Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sex, personality and conspecific density influence natal dispersal with lifetime fitness consequences in urban and rural burrowing owls

  • Álvaro Luna ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    aluna@ebd.csic.es

    Affiliations Department of Conservation Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñana—CSIC, Sevilla, Spain, Animal Demography and Ecology Unit, IMEDEA (CSIC-UIB), Esporles, Spain

  • Antonio Palma,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Conservation Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñana—CSIC, Sevilla, Spain

  • Ana Sanz-Aguilar,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Animal Demography and Ecology Unit, IMEDEA (CSIC-UIB), Esporles, Spain, Applied Zoology and Conservation Group, University of Balearic Islands, Palma, Spain

  • José L. Tella,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Conservation Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñana—CSIC, Sevilla, Spain

  • Martina Carrete

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physical, Chemical and Natural Systems, Universidad Pablo de Olavide, Sevilla, Spain

Correction

13 Nov 2023: Luna Á, Palma A, Sanz-Aguilar A, Tella JL, Carrete M (2023) Correction: Sex, personality and conspecific density influence natal dispersal with lifetime fitness consequences in urban and rural burrowing owls. PLOS ONE 18(11): e0294485. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0294485 View correction

Abstract

There is a growing need to understand how species respond to habitat changes and the potential key role played by natal dispersal in population dynamics, structure and gene flow. However, few studies have explored differences in this process between conspecifics living in natural habitats and those inhabiting landscapes highly transformed by humans, such as cities. Here, we investigate how individual traits and social characteristics can influence the natal dispersal decisions of burrowing owls (Athene cunicularia) living in urban and rural areas, as well as the consequences in terms of reproductive success and apparent survival. We found short dispersal movements among individuals, with differences between urban and rural birds (i.e., the former covering shorter distances than the latter), maybe because of the higher conspecific density of urban compared to rural areas. Moreover, we found that urban and rural females as well as bold individuals (i.e., individuals with shorter flight initiation distance) exhibited longer dispersal distances than their counterparts. These dispersal decisions have effects on individual fitness. Individuals traveling longer distances increased their reproductive prospects (productivity during the first breeding attempt, and long term productivity). However, the apparent survival of females decreased when they dispersed farther from their natal territory. Although further research is needed to properly understand the ecological and evolutionary consequences of dispersal patterns in transformed habitats, our results provide information about the drivers and the consequences of the restricted natal movements of this species, which may explain its population structuring through restricted gene flow between and within urban and rural areas.

Introduction

Natal dispersal, defined as the movement of individuals from their birthplace to their first breeding area [1] may influence the future survival, fecundity, and lifetime fitness of individuals [2,3,4], playing an important role in the evolution, persistence and spread of populations and species[5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. Thus, a large number of studies have investigated the factors driving natal dispersal decisions, in particular the importance of social and environmental cues (e.g. conspecific density and habitat characteristics; [12,13], previous experience [14]) and phenotypic attributes, including personality [15,16], structural size [17,18], body mass [13,19] and sex [20], among others (reviewed by [21]). All of these factors can interact in complex and subtle ways, varying across the spatial range of a species, such that natal dispersal decisions, as well as their conditioning, can differ among species but also among populations of the same species [21]. Therefore, studies comparing the dispersal patterns of conspecifics inhabiting areas with contrasting characteristics and subjected to different selection regimes are important to better understand the dynamic nature of dispersal as well as how drivers of global change affect the fate of animal populations.

Urbanization is the most drastic and persistent alteration of a landscape, creating new habitats starkly different from the natural habitats it replaces [22]. Currently, the urban expansion is occurring at an unprecedented rate, mainly by the migration from rural to urban areas. One century ago, only 10% of humans inhabited cities, and today about the 50% did so, with a 70% predicted to live in urbanized landscapes by 2050 [22, 23]. Furthermore, the continuous human population growth (from today’s 7 billion people to the estimated 9 billion by 2050 [24]) also contribute to the large increase in the spatial extent of urbanized areas worldwide. Although urbanization leads to an overall loss of biodiversity (the so-called ‘biotic homogenization process; [25, 26, 27]), some species seem to prosper in these environments [28]. Among birds, for example, nearly 20% of the roughly 10,000 described species can be found in cities [29]. Thus, understanding the factors that allow them to persist within these landscapes as well as the consequences for their population dynamics and structure is as important as identifying the drivers of species loss. A common finding of studies exploring traits that allow individuals to live in urban environments is that urban birds are less fearful of humans (bolder) than their rural counterparts, such that urban colonization may be driven by tame individuals from species with high interindividual variability in fear of humans crossing the disturbance frontier [30,31,32,33,34,35]. Fear of humans, measured as the distance at which an individual flees in response to an approaching person (also called flight initiation distance, hereafter FID), is repeatable throughout the adult lifespan [36, 37], heritable [38], and correlates with other behaviors such as exploration and antipredatory response [34]. Thus, it can be considered a personality trait [39]. Another common pattern found in the literature is that urbanization leads to a reduction in predators [40, 41], such that species able to colonize urbanized habitats can show larger densities or abundances than their rural counterparts [42,43,44,45]. These changes in individual behaviors or personalities, conspecific density and predation pressure can have profound effects on the breeding performance and survival prospects of individuals [33], including their dispersal decisions [39]. There is a growing literature showing how the dispersal patterns of some species change in response to local conditions and depending on the phenotypic characteristics of the individuals present in a particular population [6, 21]. Despite this, there are no studies exploring whether urban individuals show different natal dispersal movements than their rural counterparts.

