Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Comparing spousal agreement on perceived responsibility for household natural hazard preparedness to actual behavior

Abstract

This study compares husbands' and wives' views on the person in a couple who should be responsible for preparing for hurricane hazards; it also examines whether the varying levels of agreement reached by husbands and wives regarding this responsibility are associated with actual preparedness behaviors. An online survey targeting married, heterosexual couples living in Sarasota County, Florida, USA was sent out between March and May, 2015. Both the husbands and the wives were asked to fill out the survey. A total of 170 surveys were used for analysis. Results suggested that husbands and wives felt that they had shared responsibility for most of the 19 preparedness behaviors considered. However, a few stereotypically masculine preparedness behaviors were found to typically fall to husbands. Husbands' and wives' views of perceived responsibility were not statistically different, but husbands tended to favor individual responsibility, while wives tended to favor joint responsibility. Higher levels of agreement were significantly associated with greater engagement in planning-related preparedness behaviors. Policy implications are discussed.

Introduction

Natural disasters cause human fatalities, economic loss, social problems, population displacement, and environmental damage, thus resulting in unsustainable development [1]. Hazard mitigation activities, including preparedness activities, play important roles in sustainable development [2]. Household natural hazard preparedness has been championed by researchers and practitioners as a method of reducing people’s losses from natural disasters. There is established literature on household natural hazard preparedness, but studies thus far have seldom considered household decision-making regarding natural hazard preparedness in either the familial or household context (see exceptions like [3]). In particular, this study addresses: (1) married couples’ views of and levels of agreement about perceived responsibility for preparing for hurricane hazards; and (2) whether these levels of agreement are associated with actual preparedness behaviors. The next section provides a brief literature review of studies on household natural hazard preparedness, as well as couples’ views of and levels of agreement. This section is followed by research methods, results, the discussion, and the conclusion.

Literature review

Household natural hazard preparedness

Hazard preparedness encompasses the pre-disaster actions that provide the human and material resources needed to support active responses when disasters occur [4]. Hazard preparedness is aimed at various targets, including businesses [5], communities [6], households/families [7,8], and individuals [9]. At the household level, researchers have been eager to understand whether households are prepared for natural hazards, examining levels of preparedness by asking household members if they have prepared specific items, such as water or canned foods, or have engaged in specific behaviors, such as developing a household evacuation plan [10]. Most of these studies have suggested that households are ill-prepared for natural hazards [10,11].

Aiming to increase the overall level of household natural hazard preparedness, studies have examined the factors that influence preparedness levels. To date, studies have proposed various factors [10,12,13], including cognitive/psychological factors such as fear, risk perception, outcome expectancy, self-efficacy, and protective response costs [1416]; social factors such as place attachment [17]; political factors, such as trust in public natural hazard protection [18]; and geographical factors, such as hazard exposure [19]. Many of the studies have also examined the relationship between household natural hazard preparedness and household/family characteristics, such as home ownership, number of children, and sociodemographic factors such as age [20] and educational attainment [10]. The results of the examination of the relationships between abovementioned factors and household natural hazard preparedness vary, however [21].

One of the less studied topics in household natural hazard preparedness is the decision-making process that leads to household natural hazard preparedness [12]. To better understand the underlying process that leads to preparedness behaviors, one needs to take into account the interactions and communications among household members, or intra-household dynamics, surrounding household natural hazard preparedness issues [22,23]. For example, Hung [12] divided the household decision-making process regarding household hurricane preparedness into three stages–problem initiation, information search and evaluation, and final decisions–and found that at least half of the married heterosexual couples who were surveyed indicated that they made joint decisions throughout the three stages. Studies have suggested that household natural hazard preparedness behaviors are also related to household members’ gender roles, or divisions of labor [3,23,24]. Finally, some qualitative research on natural hazard preparedness, such as that of Goodman and Cottrell [25], has considered the importance of spousal agreement for household natural hazard preparedness decision-making. To the author’s knowledge, however, there are no studies in either natural hazard or natural disaster research that have systematically examined couples’ views and levels of agreement, or the relationship between levels of agreement and actual behaviors. Questions about the influence of levels of spousal agreement on actual household natural hazard related behaviors, such as evacuation and preparedness, thus remain unanswered.

Couples’ views and levels of agreement

Gender differences in human behavior have attracted the attention of scholars from various academic disciplines, including sociology, economics, psychology, and behavioral science [2628]. As heterosexual couples remain the predominant type of couple in many countries, many studies have investigated the differences between husbands and wives, such as married couples’ views of and levels of agreement regarding family- or household-related topics. Researchers in family studies believe that spousal consensus is an important factor for understanding marital relations [29]. As systems theory [30] and family systems theory [31] suggest, spouses influence each other’s attitudes and behaviors. As a result, it is important to consider the views and influence of spouses regarding household-related topics. A couple’s level of agreement or consensus is usually measured by either the events or conditions experienced by both partners, such as household decision-making or conflict resolution, or by the similarities of the partners’ attitudes on topics such as family planning [29]. Couple-level data addresses methodological issues that occur in individual-level data, reveals gender differences on targeted topics [32], and in some cases, shows that spouses’ agreement (or disagreement) is a predictor of household issues.

