234: 3633–3639. pmid:27554087
  33. 33. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9: 97–113. pmid:5146491
  34. 34. Cooper PJ, Taylor MJ, Cooper Z, Fairbum CG. The development and validation of the body shape questionnaire. Int J Eat Disord. 1987;6: 485–494.
  35. 35. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10: 433–436. pmid:9176952
  36. 36. Watson A, Pelli D. QUEST- A Bayesian adaptive psychophysical method. Percept Psychophys. 1983;33: 113–120. pmid:6844102
  37. 37. Penfield BYW, Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain. 1937;60: 389–443.
  38. 38. Weber EH. De Subtilitate Tactus. In: Ross HE, Murray DJ, editors. EH Weber on the Tactile Senses. 2nd ed. London: Academic Press; 1996. pp. 21–128.
  39. 39. Longo MR. Three-dimensional coherence of the conscious body image. Q J Exp Psychol. 2015;68: 1116–1123. pmid:25311046
  40. 40. Molinari E. Body-size estimation in Anorexia Nervosa. Percept Mot Skills. 1995;81: 23–31. pmid:8532462
  41. 41. Slade PD, Russell GFM. Awareness of body dimensions in anorexia nervosa: cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Psychol Med. 1973;3: 188–199. pmid:4715850
  42. 42. Gleghorn A a., Penner L a., Powers PS, Schulman R. The psychometric properties of several measures of body image. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 1987;9: 203–218.
  43. 43. Thompson JK, Spana RE. The adjustable light beam method for the assessment of size estimation accuracy: Description, psychometric, and normative data. Int J Eat Disord. 1988;7: 521–526.
  44. 44. Gardner RM, Bokenkamp ED. The role of sensory and nonsensory factors in body size estimations of eating disorder subjects. J Clin Psychol. 1996;52: 3–15. pmid:8682909
  45. 45. Eshkevari E, Rieger E, Longo MR, Haggard P, Treasure J. Increased plasticity of the bodily self in eating disorders. Psychol Med. 2012;42: 819–28. pmid:22017964
  46. 46. Mussap AJ, McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA. Implications of accuracy, sensitivity, and variability of body size estimations to disordered eating. Body Image. 2008;5: 80–90. pmid:18405866
  47. 47. McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA, Sitaram G, Mikhail K. Accuracy of body size estimation: Role of biopsychosocial variables. Body Image. 2006;3: 163–171. pmid:18089219