Here, we use the burrowing owl (Athene cunicularia) as a study model to assess the role played by individual characteristics (i.e., sex, and personality), and the environment where an individual was born (i.e., the breeding density and productivity of conspecifics) on the natal dispersal distances of urban and rural individuals. We predict that if natal dispersal is related to individual personality, bold and shy individuals (i.e., those with short and large FID, respectively) will breed at different distances from the sites where they were born. Some studies show that boldness is positively associated with dispersal tendency [15, 46, 47, 48] and thus, urban birds (which are bolder than rural individuals; [32, 33, 34,35]) should have longer natal dispersal distances than rural ones. However, our previous work has indicated that the breeding dispersal propensity of burrowing owls is personality-dependent among rural but not urban individuals, with shy birds moving further than bold ones but only in the rural habitat [39]. Moreover, as avian females usually move greater distances during dispersal than males [49, 50], we expect to find this general pattern among all urban and rural individuals. Social features such as conspecific density and productivity can be used by individuals as indicators of habitat quality, such that dispersers would prefer to move to high-density and high-productivity areas [51, 52, 53]. However, young individuals born in very dense areas could also disperse to low-density areas to avoid intraspecific competition [54, 55]. In our study model, predation, the main determinant of individual fitness [33, 41], is highly unpredictable; thus, conspecific presence and productivity can be used as indicative clues of predation risk. We expect that individuals born in areas with low conspecific density and/or productivity move to other areas of higher quality (i.e., high conspecific density and/or productivity). As urban areas have fewer predators than rural ones [41], this pattern is expected to be more pronounced among birds living in more natural areas. Finally, we evaluated posterior survival and reproductive output. We predict that due to the risk associated with moving to areas far from their natal sites, where individuals are not familiar with the habitat and predation is difficult to assess, birds moving greater distances should have lower reproductive output and survival than those staying close to their natal areas [4].

Material and methods

Study system and field procedures

The study area covers approximately 5,400 km2 of natural grasslands, pastures and cereal crops, surrounding the city of Bahía Blanca, Argentina [30, 36]. We surveyed burrowing owls from 2006 to 2018, monitoring ca. 2500 and ca. 3200 urban and rural nests, respectively. Urban nests were located in private and public gardens, vacant lots among houses, curbs of the streets, roundabouts, and large avenues, in contact with the intense daily activity derived from cities. Rural nests, on the contrary, were located in large extensions of natural or semi-natural grasslands, with very low human presence. It is worth noting that the city is immediately surrounded by large areas of pastures, and there is no obstacle precluding the movement of individuals between urban and rural areas. Moreover, as these owls are able to excavate their own burrows, their distribution is not constrained by the availability of nesting structures.

During the breeding period (from November to early February), all known breeding sites and areas of suitable habitat for the species were regularly visited, and active nests were georeferenced using a portable GPS. Chicks were captured at their natal nests using bow nets and ribbon carpets, and marked with plastic colored and numbered rings readable at a distance. Resightings of marked birds were done annually during the breeding period, throughout the study area [33, 36]-. At the end of every breeding season (except in 2018), we recorded the productivity of each nest as the number of young that reached fledging age.

Natal dispersal distance was measured as the straight line between an individual’s nest site and its first breeding nest. Individuals that were not observed in their first but in their second breeding season were included in our analyses, using as natal dispersal distance the straight line between their natal site and their second breeding nest. In these cases, we assumed that natal dispersal distances were larger than the short breeding dispersal distances recorded for the species [39], such that the breeding location at the second nest would not be far from the first breeding point. However, we cannot discard the possibility that those birds were actually breeding for their first time at 2 years of age, and that this dispersal distance corresponds to their natal dispersal.

We sexed adult birds based on plumage pattern and coloration [36] and, when needed, by molecular procedures [44]. FID was measured using the standard procedure of walking toward undisturbed individuals perched close to their nests during the day and recording the distance at which the bird flew away [38]. We used one FID per individual or the mean when more than one value was available, given the high repeatability of this behavior (r = 0.91; [35]). Conspecific density was calculated using an annual aggregation index for each breeding pair, obtained as their relative position within the spatial distribution of all breeding pairs [56]. This index reflects conspecific density and was obtained using the GPS location of all breeding pairs as Si = Σ exp (−dij) (with ij), where dij was the linear distance between pairs i and j. We also estimated the productivity of the breeding pairs in the surroundings of each breeding site using a modification of this aggregation index, where the distance of each breeding pair was weighted by its productivity. Conspecific productivity was then obtained as the residual of this last variable against the aggregation index calculated previously.

Ethics statements

Fieldwork and procedures were conducted under permits from the Argentinean wildlife agency (22500-4102/09), and the owners of private properties, in accordance with the approved guidelines of the Ethics Committee of CSIC (CEBA-EBD-11-28).

Statistical approach

We used Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMM) to assess the effects of individual traits (sex and personality, measured as FID), and social variables (conspecific density and productivity in the natal area) on the natal dispersal distances (log-transformed, identity link function, normal error distribution) of urban and rural burrowing owls. Due to the low proportion of birds that bred for the first time in their natal nests (philopatric individuals), dispersal distance was modeled as a continuous variable ranging from 0 to the maximum distance observed. Models were built using a different combination of variables in interaction with habitat (urban or rural) but including alternatively only one descriptor of the social environment (conspecific density or productivity) due to their slight, but significant, correlation (Spearman correlation: rho = 0.41, p<0.0001). All models included the year as a random variable. Although individuals born in the same clutches share the same social environment and habitat and can even show similar FID [38], clutch identity was not considered as a random term in models because only 20% of individuals belong to shared clutches (45 individuals of 22 clutches). Thus, the dimension of the variance-covariance matrix was exactly zero and model comparissons using likelihood ratio (LRT) tests did not support the inclusion of this random term in models.

We then compared the social environment of individuals (conspecific density and productivity; identity link functions, normal error distributions) between natal and dispersal sites. Due to differences in conspecific density and productivity between rural and urban areas, we included the habitat of recruitment as a factor in the models. We tested whether these differences were related to the individuals' dispersal distances, again considering potential habitat differences. All models included the year as a random variable to control for interannual differences.