Some of the studies by family researchers have examined topics including the relationship between marital happiness and spousal consensus on beliefs about marital conflict [33], as well as marital satisfaction and spouses’ attitudinal similarity regarding religious beliefs and marital roles [34]. In fertility studies, understanding spouses’ levels of agreement regarding fertility issues is important because disagreement may hinder fertility decisions, and when disagreement exists, surveying one person on the topic produces only partial information [35,36]. In a study on household decision-making, it was found that about 24 percent of the surveyed couples would be misrepresented in decision-making if only one person within the couple was surveyed due to discrepancies in reporting [37]. In terms of household division of labor, Kamo [38] found that husbands tend to overestimate their own contributions to household work (or wives underestimate the husbands’ contributions) but wives do not, and husbands tend to overestimate (or wives underestimate) wives’ contributions to shopping and paying bills. In a study of patients’ well-being, Cremeans-Smith et al. [39] indicated that when a patient and his or her spouse had similar perceptions (i.e., agreement) of the pain severity of osteoarthritis, the patient indicated better well-being than those patients whose spouses underestimated their pain.

In sum, although various studies have looked into the relationship between gender and natural hazards/natural disasters [4043], few studies have investigated the views of and agreements between married heterosexual partners in relation to natural hazard preparedness decision-making. Using an online survey, this study asked married couples living in Sarasota County, Florida, USA, a hurricane-prone county [44], to answer questions about household hurricane hazard preparedness and to share their views about the person who should be responsible for preparing for hurricanes. The reported results include husbands’ and wives’ views on their perceived responsibility, as well as their levels of agreement and disagreement when considering the perceived responsibility at the couple level. This study also examines whether spouses’ levels of agreement about responsibility predict actual preparedness behaviors. The research questions include:

  1. Do husbands and wives have the same views on who should be responsible for specific household hurricane hazard preparedness behaviors?
  2. Is there an association between the level of spousal agreement regarding who should be responsible for specific household hazard preparedness behaviors and the actual preparedness behaviors?

Methods

This study is based in part on the results from the author’s mixed-methods dissertation [45]. Some of the quantitative results from the dissertation have been published in Hung [12], while some of the qualitative results have been published in Hung [23]. As this study used the same survey dataset as that of Hung [12], only brief descriptions of the methods and sample characteristics are given here. Please refer to Hung [12] for more details. This study was approved by The Office for Research Protection at The Pennsylvania State University (IRB#: STUDY00002066). The associated file that contains survey data can be found at S1 File.

Survey design

An online survey was designed using the online survey platform Qualtrics. This online survey had three parts. Part I asked a couple to answer questions regarding household hurricane preparedness behaviors as well as household characteristics. Household hurricane preparedness behaviors included 19 items or behaviors [20,46,47] (See S1 Table for list). Participants answered “yes” or “no” for each item, indicating whether the household had prepared the item (such as a three-day supply of water) or had undertaken the behavior (such as making a household evacuation plan) for hurricane-related emergencies. In addition to identifying preparedness behaviors, Part I investigated household-level variables, including length of marriage, household size, residence in a mobile home, home ownership or rental, and the presence of very young children (whether a couple has any infants, toddlers, or other preschool-aged children). Husbands and wives jointly answered the questions in Part I.

Part II and Part III had identical questions. If the husband filled out Part II, then the wife was instructed to fill out Part III, and vice versa. Each part contained an identical series of multiple-choice questions regarding the spouses’ views of who should be responsible for the 19 preparedness behaviors discussed in Part I. A specific question in the survey was: “Please indicate who you think should be primarily responsible for each of the following,” and participants were given the 19 preparedness behaviors and four possible answers: “wife,” “husband,” “both equally responsible,” and “I don’t know.” The categories of “wife,” “husband,” and “joint” are widely used for understanding the roles played by husbands and wives in household decision-making [4850]. In Part II and Part III, the 19 preparedness behaviors addressed in Part I were linguistically transformed from nouns indicating preparedness items or preparedness behaviors to gerund phrases describing preparedness actions. For example, “a can opener” in Part I became “preparing a can opener” in Part II and Part III; “knowledge of how to turn off the utilities” became “knowing how to turn off the utilities” (see S1 Table for full list of wording changes). In addition to perceived preparedness responsibility questions, information regarding participants’ hurricane risk perceptions and their hurricane experiences was collected. Hurricane risk perceptions were measured using four 5-point Likert scale questions regarding the probability and severity of hurricanes striking Sarasota County (revised from [5]). Hurricane experiences were measured using five yes/no questions regarding participants’ hurricane experiences, including their own experiences, their family members’ experiences, and their relatives’ or friends’ experience (revised from [5,51]).

It should be noted that the research design of this study has two limitations: (1) it is impossible to detect whether the questionnaires were actually filled out as instructed, with husbands and wives jointly finishing Part I of the survey and each of the spouses filling out either Part II or Part III independently; (2) the survey procedure that asked husbands and wives to answer jointly in Part I, followed by individually in Part II and Part III, may have led more couples to select “equally responsible.”

Survey implementation

This online survey was distributed via two methods between March and May 2015. With the help of the Sarasota County Office of Emergency Management and participants’ homeowner associations (HOAs) in Sarasota County, the survey was sent to contact persons in the HOAs and then forwarded to potential participants in each of the HOAs. This first approach, however, generated an unsatisfactorily low response, and Research Now, an online panel company, was employed to obtain a larger sample of Sarasota County married-couple households.

Analytical methods

All of the analysis was conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics Subscription version 1.0.0.800. In answering the first research question, counts and percentages were used to indicate views regarding responsibility for household hurricane preparedness at both the individual and couple levels. Kappa statistics were used to assess the level of agreement between husbands and wives regarding responsibility for household natural hazard preparedness behaviors. Kappa coefficients were used to adjust the percentages of agreement by taking into account instances of agreement that had happened by chance. The coefficients ranged from 0, indicating complete disagreement, to 1, suggesting perfect agreement. In addition, to understand whether husbands and wives held the same views on perceived responsibility, a chi-square test (2 × 4) for each of the preparedness items/behaviors was performed between spousal views (husbands’ views and wives’ views) and perceived responsibility (perceived wife responsibility, perceived husband responsibility, perceived both responsibility, and uncertain). Statically significant results suggested that husbands’ reviews and wives’ reviews regarding the perceived responsibility for household hurricane preparedness are significantly differ from each other.

