Figures
Abstract
The extraversion personality trait has a positive correlation with social interaction. In neuroimaging studies, investigations on extraversion in face-to-face verbal interactions are still scarce. This study presents an electroencephalography (EEG)-based investigation of the extraversion personality trait in relation to eye contact during face-to-face interactions, as this is a vital signal in social interactions. A sample of healthy male participants were selected (consisting of sixteen more extraverted and sixteen less extraverted individuals) and evaluated with the Eysenck's Personality Inventory (EPI) and Big Five Inventory (BFI) tools. EEG alpha oscillations in the occipital region were measured to investigate extraversion personality trait correlates of eye contact during a face-to-face interaction task and an eyes-open condition. The results revealed that the extraversion personality trait has a significant positive correlation with EEG alpha coherence in the occipital region, presumably due to its relationship with eye contact during the interaction task. Furthermore, the decrease in EEG alpha power during the interaction task compared to the eyes-open condition was found to be greater in the less extraverted participants; however, no significant difference was observed between the less and more extraverted participants. Overall, these findings encourage further research towards the understanding of neural mechanism correlates of the extraversion personality trait—particularly in social interaction.
Citation: Roslan NS, Izhar LI, Faye I, Amin HU, Mohamad Saad MN, Sivapalan S, et al. (2019) Neural correlates of eye contact in face-to-face verbal interaction: An EEG-based study of the extraversion personality trait. PLoS ONE 14(7): e0219839. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219839
Editor: Christine E. King, University of California Irvine, UNITED STATES
Received: January 11, 2019; Accepted: July 2, 2019; Published: July 25, 2019
Copyright: © 2019 Roslan et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: This research work has been supported by Ministry of Higher Education Malaysia under Higher Institution Centre of Excellence (HICoE) Scheme (Ref No. 015MA0-001) to CISIR and Fundamental Research Grant Scheme (FRGS; Ref No. FRGS/1/2016/TK04/UTP/03/3) to Lila Iznita Izhar. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
In the Big Five theory, the extraversion personality trait is one of the five major traits alleged to form human personalities [1, 2]. People who are high in extraversion can be characterised as talkative, gregarious, energetic, and assertive and vice versa for those with low extraversion. This personality trait is believed to influence an individual's preferences and performance in various areas, such as work and education [3–5]. For instance, more extraverted persons are the most likely to seek professions with a higher level of task significance, power, and feedback [3]. They love to be assigned the responsibility to create transformation, as well as handle multiple functions [3]. This is believed to be due to their preference for feeling a sense of significance, and for evaluating their own performance in relation to that of others.
Furthermore, more extraverted persons prefer to engage with people and enjoy social interaction, while less extraverted persons tend to be quiet, reserved and less sociable [1, 6]. After social activities, more extraverted persons achieve significant increases in positive affectivity as compared to less extraverted individuals [7]. These differences influence more and less extraverted persons to behave differently when interacting with others, face-to-face. For instance, compared to more extraverted persons, less extraverted persons were found to have larger general inhibitions, which led them to hesitation in sharing their ideas in a group discussion [4]. Presumably, because of this general inhibition, less extraverted persons prefer to communicate and interact via the internet, while more extraverted persons prefer face-to-face social interaction [8, 9].
As we know, during a face-to-face social interaction, eye contact is an important social signal [10]. This can be described as two persons gazing at each other's eyes [11, 12]. These gazing activities could be a cue of one person's attention towards another [13–15]. For instance, three-month-old infants' smiling behaviour declined when an interacting person gazed away after having made eye contact [16]. In terms of the extraversion personality trait, [17] summarized several studies and found that more and less extraverted persons have differences in terms of gaze direction, duration, and frequency. They reported that the direction, duration, and frequency of gaze were found to be toward, long, and often, respectively, in more extraverted persons; and corresponded to avoidance, short, and seldom in less extraverted persons [17]. Supporting these findings was a study that claimed the extraversion personality trait is directly proportional to the attention an individual commits to the eyes of another person during social tasks [18]. By referring to these findings, we could say that more and less extraverted persons could be differentiated based on eyes contact during social events. Although many studies showed differentiation of extraversion personality trait in terms of eye contact, research involving electroencephalography (EEG) remains scarce—particularly involving the naturalistic paradigm of face-to-face verbal interaction [19, 20].