Next, we assessed the effect of dispersal distance on immediate (the first breeding event) and long term productivity of individuals (log link functions, negative binomial error distributions). For long term productivity analyses we only considered individuals with known reproductive output for every year during their reproductive careers and not seen during at least 2 years prior to the end of this study (until 2016), which had a very high probability of being dead (probability of not resighting an alive individual over 2 years at least once was 0.005 for males and 0.033 for females). Models included the dispersal distance of individuals, the habitat where the bird bred, and its sex. Models obtained for long term productivity also included, for each year (random term), the age of each bird (covariate) and its identity (random term) to control for potential improvements along years and pseudoreplication [4]. We also considered potential habitat and sexual differences by including the interaction between sex and dispersal distance and between habitat and dispersal distance in models.

Model selection was performed using the Akaike Information Criterion corrected for small sample sizes (AICc; [57]). Models within 2 AICc units of the best one were considered as alternatives and used to perform model averaging (MuMIn package, [58]). All continuous variables were centered and standardized before modeling to properly estimate their main effects regardless of whether we included the interaction [59]. We considered that a given effect received no, weak or strong statistical support when the 95% confidence interval (CI) strongly overlapped with zero, barely overlapped with zero, or did not overlap with zero, respectively. Finally, we calculated the coefficient of determination, R2, as a measure of the variance explained by a model [60]. Statistical analyses were conducted in R 3.1.2 ([61] using the package “lme4” [62]).

We modeled apparent survival following basic capture–mark–recapture methods for open populations, in which return rates were corrected for detection (p) probabilities [63]. For this purpose, we created encounter histories for every marked individual with known natal dispersal distances. We evaluated if adult apparent survival was related to natal dispersal distance (log transformed) by including it as an individual covariate while habitat (rural and urban) and sex were considered as factors. We first tested the effects of time, habitat and sex on detection probability and, once the best structure for this parameter was selected, we modeled survival. Models differing by < 2 AICc points were considered equivalent [57]. We tested the goodness-of-fit of our global model using the program U-CARE [64]. Models were implemented in the program E-SURGE 2.1.4 [65].

Results

During the study period, we marked 830 urban and 566 rural burrowing owls with PVC rings, and were able to record 321 natal dispersal events in 75 rural (48 males and 27 females) and 246 urban (129 males and 117 females) birds. However, as FID was not measured for all individuals, our dataset was reduced to 224 individuals, 85% of which were resighted during their first breeding. Although some individuals moved more than 10km, median dispersal distance was much shorter (322m), and most birds bred for the first time close to their natal areas (percentage of individuals remaining within 1 km of their natal sites: 75%; Fig 1A) and in the same habitat of birth (96% of dispersions were within the same habitat type). Movements between habitats were rare (10 individuals out of 224), and mainly from urban to rural areas (6 movements from urban to rural areas vs 3 movements from rural to urban ones).

thumbnail
Fig 1.

A) Natal dispersal distances of urban and rural burrowing owls Athene cunicularia (males: gray bars; females: black bars). The inserted figure shows a detailed distribution of dispersal distances lower than 1km. B) Relationship between natal dispersal distances (log-transformed) and individual personality (measured as FID, flight initiation distance). Lines show the tendency observed for males (gray line) and females (black line). Dashed lines represent the 95% confidence interval. Dots are raw data (males: gray dots, females: black dots).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.g001

Correlates of natal dispersal

Sex, individual personality and habitat were the most important variables to explain interindividual differences in natal dispersal distances (Tables 1 and 2). Urban birds dispersed over shorter distances than rural ones, with females moving farther than males in both habitat types. Moreover, individuals with shorter FID dispersed over greater distances than their counterparts, regardless of their sex or the habitat where they were born (Fig 1B). Although social variables were weakly related to dispersal distances, individuals born in areas with low conspecific density tended to dispersed greater distances than those born in high-density areas (Table 2). Importantly, when habitat was removed from the analysis, conspecific density received stronger support as a predictor of dispersal distances, with individuals born in high-density areas dispersing less than those born in more isolated sites (Table 2). This change in the result suggests that habitat differences in dispersal distances are actually related to the higher conspecific density recorded in the surroundings of urban compared to rural sites (estimate: 13.16, 95% CI: 10.99–15.33). Finally, we found no differences in conspecific density or productivity between natal and dispersal areas in both habitat types (conspecific density: estimate: 2.44, 95% confidence interval: -5.58–1.35; conspecific productivity: estimate: 0.65, 95% confidence interval: -0.29–1.59). However, individuals dispersing farther settled in areas more similar in terms of conspecific densities than those staying close to their natal areas (estimate: -1.17, 95% CI: -2.20 - -0.15), a pattern not observed when considering changes in conspecific productivity (estimate: 0.27; 95% CI: -0.77–1.32). It is worth noting that similar results were obtained when restricting our dataset to individuals resighted during their first breeding attempt (S1 Table).

thumbnail
Table 1. Models obtained to assess the relative importance of individual’s traits (sex and personality, measured as FID), and social variables (conspecific density and productivity in the natal area) on the natal dispersal distances of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

Models shown are the first 10 models ranked using their AICc. K: number of estimated parameters in approximating model, AICc: Akaike information criteria with small sample bias adjustment, ΔAICc: difference between each model and the best model (i.e., the model with the lowest AICc), weight: Akaike weight.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Relative importance of individual’s traits (sex and personality, measured as FID), and social variables (conspecific density and productivity in the natal area) on the natal dispersal distances of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (2.5% and 97.5%) obtained after averaging models in Table 1 (all models) and using the subset of models that did not include habitat (models without habitat). We considered that a given variable has no, weak or strong support when the 95% confidence interval strongly overlapped with zero, barely overlapped with zero (asterisk), or did not overlap with zero (in bold), respectively. Results remained unchanged when model averaging was performed using all candidate models, not only those with ΔAICc < 2 (S3 Table).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.t002