To answer the second research question, chi-square tests (2 x 2) were used to examine the association between the level of agreement regarding preparedness responsibility (i.e., agreement and disagreement) and whether the preparedness items were actually secured or preparedness behaviors were actually completed (i.e., prepared or not prepared). The designation of spousal agreement on perceived preparedness responsibility was used to indicate that the husband and wife in a given couple both chose “wife,” “husband,” or “both equally responsible” for a given preparedness behavior. No agreement indicates that the husband and wife did not choose the same answers, or they both chose “I don’t know.” While both the husband and wife in a given couple choosing “I don’t know” indicates agreement, this agreement was not likely to be associated with actual behaviors. As a result, those cases in which the husband and wife in a given couple both chose “I don’t know” were labeled as indicative of no agreement. To confirm the relationships, logistic regressions were used. These regressions examined whether agreement remained a statistically significant predictor of those preparedness behaviors that had shown statistically significant associations in chi-square tests when controlling for particular variables (see the discussion in the next paragraph). A model comparison approach was used: In Model 1, only the control variables were included as predictors. In Model 2 (block 2), variable agreement was added. Statistically significant results for the difference in chi-squares between Model 1 and Model 2 suggested that incorporating variable agreement led to statistically significant improvement of the model.

Control variables for the logistic regressions included hurricane risk perceptions, hurricane experiences, household size, length of marriage, home ownership or rental, residence in a mobile home, and the presence of very young children. The selection of control variables was based on previous studies investigating household natural hazard preparedness [11,12,16,20,52]. While the latter five control variables are self-explanatory household-level sociodemographic variables, the treatment of hurricane risk perceptions and hurricane experiences, which were individual-level variables originally, needs more explanation. In accordance with the existing literature [53,54], the following strategy for creating couple-level variables based on individual-level data was used: For both hurricane risk perceptions and hurricane experiences, summative indices from husbands’ answers and wives’ answers were created. Then, the average number of hurricane risk perceptions and hurricane experiences between a particular husband and wife was calculated and used as the surrogate index representing hurricane risk perceptions and hurricane experiences for the given household.

Results

Descriptive statistics

A total of 305 survey responses were collected. Many of the responses, however, were not included in the analysis because the responses were either incomplete or identified as likely to have been filled out by only one participant in the household. Overall, 21 responses from HOAs and 149 responses from Research Now were available for analysis. As independent t-tests showed that the two samples did not statistically differ in terms of husbands’ age, wives’ age, husbands’ educational attainment, and wives’ educational attainment (data not shown), the two samples were than combined into one sample for further analysis (N = 170 couples).

Most of the surveyed households (84%) consisted of only two people–the surveyed married couple. About 13% of the households had children under 18 years old, and about 5% of the households included infants, toddlers, or preschool-aged children. Almost all of the surveyed married couples (95%) owned the houses they lived in, and only 8% of the surveyed married couples lived in mobile homes. For length of marriage, about 28% of the survey couples had been married 20 years or fewer, about 31% of the couples had been married for 21 to 40 years, and about 42% of the couples had been married for 41 to 60 years. More than half (60%) of the surveyed husbands were 66 years old or older, and many of them (57%) had at least a bachelor’s degree. Almost all of the husbands were non-Hispanic white. And about 70% of them were retired. For the surveyed wives, nearly half (44%) of them were 66 old or older, with similar percentage of them (43%) had at least a bachelor’s degree. The surveyed wives were also predominately non-Hispanic white. And about half of them (52%) were retired. Our sample is generally reflective of the demographics of Sarasota County. However, the dataset does have some differences when compared to the 2010 census data: This dataset contained fewer married-couple households with children, had significantly more male participants who are 66 years old or older, had fewer female participants between 26–45 years old, and had more female participants between 45–65 years old. For detailed information regarding the descriptive analysis of the sociodemographic variables and the comparison between the sample dataset and 2010 U.S. Census, see Hung [12].

Hurricane risk perceptions and hurricane experiences

Husbands (M = 12.59, SD = 2.64) and wives (M = 12.56, SD = 2.48) in this study had very similar levels of hurricane risk perceptions. The average number of hurricane risk perceptions between a particular husband and wife was calculated and used as the surrogate index representing the hurricane risk perceptions for the given household (M = 12.57, SD = 2.34). Husbands (M = 1.22, SD = 1.04) and wives (M = 1.25, SD = 1.06) also had very similar levels of hurricane experiences. The average number of hurricane experiences between a particular husband and wife was calculated and used as the surrogate index representing the hurricane experiences for the given household (M = 1.24, SD = 1).

Preparedness behaviors

Households in this sample had, on average, secured or completed 12.61 preparedness items/behaviors from the list of 19 preparedness items/behaviors. Table 1 shows the percentage of households that had prepared each of the specific items/behaviors. The most common preparedness items/behaviors were a can opener (98%), knowledge of how to turn off utilities (91%), and a three-day supply of medicine (91%). The least common preparedness items/behaviors were an electric generator (24%), a whistle and/or distress flag (24%), and a roof anchor (26%). The above three preparedness items/behaviors had lowest preparedness rates, likely due to the fact that they are hurricane-specific preparedness, compared to other items that are more likely to be commonly seen household items [12].

thumbnail
Table 1. Counts and percentages of households that had prepared each household hurricane preparedness item/behavior.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.t001

Couples’ views of perceived responsibility for household hurricane preparedness behaviors