EEG is an effective neuroimaging modality to study individual differences in personality [21–23]. This is because of its direct measurement of neural signals and remarkable temporal resolution compared to other neuroimaging modalities, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) [24]. One of the well-known theories of extraversion was proposed by Eysenck [25], which stated that the extraversion personality trait has a negative correlation with cortical arousal. Based on this theory, some EEG studies discovered that more extraverted persons demonstrated lower levels of cortical arousal and less extraverted persons demonstrated higher levels of cortical arousal, as measured through EEG alpha oscillations [26, 27]. Additionally, several researchers investigated the extraversion personality trait and EEG alpha oscillations in posterior regions, such as the parietal and occipital regions [28–31]. Although these studies generally showed a positive association between the extraversion personality trait and EEG alpha oscillations in the posterior region [28, 30], one study was unable to find such a significant association [29]. Along with technological development in removing eye movement artefacts in the frontal region, [32] investigated positive and negative empathic moods with the extraversion personality trait in broad brain regions, including frontal (F3, F4), temporal (T3, T4), and occipital (O1, O2). They claimed that extraversion was related to larger alpha amplitude in widespread regions. Furthermore, [26] focused on the frontal region (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, F8, Fz) in an investigation of EEG alpha oscillations, while participants opened and closed their eyes on instruction. They suggested similar findings to those of [32] in that more extraverted persons exhibited a larger alpha amplitude compared to that of less extraverted persons. Investigations of EEG alpha oscillations and the extraversion personality trait are ongoing in various aspects, such as intelligence [33], memory performance [34, 35], creativity and originality [27], and emotional processing [36]. For analysis, several studies focused on the entire brain including the occipital region [27, 36], while others excluded the occipital region [34, 35]. Such differences in the brain regions of interest in these studies could partly be due to the different sets of objectives and experimental tasks considered.
Based on our review, although there have been many studies investigating EEG correlates of the extraversion personality trait, none have incorporated the aspect of eye contact during a face-to-face interaction. Hence, the aim of this study was to investigate EEG correlates of extraversion in relation to eye contact by measuring EEG alpha coherence and power in the occipital region during a face-to-face interaction task. EEG alpha oscillations were considered on the basis of Eysenck's well-known theory of extraversion [25], and for having been explored in many EEG studies related to extraversion personality trait [26, 27, 37].
Methods
Participants
Ninety-one healthy male students of Universiti Teknologi PETRONAS volunteered to participate in the experiment. The volunteers were exclusively male so as to avoid any possible gender effect on the EEG results [38, 39]. According to their self-report, they were right-handed, had normal or corrected to normal vision, had no hearing impairment, were not suffering from or having family history related to cognitive disorder, were not taking any drugs or any medication, nor experiencing chronic mental stress or adverse psychology states. All volunteers had to complete two personality tests—the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) and Big Five Inventory (BFI)—to determine their level of extraversion before they could participate in this study [40, 41]. The volunteers were characterised as less extraverted if they scored less than 50% for both EPI and BFI, and more extraverted if they scored more than 50% for both EPI and BFI. After a screening session, 50 volunteers were excluded due to conflicting results between their personality tests. Of the 41 remaining volunteers who met the requirement, only 32 returned to the study, consisting of 16 less and 16 more extraverted participants. These were between 18 and 23 years old (M = 19.53 and SD = 1.22). All participants gave informed consent and were paid for their participation in the EEG experiment for this study.
This research was approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of the University of Kuala Lumpur Royal College of Medicine Perak, Malaysia.
Experimental task: A face-to-face interaction task
The experiment conducted involved a form of a face-to-face interaction task, which was performed individually by each participant. During the task, the participant was instructed to sit facing an inquirer who was a stranger to them (see Fig 1). This face-to-face interaction task with a stranger was selected for its potential to induce stress and anxiety related to social awkwardness, which could be one of the challenges for less extraverted participants. In the task, four questions were asked by the inquirer to the participants, one at a time. The participants were instructed to answer each question spontaneously, within two minutes. The instructor stopped the session when the time had reached two minutes. See Fig 2 for a detailed timeline of the interaction task. All questions remained unknown to the participants until the inquirer asked them.