Correlates of natal dispersal distances on productivity and survival

Birds breeding for their first time in rural areas were less productive than those inhabiting urban ones (Tables 3 and 4). However, when they dispersed farther from their natal areas, they raised more chicks during their first breeding attempt than those staying nearby, a relationship that was absent among urban individuals. Besides, females dispersing at larger distances of their natal areas also increased their productivity in the first breeding event. When considering the long term productivity of individuals (data available for 144 individuals), we found that all birds, urban and rural, tended to increase productivity with age and when at greater natal dispersal distances (Table 4). Results remained consistent when we repeated models using only individuals resighted during their first breeding attempt (S2 Table).

thumbnail
Table 3. Models obtained to assess the relationship between natal dispersal distances and productivity during the first breeding attempt, and long term productivity of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

All models were run including year as a random term; models for long term productivity also included individual as a random term. Models shown are the first 10 models ranked using their AICc. K: number of estimated parameters in approximating model, AICc: Akaike information criteria with small sample bias adjustment, ΔAICc: difference between each model and the best model (i.e., the model with the lowest AICc), weight: Akaike weight.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (2.5% and 97.5%) obtained after model averaging to assess the relationship between natal dispersal distances and productivity during the first breeding attempt, and long term productivity of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

We considered that a given variable has no, weak or strong support when the 95% confidence interval strongly overlapped with zero, barely overlapped with zero (asterisk), or did not overlap with zero (in bold), respectively. Results remained unchanged when model averaging was performed using all candidate models, not only those with ΔAICc < 2 (S3 Table).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.t004

We estimated the effect of dispersal distances on future apparent survival probabilities using encounter histories of 248 individuals (1411 resightings). The overall goodness-of-fit test of the model was not statistically significant (χ2 = 34.34, d.f. = 43, p = 0.824). Model selection showed that resighting probabilities were lower for females (estimate: 0.82, 95% CI: 0.72–0.90) than for males (estimate: 0.93, 95% CI: 0.86–0.96; Table 5). Using this resighting structure, we tested the effect of natal dispersal distances on survival probabilities. The best model supported an interaction between dispersal distance and sex (Table 5), with females dispersing farther reducing their survival prospects compared to females staying closer (beta estimate: -1.21, 95% CI: -2.29 - -0.14). For males, future survival was not correlated with dispersal distances, as the estimate of the slope of this variable was not different to 0 (beta estimate: 1.60, 95% CI: -0.30–3.50; Fig 2). Habitat was not retained in models as a significant predictor of survival.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Relationship between natal dispersal and survival probabilities of male (gray lines) and female (black lines) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

Solid lines represent the general tendency obtained using the first model shown in Table 5; dashed lines: 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.g002

thumbnail
Table 5. Model comparison to assess the effects of natal dispersal distances (distance) on immediate survival probabilities of urban and rural (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

K: number of estimated parameters in approximating model, AICc: Akaike information criteria with small sample bias adjustment, ΔAICc: difference between each model and the best model (i.e., the model with the lowest AICc), deviance: deviance explained by each model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.t005

Discussion

Our results show that variability in the natal dispersal distances observed among burrowing owls was mainly explained by the additive effects of sex, personality and habitat. As expected, females moved farther distances than males in both habitat types, while urban birds (both males and females) dispersed over shorter distances compared to rural ones, partly because of the higher conspecific density recorded in urban compared to rural areas. Moreover, bold individuals—those with shorter FID—dispersed larger distances than their counterparts, regardless of their sex or the habitat and social environment in which they were born.

Sexual differences in natal dispersal distances are common among vertebrates to prevent mating between close relatives (inbreeding avoidance [66, 67, 68, 69]). However, the sex that disperses farther is not always the same and largely depends on the prevailing mating system of the species. In resource-defense mating systems, which is the prevailing system among birds, natal dispersal distances are generally shorter for males because they have to acquire and defend territories and, therefore, they may benefit from remaining near their natal area, where they are most familiar with resources and are probably best able to compete for them [49,70,71]. In our study species, males, but rarely females, actively defend an area larger than that immediately surrounding the nest burrow from conspecifics, with a more active response toward intruders at distances shorter than 100m [72]. This behavioral difference between sexes may underlie the sexual differences observed in the dispersal distances of individuals.

Sex is not the only individual trait affecting dispersal distances among burrowing owls. Previous findings showed that bolder, more asocial, and more active individuals can move greater distances and are more suited to colonizing new areas than shyer, social and less active ones [15,73,74]. In agreement with these results, we found a significant negative relationship between FID and natal dispersal among both urban and rural males and females. After settling in a breeding site, however, these bold individuals are more faithful than shy ones [39]. This apparent contradiction can be solved when considering the different behaviors correlated with FID. On the one hand, FID is positively correlated with explorative behavior [34]. Thus, bold individuals, which are also more explorative, can search for breeding sites exploring wider areas during their natal displacements than shy owls. When settled, however, bold individuals tend to remain in their breeding sites during consecutive years, even after suffering predation events that may cause their breeding failure [39]. A frequent finding from a wide range of vertebrate species is that individuals may change breeding sites after a poor breeding performance [75, 76, 77], or under predation pressures [78, 79, 80]. However, bolder individuals, which are also more aggressive toward predators [34], can choose to remain in their breeding site and cope with this important conditioning to take advantage of site familiarity.