Table 2 shows the surveyed husbands’ and wives’ views, in counts and percentages, of who should be responsible for the 19 household preparedness items/behaviors. The results shown in Table 2 indicate that for the 19 preparedness items/behaviors considered in this survey, both the husbands and wives surveyed thought that both members of the couple should be held equally responsible for most of the behaviors: over 50% of both husbands and wives chose the “both equally responsible” answer for 15 out of the 19 preparedness behaviors. Evacuation-related behaviors, such as “making a family evacuation plan,” “having an emergency contact outside of the family,” and “knowing the evacuation zone for your family,” garnered responses greater than 80% for “both equally responsible,” making them the highest percentages of all “both equally responsible” answers. Many of the survival items, such as “preparing a three-day supply of water” and “preparing sleeping bags or extra bedding,” had more than 70% “both equally responsible” answers. In contrast, tool-oriented and heavy items, such as “preparing an electric generator,” “preparing shutters for windows or stormproof windows,” and “preparing a roof anchor,” had lower percentages of “both equally responsible” answers and higher percentages of “husband-responsible” answers.

thumbnail
Table 2. Couples’ views on perceived responsibility for household hurricane preparedness behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.t002

At the aggregate level, the highest percentage in the “wife responsible” category was “preparing a three-day supply of canned food” (22%), and the lowest was “preparing an electric generator” (0%). The highest percentage in the “husband responsible” category was “preparing an electric generator” (51%), and the lowest was “having an emergency contact outside of the family” (2%). The highest percentage in the “both equally responsible category” was “knowing the evacuation zone for your family” (89%), and the lowest percentage was “preparing a roof anchor” (36%).

Table 3 shows the discrepancies (subtracting wives’ responses from husbands’ responses) in the couples’ views and in the chi-square test results between husbands’ views and wives’ views. Since the numbers come from subtracting wives’ responses from husbands’ responses, positive discrepancies in the “wife” category indicate that more husbands than wives think that wives are responsible for these behaviors, and negative discrepancies mean that more wives than husbands think that wives are responsible for these behaviors. Positive discrepancies in the “husband” category mean that more husbands than wives believe it is the husbands’ responsibility for these preparedness behaviors, and negative discrepancies mean that more wives than husbands believe it is husbands’ responsibility for these preparedness behaviors. Positive discrepancies in the “both equally responsible” category suggest that more husbands than wives believe the responsibility to prepare falls equally upon spouses, while negative discrepancies suggest that more wives than husbands believe this. None of the nineteen chi-square tests showed significant results, suggesting that husbands’ views and wives’ views regarding the perceived responsibility for household hurricane preparedness do not significantly differ from each other. However, there were discrepancies in the couples’ views that seem to suggest a trend. On the one hand, more wives than husbands think that both spouses are responsible for hurricane preparedness behaviors, even in the case of those items/behaviors considered to be stereotypically masculine, such as “preparing an electric generator” or “preparing a roof anchor.” This point is supported by the fact that negative discrepancies were found in all 19 preparedness behaviors for the “both equally responsible” category. The largest discrepancies were seen in “preparing charcoal, lighter, and a grill,” for which 49% of husbands and 61% of wives suggested that spouses had equal responsibility to prepare (percentages given in Table 2). On the other hand, when preparedness responsibilities were not considered as joint responsibilities but rather as individual responsibilities, more husbands than wives considered it their responsibility or their wives’ responsibility to prepare. This point is supported by the fact that 15 out of the 19 preparedness behaviors showed positive discrepancies in both the “husband responsible” category and the “wife responsible” category. Moreover, the largest discrepancy was found to be in “preparing a charcoal, lighter, and a grill,” which 46% of the husbands, but only 36% of the wives, believed it was their responsibility to prepare (percentages given in Table 2). Specifically, it is noteworthy that more husbands thought it was their responsibility to engage in stereotypically masculine preparedness behaviors, such as “preparing an electric generator” or “acquiring a fire extinguisher.” At the same time, more husbands thought it was their wives’ responsibility to undertake traditionally feminine preparedness behaviors, including many of those related to survival items, like “preparing a can opener” or “preparing rainwear or other protective clothing.” Note that more husbands than wives also thought that it was their wives’ responsibility to establish an emergency contact outside of a family.

thumbnail
Table 3. Discrepancies in husbands’ views and wives’ views of perceived responsibility for preparing for hurricane hazards and chi-square test results between husbands’ views and wives’ views.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.t003

Spousal agreement (or disagreement) about responsibility for household hurricane preparedness behaviors

Table 4 shows the level of agreement (or disagreement) on household hurricane-preparedness responsibility between a husband and a wife in a given family. The results show that for each of the 19 preparedness behaviors, at least 56% of the husbands and wives agreed on who was primarily responsible. The level of agreement ranged from 56% (“preparing a roof anchor”) to 89% (“knowing the evacuation zone for your family”). In addition, for 16 out of the 19 preparedness behaviors, the majority of couples agreed that the husband and wife are equally responsible for the given behavior. For the remaining three preparedness behaviors–“preparing shutters for windows or stormproof windows” (37%), “preparing an electric generator” (38%), and “preparing a roof anchor” (31%)–the majority of husbands and wives agreed that the husbands are primarily responsible. There was no specific item for which both husbands and wives thought that the wives are primarily responsible. The highest percentage of agreement among those behaviors deemed “wife responsible” preparedness behaviors was found for “preparing a three-day supply of canned food” (15%). It is worth noting that the three “husband responsible” items about which couples agreed showed a high percentage of “no agreement.”

thumbnail
Table 4. Levels of agreement on perceived responsibility for preparedness behaviors and Kappa coefficients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.t004