Social variables received limited support in our modeling approach when we considered the habitat where individuals were born. However, after removing habitat from models, conspecific density became a strong predictor of dispersal distances, with individuals born in areas with low conspecific density covering greater distances than those born in high density areas. Thus, differences in natal dispersal patterns among urban and rural birds could be partially attributed to differences in conspecific density between habitats. Although high population densities can reduce individual fitness via increased competition for resources or direct interferences between individuals, thus promoting dispersal [81], our results did not support this hypothesis. Conversely, burrowing owls dispersed shorter distances when born in high-density areas. Several studies have shown that individuals use information about conspecifics (i.e., their presence, density or breeding performance) to predict habitat quality and thus select whether or not to settle [52, 82, 83, 84]. Young burrowing owls recruit into their breeding territories during their first year of life, so they are not able to use conspecific productivity as a proxy of habitat quality. Conversely, they can use conspecific density. Conspecific density can correlate with habitat quality due to the movement of individuals to high-quality patches and/or to the differential mortality of resident conspecifics. In our study species, predation is the main determinant of breeding failure [39, 41], so areas with a high density of conspecifics can represent areas where predation risk is rather low.

In vertebrates, current evidence suggests that natal dispersal could be penalized in terms of fitness [4], although some researches have reported opposite patterns [85, 86]. The low natal dispersal distances observed in our study suggest that moving far from areas where individuals were born is not the preferred strategy for burrowing owls. However, when analyzing the relationships between individual survival and breeding prospects, we found that females and rural individuals dispersing farther from their natal areas raised more chicks during their first breeding attempt than those staying close, a relationship that was absent among urban individuals. Moreover, long term productivity tended to increase when both urban and rural individuals dispersed at greater distances from their natal areas. Contrarily, females dispersing farther reduced their survival prospects compared to females staying closer and males. Thus, the reproductive benefits associated with large natal dispersal in females are outweighed by its survival costs [87, 88, 89]. This, along with the benefits obtained by males that stay close to natal areas, explain the low dispersal distances observed in the whole population. Although we cannot discard the possibility that the lower survival of females dispersing longer distances arises as a consequence of permanent emigration, the large size our study area (5400km2) and the intensive monitoring we performed (as shown by the large resignting probability observed for all individuals, independently of their sex and habitat) reduce this likelihood [4].

In conclusion, we found evidence supporting a role for individual traits (sex and personality) and conspecific density in explaining variability in the natal dispersal patterns of burrowing owls. Although urban areas per se did not affect this demographic parameter, the lower predation risk experienced by urban individuals may favor greater conspecific densities, which actually reduce dispersal propensity. From an evolutionary perspective, and although it is known that very low rates of dispersal among subpopulations are sufficient for the system to behave as a panmictic population [90], rates of dispersal among subpopulations determine the level of gene flow and could, therefore, affect processes such as local adaptation. Thus, the low natal dispersal distances combined with reduced breeding dispersal may be the underlying cause explaining the genetic structure detected among urban and rural populations of burrowing owls at small spatial scales [91]. Further research is needed to assess the generalizability of these results to other taxa to properly ascertain the consequences of urbanization in the ecology and evolution of species thriving in anthropogenic areas.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Relative importance of individual’s traits (sex and personality, measured as FID), and social variables (conspecific density and productivity in the natal area) on the natal dispersal distances of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

These models were run using individuals resighted during their first breeding attempts (n = 189 individuals). Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (2.5% and 97.5%) were assessed after model averaging. We considered that a given variable has no, weak or strong support when the 95% confidence interval strongly overlapped zero, barely overlapped zero (asterisk), or did not overlap zero (in bold), respectively. Models shown are the first 10 models ranked using their AICc. Variable (*): model averaging performed using the subset of models that did not include habitat.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Relationship between natal dispersal distances and productivity during the first breeding attempt, and long term productivity of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

These models were run using individuals resighted during their first breeding attempts (n = 189 individuals). Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (2.5% and 97.5%) were assessed after model averaging. We considered that a given variable has no, weak or strong support when the 95% confidence interval strongly overlapped zero, barely overlapped zero (asterisk), or did not overlap zero (in bold), respectively. All models were run including year as a random term; models for long term productivity also included individual as a random term. Models shown are the first 10 models ranked using their AICc.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Relative importance of individual’s traits (sex and personality, measured as FID), and social variables (conspecific density and productivity in the natal area) on the natal dispersal distances of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (2.5% and 97.5%) obtained after averaging all candidate models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (2.5% and 97.5%) obtained after model averaging to assess the relationship between natal dispersal distances and productivity during the first breeding attempt, and long term productivity of rural and urban (habitat) burrowing owls Athene cunicularia.

Estimates and confidence intervals were obtained after averaging all candidate models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226089.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Sergio Zalba (Universidad Nacional del Sur, Argentina) for his continuous support. We also thank the volunteers, technicians and researchers who helped in capturing and monitoring owls over the years, and the neighbors of Bahia Blanca for their hospitality and information provided. The fieldwork in Argentina was conducted with authorization from Argentinean wildlife agencies and with the permission of the owners of houses and farms. Sarah Young revised the English and two anonymous reviewers provide comments to improve the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Greenwood PJ, Harvey PH. The natal and breeding dispersal of birds. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1982; 13(1): 1–21. www.jstor.org/stable/2097060.
  2. 2. Forero MG, Donázar JA, Hiraldo F. Causes and fitness consequences of natal dispersal in a population of black kites. Ecology. 2002; 83(3): 858–872.
  3. 3. Doligez B, Pärt T. Estimating fitness consequences of dispersal: A road to “know-where”? Non-random dispersal and the underestimation of dispersers’ fitness. J Anim Ecol. 2008; 77(6):1199–211. pmid:18808435
  4. 4. Serrano D, Tella JL. Lifetime fitness correlates of natal dispersal distance in a colonial bird. J. Anim. Ecol. 2012; 81(1): 97–107. pmid:21644982
  5. 5. Skellam JG. Random dispersal in theoretical populations. Biometrika. 1951; 38(1/2): 196–218. https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/38.1-2.196.
  6. 6. Kokko H, López-Sepulcre A. From individual dispersal to species ranges: perspectives for a changing world. Science. 2006; 313(5788): 789–791. pmid:16902127
  7. 7. Jongejans E, Shea K, Skarpaas O, Kelly D, Sheppard AW, Woodburn TL. Dispersal and demography contributions to population spread of Carduus nutans in its native and invaded ranges. J Ecol. 2008; 96(4): 687–697.
  8. 8. Baguette M, Blanchet S, Legrand D, Stevens V M, Turlure C. Individual dispersal, landscape connectivity and ecological networks. Biol Rev. 2013; 88(2): 310–326. pmid:23176626
  9. 9. Kubisch A, Holt RD, Poethke HJ, Fronhofer EA. Where am I and why? Synthesizing range biology and the eco‐evolutionary dynamics of dispersal. Oikos. 2014; 123(1): 5–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2013.00706.x.
  10. 10. Canestrelli D, Bisconti R, Carere C. Bolder takes all? The behavioral dimension of biogeography. Trends Ecol Evol. 2016; 31(1): 35–43. pmid:26688444
  11. 11. Bonte D, Dahirel M. Dispersal: a central and independent trait in life history. Oikos. 2017; 126(4): 472–479.
  12. 12. Doligez B, Pärt T, Danchin E, Clobert J, Gustafsson L. Availability and use of public information and conspecific density for settlement decisions in the collared flycatcher. J Anim Ecol. 2004; 73(1): 75–87. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2004.00782.x.
  13. 13. Garant D, Kruuk LE, Wilkin TA, McCleery, RH, Sheldon, BC. Evolution driven by differential dispersal within a wild bird population. Nature. 2005; 433(7021): 60. pmid:15635409
  14. 14. Davis JM, Stamps JA. The effect of natal experience on habitat preferences. Trends Ecol Evol. 2004; 19(8): 411–416. pmid:16701298
  15. 15. Fraser DF, Gilliam JF, Daley MJ, Le AN, Skalski GT. Explaining leptokurtic movement distributions: intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration. Am Nat. 2001; 158(2): 124–135. pmid:18707341
  16. 16. Bize P, Daniel G, Viblanc VA, Martin JG, Doligez B. Negative phenotypic and genetic correlation between natal dispersal propensity and nest-defence behaviour in a wild bird. Biol Lett. 2017; 13(7): 20170236. pmid:28747532
  17. 17. Camacho C, Canal D, Potti J. Nonrandom dispersal drives phenotypic divergence within a bird population. Ecol Evol. 2013; 3(14): 4841–4848. pmid:24363908
  18. 18. Camacho C, Canal D, Potti J. Testing the matching habitat choice hypothesis in nature: phenotype-environment correlation and fitness in a songbird population. Evol Ecol. 2015; 29(6): 873–886.
  19. 19. Barbraud C, Johnson AR, Bertault G.Phenotypic correlates of post‐fledging dispersal in a population of greater flamingos: the importance of body condition. J Anim Ecol. 2003; 72(2): 246–257. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2656.2003.00695.x.
  20. 20. Trochet A, Courtois EA, Stevens VM, Baguette M, Chaine A, Schmeller DS, et al. Evolution of sex-biased dispersal. Q Rev Biol. 2016; 91(3): 297–320. pmid:29558614
  21. 21. Clobert J, Le Galliard JF, Cote J, Meylan S, Massot M. Informed dispersal, heterogeneity in animal dispersal syndromes and the dynamics of spatially structured populations. Ecol Lett. 2009; 12(3):197–209. pmid:19170731
  22. 22. Grimm NB, Faeth SH, Golubiewski NE, Redman CL, Wu J, Bai X, et al. Global change and the ecology of cities. Science. 2008; 319756–60. pmid:18258902
  23. 23. Montgomery M. United Nations Population Fund: State of world population 2007: Unleashing the potential of urban growth. Population and Development Review. 2007; 33(3): 639–641. https://www.unfpa.org/sites/default/files/pub-pdf/695_filename_sowp2007_eng.pdf.
  24. 24. Roberts L. 9 Billion?. Science. 2011; 6042 (333):540–543.
  25. 25. McKinney ML, Lockwood JL. Biotic homogenization: a few winners replacing many losers in the next mass extinction. Trends Ecol Evol. 1999; 14(11): 450–453. pmid:10511724
  26. 26. McKinney ML. Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homogenization. Biol Conserv. 2006; 127(3): 247–260.
  27. 27. Sol D, González‐Lagos C, Moreira D, Maspons J, Lapiedra O. Urbanisation tolerance and the loss of avian diversity. Ecol Lett. 2014; 17(8): 942–950. pmid:24835452
  28. 28. Luna Á, Romero-Vidal P, Hiraldo F, Tella JL. Cities may save some threatened species but not their ecological functions. PeerJ. 2018; 6, e4908. pmid:29942675
  29. 29. Aronson MF, La Sorte FA, Nilon CH, Katti M, Goddard MA, Lepczyk CA, et al. A global analysis of the impacts of urbanization on bird and plant diversity reveals key anthropogenic drivers. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014; 281(1780): 20133330. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3330.