Kappa coefficients for the items are listed in Table 3. Because of the nature about how Kappa coefficient is calculated, the situation when both husband and wife in a given couple chose “I don’t know” was treated as an agreement when calculating Kappa coefficients. The Kappa coefficients ranged from 0.413 (“fire extinguisher”) to 0.578 (“preparing a three-day supply of canned food”). Some of the items that had higher Kappa coefficients include: “preparing a three-day supply of canned food” (0.578); “having an emergency contact outside of the family” (0.564); “filling the car’s gas tank” (0.532); “purchasing flood and/or wind insurance” (0.528); “preparing charcoal, lighter, and a grill” (0.527); and “clearing your yard of potential airborne items” (0.503). On the other hand, some of the items that had lower Kappa coefficients include “having a fire extinguisher” (0.413); “preparing rainwear or other protective clothing” (0.414); “knowing how to turn off the utilities” (0.414); “preparing sleeping bags or extra bedding” (0.433); and “making a family evacuation plan” (0.436). Overall, the Kappa coefficients indicated moderate agreement between husbands and wives about who should be responsible for preparing specific items.

Couples’ level of agreement and actual preparedness behaviors

Nineteen (19) chi-square tests were conducted, one for each of the preparedness items, to examine the association between level of agreement about perceived preparedness responsibility and whether the preparedness item was actually secured or the preparedness behavior was actually undertaken. The results are presented in Table 5.

thumbnail
Table 5. Chi-Square results for association between level of agreement and actual behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.t005

Among the tests, 3 out of the 19 preparedness items, all of which were planning-related preparedness behaviors, showed statistical significance: “having an emergency contact outside of the family,” “purchasing flood and/or wind insurance,” and “knowing the evacuation zone for your family.” A cross-tabulation indicated that for all three of these preparedness items, higher percentages of agreement were related to higher percentages of actual preparedness behaviors. For example, among the couples who agreed on who should be responsible for establishing an emergency contact, 77% of them said that they actually had emergency contacts outside of their families. On the other hand, for those couples who did not agree, only 52% said that they had emergency contacts.

To verify the relationships, six (6) logistic regressions were performed, using the preparedness behaviors “having an emergency contact outside of the family,” “purchasing flood and/or wind insurance,” and “knowing the evacuation zone for your family” as dependent variables. In predicting each of the three aforementioned preparedness behaviors, two different regression analyses were performed. In Model 1, only the control variables were included as predictors. In Model 2, variable agreement was included in the analysis.

The results (Table 6) indicate that for each of the three analyses, after controlling for related variables, variable agreement remained a positive and significant predictor of the preparedness behaviors examined. More importantly, incorporating variable agreement statistically improved the model fit for all three analyses.

thumbnail
Table 6. Logistic regression results in predicting specific preparedness behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.t006

It is worth noting that the results in Table 5 indicate that agreement was associated with higher percentages of actual completion for 14 of the 19 preparedness behaviors. The five preparedness behaviors that showed counter-effects were “preparing a three-day supply of canned food,” “preparing a can opener,” “preparing sleeping bags or other protective clothing,” “clearing your yard of potential airborne items,” and “filling the car’s gas tank.

Discussion

The analysis suggests that husbands and wives have moderate levels of agreement on who is responsible for preparing for hurricanes, and husbands’ and wives’ views on who is responsible do not statistically differ from each other. In terms of “having an emergency contact outside of the family,” “purchasing flood and/or wind insurance,” and “knowing the evacuation zone for your family,” which are usually considered planning-related preparedness behaviors [55,56], the results show that higher levels of agreement are associated with higher levels of task and behavior completion.

Both husbands and wives in this study indicated that they felt a shared responsibility for most of the preparedness behaviors, except for those preparedness behaviors that can be characterized as stereotypically masculine. Since couples consider preparing for natural hazards a matter of shared responsibility, couples are very likely to discuss and negotiate with one another about preparedness decisions (as discussed in the household decision-making literatures such as Ashraf [57]). Attention should be paid to better understanding these dynamics. Past research on household behaviors related to natural hazards or natural disasters, such as preparedness or evacuation, has usually assumed that the person who fills out the survey sufficiently represents all of the members of his or her household [16]. More studies considering household natural hazard preparedness as a household or family process are thus needed [3]. An increased focus on understanding the influence of these intra-household dynamics on household behaviors related to natural hazards or natural disasters is also needed [12,23,24]. In this study, several of the planning-related preparedness behaviors, including evacuation preparedness behaviors, had very high percentages of “both equally responsible” answers, suggesting that the execution of these behaviors is especially dependent upon interactions and communications between spouses [24,58]. Stereotypically masculine preparedness behaviors had high percentages of “husband responsible” answers, which is logical due to the stereotypically masculine physical and mechanical dimensions of the behaviors.

The discrepancies between husbands’ views and wives’ views, reported in Table 3, are particularly noteworthy. While the statistical results suggest that husbands’ and wives’ views regarding preparedness behaviors did not differ from each other, the general trend seen in this study is that the wives surveyed tended to favor shared responsibility, while the husbands tended to favor individual responsibility, either theirs or their wives’. This egalitarian perspective might present a problem for preparedness behaviors. That is, the “both responsible” view might lead to situations in which one spouse believes that if he or she does not prepare, his or her partner will, or one spouse thinks that he or she cannot undertake any preparedness action without consulting with his or her partner first [24]. Both thoughts might make people act more passively when approaching preparedness behaviors than they otherwise would, thereby resulting in delayed or unaccomplished actions. Views on the need for interaction before making preparedness decisions are likely to be an especially important issue in emergency situations, during which the time to react is short [24]. The results of this study suggest that more wives than husbands may find themselves in such an emergency situation, but it is worth noting that fairly large percentages of husbands and wives indicated that they believe they share responsibility for preparedness behaviors. The individual view of responsibility suggests that preparedness behaviors should be undertaken independently. Note the tendency for husbands to favor individual responsibility for either themselves or their wives. Situations in which husbands (or wives) consider themselves to have designated preparedness behaviors show consistency, but inconsistency appears when husbands (or wives) consider their spouses to have designated preparedness behaviors. However, the general trend observed in this study is that husbands considered stereotypically masculine preparedness behaviors their responsibility and considered stereotypically feminine preparedness behaviors their wives’ responsibility. While the husbands showed inconsistency in their perceptions of who was responsible for stereotypically feminine preparedness behaviors, this might not be very important because most of the stereotypically feminine preparedness items, such as “preparing a three-day supply of canned food” or “preparing a can opener,” are not preparedness-specific items and are completed in most of the households. Thus, this inconsistency is not likely to result in a lack of preparation.