  30. 30. Carrete M, Tella JL. Individual consistency in flight initiation distances in burrowing owls: a new hypothesis on disturbance-induced habitat selection. Biol Lett. 2010; 6: 167–170. pmid:19864278
  31. 31. Møller AP. Interspecific variation in fear responses predicts urbanization in birds. Behav Ecol. 2010; 21(2): 365–371. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arp199.
  32. 32. Carrete M, Tella JL. Inter-individual variability in fear of humans and relative brain size of the species are related to contemporary urban invasion in birds. PLoS One. 2011; 6(4). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018859.
  33. 33. Rebolo-Ifrán N, Carrete M, Sanz-aguilar A, Rodriguez-Martínez S. Links between fear of humans, stress and survival support a non-random distribution of birds among urban and rural habitats. Sci. Rep. 2015; 5, 13723. pmid:26348294
  34. 34. Carrete M, Tella JL. Behavioral correlations associated with fear of humans differ between rural and urban burrowing owls. Front Ecol Evol. 2017; 5,54. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2017.00054.
  35. 35. Sprau P, Dingemanse NJ. An approach to distinguish between plasticity and non-random distributions of behavioral types along urban gradients in a wild passerine bird. Front Ecol Evol. 2017; 5, 92. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2017.00092.
  36. 36. Carrete M, Tella JL. High individual consistency in fear of humans throughout the adult lifespan of rural and urban burrowing owls. Sci Rep. 2013; 3, 3524. pmid:24343659
  37. 37. Bubac CM, Coltman DW, Bowen WD, Lidgard DC, Lang SL, den Heyer CE. Repeatability and reproductive consequences of boldness in female gray seals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2018; 72(6): 100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2515-5.
  38. 38. Carrete M, Martínez-Padilla J, Rodríguez-Martínez S, Rebolo-Ifrán N, Palma A, Tella JL. Heritability of fear of humans in urban and rural populations of a bird species. Sci Rep. 2016; 6,31060. pmid:27499420
  39. 39. Luna Á, Palma A, Sanz-Aguilar A, Tella JL, Carrete M. Personality-dependent breeding dispersal in rural but not urban burrowing owls. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1), 2886. pmid:30814548
  40. 40. Díaz M, Møller AP, Flensted-Jensen E, Grim T, Ibáñez-Álamo JD, Jokimäki J, et al. The geography of fear: a latitudinal gradient in anti-predator escape distances of birds across Europe. PloS one. 2013; 8(5), e64634. 10.1371/journal.pone.0064634. pmid:23724070
  41. 41. Rebolo-Ifrán N, Tella JL, Carrete M. Urban conservation hotspots: predation release allows the grassland-specialist burrowing owl to perform better in the city. Sci Rep. 2017; 7(1), 3527. pmid:28615700
  42. 42. Møller AP, Diaz M, Flensted-Jensen E, Grim T, Ibáñez-Álamo JD, Jokimäki J, et al. High urban population density of birds reflects their timing of urbanization. Oecologia. 2012; 170(3): 867–875. pmid:22588633
  43. 43. Stracey CM, Robinson SK. Are urban habitats ecological traps for a native songbird? Season‐long productivity, apparent survival, and site fidelity in urban and rural habitats. J Avian Biol. 2012; 43(1): 50–60.
  44. 44. Rodriguez-Martínez S, Carrete M, Roques S, Rebolo-Ifrán N, Tella JL. High urban breeding densities do not disrupt genetic monogamy in a bird species. PLoS One. 2014; 9(3). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091314.
  45. 45. Tella JL, Canale A, Carrete M, Petracci P, Zalba SM. Anthropogenic nesting sites allow urban breeding in burrowing parrots Cyanoliseus patagonus. Ardeola. 2014; 61(2): 311–322.
  46. 46. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Van Noordwijk AJ, Rutten AL, Drent PJ. Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits (Parus major). Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003; 270(1516): 741–747. pmid:12713749
  47. 47. Cote J, Clobert J, Brodin T, Fogarty S, Sih A. Personality-dependent dispersal: characterization, ontogeny and consequences for spatially structured populations. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2010; 365(1560):4065–76. pmid:21078658
  48. 48. Duckworth RA, Badyaev AV. Coupling of dispersal and aggression facilitates the rapid range expansion of a passerine bird. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104(38): 15017–15022. pmid:17827278
  49. 49. Clarke AL, Sæther BE, Roskaft E. Sex biases in avian dispersal: a reappraisal. Oikos. 1997; 79(3): 429–438. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3546885.
  50. 50. Dale S. Female biased dispersal, low female recruitment, unpaired males, and the extinction of small and isolated bird populations. Oikos. 2001; 92(2): 344–356. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3547240.
  51. 51. Boulinier T, Yoccoz NG, McCoy KD, Erikstad KE, Tveraa T. Testing the effect of conspecific reproductive success on dispersal and recruitment decisions in a colonial bird: design issues. J Appl Stat. 2002; 29(1–4):509–520.
  52. 52. Serrano D, Tella JL, Donázar JA, Pomarol M. Social and individual features affecting natal dispersal in the colonial lesser kestrel. Ecology. 2003; 84(11): 3044–3054.
  53. 53. Payo-Payo A, Sanz-Aguilar A, Genovart M, Bertolero A, Piccardo J, Camps F, et al. Predator arrival elicits differential dispersal, change in age structure and reproductive performance in a prey population. Sci Rep. 2018; 8,1971. pmid:29386550
  54. 54. Matthysen E. Density-dependent dispersal in birds and mammals. Ecography. 2005; 28(3):403–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0906-7590.2005.04073.
  55. 55. Rodrigues AM, Johnstone RA. Evolution of positive and negative density-dependent dispersal. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014; 281(1791): 20141226. pmid:25100700
  56. 56. Carrete M, Sánchez-Zapata JA, Tella JL., Gil-Sánchez JM, Moleón M. Components of breeding performance in two competing species: habitat heterogeneity, individual quality and density-dependence. Oikos. 2006; 112: 680–690. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3548734.
  57. 57. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference. A practical information-theoretic approach. New York: Springer-Verlag.2002. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-2917-7_3
  58. 58. Barton K. MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. 2018. R package version 1.42.1 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn.