The association between level of agreement and actual behaviors was significant for certain planning-related preparedness behaviors: “having an emergency contact outside of the family,” “purchasing flood and/or wind insurance,” and “knowing the evacuation zone for the family.” These results again suggest that many planning-related preparedness behaviors are behaviors that are especially tied to interactions among household members, rather than to single individuals’ thinking. However, while the results could be understood as suggesting that agreement in planning-related behaviors leads to actual behaviors, the counterstatement, that the behaviors are completed because agreement is reached, could also be true. Future research is needed to examine the causal effect of agreement on actual preparedness behaviors, especially planning-related behaviors. Note that for 14 of the 19 preparedness behaviors considered in this study, agreement was associated with a higher completion rate; the remaining 5 behaviors that did not show this association were everyday, not preparedness-specific, items, such as “preparing a three-day supply of canned food” or “preparing a can opener.” Negotiations about these items are usually not necessary. In addition, although some studies have suggested that the presence of children, especially very young children, in households is related to lower levels of motivation to prepare, greater perceived difficulty in preparing, greater lack of time to prepare, and even lower preparedness levels than in childless households [59], the results of the logistic regressions shown in Table 6 do not indicate that the presence of infants, toddlers, or preschool-aged children is a statistically significant predictor of the aforementioned planning-related preparedness behaviors. Future studies might examine whether these results are related to the specific preparedness behaviors investigated by this study.

The consideration that spousal agreement may be an important factor affecting household natural hazard preparedness stems from academic fields such as family studies, sociology of the family, and family psychology. The results of this study confirm that couple-level variables are important in preparedness behaviors [3]. Few studies, if any, treat existing individual-level variables that are usually considered important drivers of preparedness behaviors, such self-efficacy, fear, risk perceptions, and experiences, at the couple level in order to understand their influences on household natural hazard preparedness behaviors. In our study, the couple-level hurricane risk perception and hurricane experience variables were not statistically significant predictors of the three preparedness behaviors examined. More studies are needed to establish these variables’ relationships in different contexts.

Practitioners and policymakers should note the different views that husbands and wives might have regarding preparing for natural hazards. High levels of agreement between husbands and wives on the perceived responsibility for preparing for natural hazards are likely to increase levels of household natural hazard preparedness, while disagreements are likely to impede the undertaking of preparedness behaviors (see also Paton [22]). Encouraging couples to discuss preparedness issues, especially planning-related behaviors, is likely to facilitate the completion of preparedness behaviors.

The limitations of using this dataset are discussed in Hung [12]. One of these limitations is the non-representative nature of the dataset. However, the fact that the sample is made up of wealthy, highly educated, and predominantly white elderly retirees means that this study is especially suitable for understanding preparedness behaviors for this demographic group, whether its members reside in Florida or in other Sun Belt states in the southern United States. Also, this study followed some literatures [6062] and used a dichotomous scale (yes/no) for preparedness behaviors, but the results of the analysis could be differ if a continuous scale (e.g., from never prepare to always prepare) was used for preparedness behaviors, in which other studies have been adopted [63]. In addition, two additional limitations have been discussed in the methods section because of the research design: (1) it is impossible to detect whether the questionnaires were actually filled out as instructed, and (2) answering individual questionnaires after the joint questionnaires may led more couples to select “equally responsible” category when measuring preparedness responsibility.

Future studies can focus on uncovering the factors affecting the agreement between husband and wife on perceived responsibility for preparing for natural hazards. In addition, household decision-making is often influenced by power dynamics or resources controlled by different household members. For instance, the main financial contributor to the family often has greater say in the household decision-making process [64]. Future studies would benefit from addressing how power dynamics influence household decision-making regarding natural hazard preparedness.

Conclusion

Many of the quantitative, survey-based studies on household natural hazard preparedness take an individual view of factors influencing preparedness behaviors, neglecting the role of couple-level variables in facilitating or impeding preparedness behavior. This study suggests that spousal agreement on perceived responsibility in preparing for natural hazards is a statistically significant predictor of household hurricane preparedness. Given the scarcity of studies that quantitatively examine how couple-level variables influence preparedness behaviors, further research is necessary.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Original and revised questionnaire language.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221217.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Stephanie M. Scott for English editing.

References

  1. 1. Shah Alam Khan M. Disaster preparedness for sustainable development in Bangladesh. Disaster Prev and Management. 2008;17: 662–671.
  2. 2. Mileti DS, Gailus JL. Sustainable development and hazard mitigation in the United States: Disasters by design revisited. Mitig Adapt Strat Glob Change. 2005;10: 491–504.
  3. 3. Families Kirschenbaum A. and disaster behavior: a reassessment of family preparedness. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters. 2006;24: 111–143.
  4. 4. Lindell MK. Disaster studies. Current Sociology. 2013;61: 797–825.
  5. 5. Howe PD. Hurricane preparedness as anticipatory adaptation: a case study of community businesses. Global Environmental Change. 2011;21: 711–720.
  6. 6. Allen KM. Community-based disaster preparedness and climate adaptation: local capacity-building in the Philippines. Disasters. 2006;30: 81–101. pmid:16512863
  7. 7. Becker JS, Paton D, Johnston DM, Ronan KR. A model of household preparedness for earthquakes: how individuals make meaning of earthquake information and how this influences preparedness. Nat Hazards. 2012;64: 107–137.
  8. 8. Tam G, Huang Z, Chan EYY. Household Preparedness and Preferred Communication Channels in Public Health Emergencies: A Cross-Sectional Survey of Residents in an Asian Developed Urban City. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018;15: 1598. pmid:30060535
  9. 9. Paton D, Kelly G, Burgelt P, Doherty M. Preparing for bushfires: understanding intentions. Disaster Prev and Management. 2006;15: 566–575.
  10. 10. Levac J, Toal-Sullivan D, O’Sullivan TL. Household Emergency Preparedness: A Literature Review. J Community Health. 2012;37: 725–733. pmid:21984406
  11. 11. Kapucu N. Disaster Resilience and Adaptive Capacity in Central Florida, US, and in Eastern Marmara Region, Turkey. Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis: Research and Practice. 2012;14: 202–216.
  12. 12. Hung L-S. Married couples’ decision-making about household natural hazard preparedness: a case study of hurricane hazards in Sarasota County, Florida. Nat Hazards. 2017;87: 1057–1081.
  13. 13. Kohn S, Eaton JL, Feroz S, Bainbridge AA, Hoolachan J, Barnett DJ. Personal disaster preparedness: an integrative review of the literature. Disaster medicine and public health preparedness. 2012;6: 217–231. pmid:23077264
  14. 14. Duval TS, Mulilis J-P. A Person-Relative-to-Event (PrE) approach to negative threat appeals and earthquake preparedness: A field study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1999;29: 495–516.
  15. 15. Paton D. Disaster preparedness: a social-cognitive perspective. Disaster Prevention and Management. 2003;12: 210–216.
  16. 16. Grothmann T, Reusswig F. People at risk of flooding: why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Natural hazards. 2006;38: 101–120.
  17. 17. Mishra S, Mazumdar S, Suar D. Place attachment and flood preparedness. J Environ Psychol. 2010;30: 187–197.
  18. 18. Terpstra T. Emotions, Trust, and Perceived Risk: Affective and Cognitive Routes to Flood Preparedness Behavior. Risk Analysis. 2011;31: 1658–1675. pmid:21477090
  19. 19. Lindell MK, Perry RW. Communicating Environmental Risk in Multiethnic Communities. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2004.
  20. 20. Baker EJ. Household preparedness for the aftermath of hurricanes in Florida. Applied Geography. 2011;31: 46–52.
  21. 21. Wachinger G, Renn O, Begg C, Kuhlicke C. The risk perception paradox—implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Analysis. 2013;33: 1049–1065. pmid:23278120
  22. 22. Paton D. Risk, resilience, and readiness: Developing an all-hazards perspective. In: Collins AE, Samantha J, Manyena B, Jayawickrama J, editors. Hazards, Risks, and Disasters in Society. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2015. pp. 307–323. Available: https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=GlxzAwAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA307&dq=risk+resilience+and+readiness+paton&ots=pM9XDlQmRu&sig=BKRl1pDqVgiGpHO7z-ONNc_1MnM
  23. 23. Hung L-S. Gender, intra-household dynamics, and household hurricane preparedness: An exploratory study employing a dyadic interview approach. Int J Disaster Risk Sci. 2018;9: 16–27.
  24. 24. Proudley M. Fire, Families and Decisions. Australian Journal of Emergency Management. 2008;23: 37–43.
  25. 25. Goodman H, Cottrell A. Responding to a fire threat, gender roles, dependency and responsibility. In: Paton D, Tedim F, editors. Wildfire and Community: facilitating preparedness and resilience. Charles C Thomas Publisher: Springfield, Illinois, USA; 2012. pp. 281–299. Available: http://www.ccthomas.com/details.cfm?P_ISBN13=9780398088422
  26. 26. Brañas-Garza P, Capraro V, Rascon-Ramirez E. Gender differences in altruism on Mechanical Turk: Expectations and actual behaviour. Economics Letters. 2018;170: 19–23.
  27. 27. Rand DG, Brescoll VL, Everett JA, Capraro V, Barcelo H. Social heuristics and social roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2016;145: 389.
  28. 28. Rand DG. Social dilemma cooperation (unlike Dictator Game giving) is intuitive for men as well as women. Journal of experimental social psychology. 2017;73: 164–168. pmid:29686434
  29. 29. Booth A, Welch S. Spousal Consensus and Its Correlates: A Reassessment. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1978;40: 23–32.
  30. 30. Bronfenbrenner U. The ecology of human development: Experiments by design and nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1979.
  31. 31. Hammer LB, Bauer TN, Grandey AA. Work-Family Conflict and Work-Related Withdrawal Behaviors. Journal of Business and Psychology. 2003;17: 419–436.