  59. 59. Grueber CE, Nakagawa S, Laws RJ,Jamieson IG. Multimodel inference in ecology and evolution: challenges and solutions. J Evol Biol. 2011; 24(4): 699–711. pmid:21272107
  60. 60. Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed effects models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2013; 4(2): 133–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x.
  61. 61. R Development Core Team R. A language and environment for statistical computing, http://www.R-project.org. 2008.
  62. 62. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015; 67(1): 1–48.
  63. 63. Lebreton JD, Burnham KP, Clobert J, Anderson DR. Modeling survival and testing biological hypotheses using marked animals: a unified approach with case studies. Ecol Monogr. 1992; 62(1): 67–118. https://doi.org/10.2307/2937171.
  64. 64. Choquet R, Lebreton JD, Gimenez O, Reboulet AM, Pradel R. U‐CARE: Utilities for performing goodness of fit tests and manipulating CApture–REcapture data. Ecography. 2009; 32(6): 1071–1074. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0587.2009.05968.x.
  65. 65. Choquet R, Rouan L, Pradel R. Program E-SURGE: a software application for fitting multievent models. In Modeling demographic processes in marked populations. pp. 845–865. Springer, Boston, MA,2009.
  66. 66. Pusey AE. Sex-biased dispersal and inbreeding avoidance in birds and mammals. Trends Ecol Evol.1987; 2(10): 295–299. pmid:21227869
  67. 67. Pusey A, Wolf M. Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends Ecol Evol. 1996; 11(5): 201–206. pmid:21237809
  68. 68. Szulkin M, Sheldon BC. Dispersal as a means of inbreeding avoidance in a wild bird population. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008; 275(1635): 703–711. pmid:18211876
  69. 69. Lebigre C, Alatalo RV, Siitari H. Female‐biased dispersal alone can reduce the occurrence of inbreeding in black grouse (Tetrao tetrix). Mol Ecol. 2010; 19(9): 1929–1939. pmid:20345672
  70. 70. Pärt T. The importance of local familiarity and search costs for age-and sex-biased philopatry in the collared flycatcher. Anim Behav. 1995; 49(4): 1029–1038. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1995.0132.
  71. 71. Piper WH, Walcott C, Mager JN, Spilker FJ. Nestsite selection by male loons leads to sex‐biased site familiarity. J Anim Ecol. 2008; 77(2): 205–210. pmid:17976165
  72. 72. Moulton CE, Brady RS, Belthoff JR. Territory defense of nesting Burrowing Owls: responses to simulated conspecific intrusion. J. Field Ornithol. 2004; 75(3): 288–295. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4151311?origin=JSTOR-pdf.
  73. 73. Cote J, Fogarty S, Weinersmith K, Brodin T, Sih A. Personality traits and dispersal tendency in the invasive mosquitofish (Gambusia affinis). Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010; 277(1687): 1571–1579. pmid:20071380
  74. 74. Fogarty S, Cote J, Sih A. Social personality polymorphism and the spread of invasive species: a model. Am Nat. 2001; 177(3): 273–287. pmid:21460537
  75. 75. Beletsky LD, Orians GH. Effects of breeding experience and familiarity on site fidelity in female red‐winged blackbirds. Ecology.1991. 72(3): 787–796. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1940581.
  76. 76. Nager RG, Johnson AR, Boy V, Rendon-Martos M, Calderon J, Cézilly F. Temporal and spatial variation in dispersal in the greater flamingo (Phoenicopterus ruber roseus). Oecologia. 1996; 107(2): 204–211. pmid:28307306
  77. 77. Forero MG, Donázar JA, Blas J, Hiraldo FCauses and consequences of territory change and breeding dispersal distance in the Black Kite. Ecology. 1999; 80(4): 1298–1310.
  78. 78. Greig-Smith PW. Dispersal between nest-sites by stonechats Saxicola torquata in relation to previous breeding success. Ornis Scandinavica;1982; 232–23.
  79. 79. Gavin TA, Bollinger EK. Reproductive correlates of breeding‐site fidelity in Bobolinks (Dolichonyx oryzivorus). Ecology. 1988; 69(1): 96–103.
  80. 80. Wiklund CG. Breeding lifespan and nest predation determine lifetime production of fledglings by male Merlins Falco columbarius. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;263(1371): 723–728.
  81. 81. Bowler DE, Benton TG. Causes and consequences of animal dispersal strategies: relating individual behaviour to spatial dynamics. Biol. Rev. 2005; 80: 205–225. pmid:15921049
  82. 82. Danchin E, Boulinier T, Massot M. Conspecific reproductive success and breeding habitat selection: implications for the study of coloniality. Ecology. 1998; 79(7): 2415–2428.
  83. 83. Brown CR, Brown MB, Danchin E. Breeding habitat selection in cliff swallows: The effect of conspecific reproductive success on colony choice. J Anim Ecol. 2000; 69(1):133–42. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2656.2000.00382.x.
  84. 84. Danchin E, Giraldeau LA, Valone TJ, Wagner RH. Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science.2004; 305(5683): 487–491. pmid:15273386
  85. 85. Lemel JY, Belichon S, Clobert J, Hochberg ME. The evolution of dispersal in a two-patch system: some consequences of differences between migrants and residents. Evo Ecol. 1997; 11(5): 613–629. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-997-1516-z.
  86. 86. Spear LB, Pyle P, Nur N. Natal dispersal in the western gull: proximal factors and fitness consequences. J Anim Ecol. 1998;67(2): 165–179. https://www.jstor.org/stable/2647486.
  87. 87. Tarwater CE, Brawn JD. Family living in a Neotropical bird: variation in timing of dispersal and higher survival for delayed dispersers. Anim Behav. 2010; 80(3): 535–542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.017.
  88. 88. Bonte D, Van Dyck H, Bullock JM, Coulon A, Delgado M, Gibbs M, et al. Costs of dispersal. Biol Rev. 2012; 87(2): 290–312. pmid:21929715
  89. 89. Cox AS, Kesler DC. Reevaluating the cost of natal dispersal: post-fledging survival of red-bellied woodpeckers. The Condor. 2012; 114(2): 341–347. https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2012.110070.
  90. 90. Hoelzel AR, Dover GA. Genetic differentiation between sympatric killer whale populations. Heredity. 1991; 66(2): 191.
  91. 91. Mueller JC, Kuhl H, Boerno S, Tella JL, Carrete M, Kempenaers B. Evolution of genomic variation in the burrowing owl in response to recent colonization of urban areas. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2018; 285(1878): 20180206.