  32. 32. Szinovacz ME, Egley LC. Comparing One-Partner and Couple Data on Sensitive Marital Behaviors: The Case of Marital Violence. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1995;57: 995–1010.
  33. 33. Crohan SE. Marital Happiness and Spousal Consensus on Beliefs about Marital Conflict: A Longitudinal Investigation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 1992;9: 89–102.
  34. 34. Craddock AE. Relationships between attitudinal similarity, couple structure, and couple satisfaction within married and de facto couples. Australian Journal of Psychology. 1991;43: 11–16.
  35. 35. Williams L, Sobieszczyk T. Couple Attitudes and Agreement Regarding Pregnancy Wantedness in the Philippines. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65: 1019–1029.
  36. 36. Kulczycki A. Husband-Wife Agreement, Power Relations And Contraceptive Use in Turkey. International Family Planning Perspectives. 2008;34: 127–137. pmid:18957355
  37. 37. Monroe PA, Bokemeier JL, Kotchen JM, McKean H. Spousal Response Consistency in Decision-Making Research. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1985;47: 733–738.
  38. 38. Kamo Y. “He Said, She Said”: Assessing Discrepancies in Husbands’ and Wives’ Reports on the Division of Household Labor. Social Science Research. 2000;29: 459–476.
  39. 39. Cremeans-Smith JK, Stephens MAP, Franks MM, Martire LM, Druley JA, Wojno WC. Spouses’ and physicians’ perceptions of pain severity in older women with osteoarthritis: dyadic agreement and patients’ well-being. Pain. 2003;106: 27–34. pmid:14581107
  40. 40. Peacock WG, Gladwin H, Morrow BH. Hurricane Andrew: Ethnicity, gender and the sociology of disasters. Routledge; 2012.
  41. 41. Enarson E, Fothergill A, Peek L. Gender and disaster: Foundations and directions. In: Rodriguez H, Guarantelli G, Dynes R, editors. Handbook of disaster research. New York: Springer; 2007. pp. 130–146.
  42. 42. Fothergill A. Gender, risk, and disaster. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters. 1996;14: 33–56.
  43. 43. Bateman J, Edwards B. Gender and Evacuation: A Closer Look at Why Women Are More Likely to Evacuate for Hurricanes. Natural Hazards Review. 2002;3: 107–117.
  44. 44. Wang C, Yarnal B. The vulnerability of the elderly to hurricane hazards in Sarasota, Florida. Nat Hazards. 2012;63: 349–373.
  45. 45. Hung L-S. Gender, household decision-making, and hurricane preparedness [Internet]. The Pennsylvania State University; 2016. Available: https://etda.libraries.psu.edu/catalog/h702q636h
  46. 46. Cherniack EP, Sandals L, Brooks L, Mintzer M. Trial of a Survey Instrument to Establish the Hurricane Preparedness of and Medical Impact on a Vulnerable Older Population. Prehospital and Disaster Medicine. 2008;23: 242–249. pmid:18702270
  47. 47. Tampa Bay Regional Planning Council. Disaster planning guide [Brochure]. Pinellas Park, FL: Tampa Bay Regional Planning Council; 2013.
  48. 48. Davis HL. Dimensions of Marital Roles in Consumer Decision Making. Journal of Marketing Research. 1970;7: 168–177.
  49. 49. Rojas-de Gracia MM, Alarcón-Urbistondo P. Toward a gender understanding of the influence of the couple on family vacation decisions. Tourism Management Perspectives. 2016;20: 290–298.
  50. 50. Lackman C, Lanasa JM. Family decision-making theory: An overview and assessment. Psychology & Marketing. 1993;10: 81–93.
  51. 51. Lindell MK, Prater CS. Household adoption of seismic hazard adjustments: A comparison of residents in two states. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters. 2000;18: 317–338.
  52. 52. Lindell MK, Perry RW. The protective action decision model: theoretical modifications and additional evidence. Risk Analysis. 2012;32: 616–632. pmid:21689129
  53. 53. Thompson L, Walker AJ. Gender in Families: Women and Men in Marriage, Work, and Parenthood. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1989;51: 845–871.
  54. 54. Lyons KS, Sayer AG. Using multilevel modeling in caregiving research. Aging & Mental Health. 2005;9: 189–195. pmid:16019272
  55. 55. Russell LA, Goltz JD, Bourque LB. Preparedness and hazard mitigation actions before and after two earthquakes. Environment and Behavior. 1995;27: 744–770.
  56. 56. Prior T, Eriksen C. What does being ‘well prepared’for wildfire mean. In: Paton D, Tedim F, editors. Wildfire and Community: Facilitating Preparedness and Resilience. Springfield, Illinois, USA: Charles C Thomas Publisher; 2012. pp. 190–206.
  57. 57. Ashraf N. Spousal Control and Intra-Household Decision Making: An Experimental Study in the Philippines. American Economic Review. 2009;99: 1245–1277.
  58. 58. Sharan M, Valente TW. Spousal Communication and Family Planning Adoption: Effects of a Radio Drama Serial in Nepal. International Family Planning Perspectives. 2002;28: 16–25.
  59. 59. McNeill IM, Ronan KR. Children in disasters: the role of household preparedness. Nat Hazards. 2017;89: 1239–1254.
  60. 60. Lindell MK, Hwang SN. Households’ Perceived Personal Risk and Responses in a Multihazard Environment. Risk Analysis. 2008;28: 539–556. pmid:18419668
  61. 61. Lindell MK, Arlikatti S, Huang S-K. Immediate behavioral response to the June 17, 2013 flash floods in Uttarakhand, North India. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 2019;34: 129–146.
  62. 62. Kerstholt J, Duijnhoven H, Paton D. Flooding in The Netherlands: How people’s interpretation of personal, social and institutional resources influence flooding preparedness. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 2017;24: 52–57.
  63. 63. Norris FH. Frequency and structure of precautionary behavior in the domains of hazard preparedness, crime prevention, vehicular safety, and health maintenance. Health Psychology. 1997;16: 566. pmid:9387002
  64. 64. Davis HL. Decision making within the household. Journal of Consumer Research. 1976;2: 241–260.