(The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent—as outlined in the PLOS consent form—to publish these case details.)
Procedure
Prior to the beginning of the experiment, participants were provided with information about the experiment and their informed consents were obtained. The enrolled participants were then seated in a partially sound-attenuated EEG experiment room to perform the experiment individually. Subsequently, the EEG device was set up, which included assisting participants in donning an EEG cap. EEG recordings began with a 5-minute eyes-closed period, followed by a 5-minute eyes-open period. During the eyes-open period, participants were advised to focus on one point and avoid eye movement. Next, a practice session was held in which they needed to answer one question within two minutes. The purpose of this practice session was to ensure participants provided maximal EEG data quality during the actual experimental task. During the practice session, participants were guided on how to minimise EEG artefacts, such as placing their hands on their laps to avoid hand movements and thereby improve the quality of the EEG data. Such artefacts are due to the fact that EEG devices are sensitive and easily interrupted by other unwanted electrical activities. After the practice session, participants needed to introduce themselves. They were then asked to perform the face-to-face interaction task. For each question in the interaction task, the EEG recording started when the inquirer began to ask the question and stopped when the time to answer reached two minutes (see Fig 2). Following the interaction task, participants were asked to fill a self-assessment survey related to the task (see Table 1). At the end of their session, each participant was instructed to keep all questions asked during the interaction task strictly confidential. This was to avoid other participants from preparing their answers before the experiment.
(The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent—as outlined in the PLOS consent form—to publish these case details.)
EEG acquisition
The EEG data was recorded using a 32-channels eegoTM sports system (ANT Neuro, Netherlands) with Ag/AgCl scalp electrodes arranged based on the International 10–10 System. The CPz electrode was selected as a reference electrode. The impedance of each electrode was kept below 10 kΩ. The EEG data were digitised at a sampling rate of 512 Hz.
Data analysis
EEG preprocessing.
The recorded EEG data were pre-processed using BESA Research 6.0 (www.besa.de). First, the raw EEG data were bandpass filtered with a cut-off frequency of 0.53–48 Hz. Eye artefacts (movements and blinks) and muscle movements were corrected using the Berg & Scherg method [42] implemented in the BESA software as used by [43]. The cleaned EEG data were then exported to MATLAB for EEG features extraction.
EEG features.
EEG alpha coherence: This is one of the methods used to measure brain functional connectivity by measuring signals from two electrodes, or two brain regions. In mathematics, coherence can be defined as the ratio of normalised cross-power spectrum to auto-power spectrum [44]. Coherence ranges from 0 to 1, where 0 means the corresponding two signals are not functionally coherent and 1 means the two signals are fully functionally coherent with a constant phase difference, suggesting that the two electrodes (brain areas) are working together. EEG coherence can be calculated using Eq (1) [44]: (1) where f is the frequency, Pxy is the cross-power spectrum between the signals of two electrodes x and y, Pxx is the auto-power spectrum for the signals of electrode x, and Pyy is the auto-power spectrum for the signals of electrode y.
For EEG coherence in this study, only 19 of the 32 electrodes were selected based on the 10–20 placement system. The number of pairs was calculated using pairs , where n = 19 resulted in 171 pairs. The EEG coherence for all 171 pairs was calculated for the four oscillations (alpha, delta, theta, and beta) using Eq (1). The EEG alpha coherence for the paired occipital electrodes (O1 and O2) were extracted and averaged within the participants and within the questions to investigate extraversion personality trait correlates of eye contact during the interaction task.
EEG alpha power: In this study, EEG alpha power was measured in the eyes-open condition to serve as a baseline, and during an interaction task. A fast Fourier transform (FFT) with a Hanning window was used on 512 samples with a 50% overlap between successive 2-second segments (1024 points) for estimating the power of the EEG signals. The EEG alpha power during the interaction task and in the eyes-open condition in the occipital region (electrodes O1 and O2) was extracted and averaged at a standard frequency of 8–13 Hz. Subsequently, the EEG alpha power data were averaged within the participants and within the interaction task questions. The mean EEG alpha power for the interaction task was compared with that of the eyes-open baseline. Increase or decrease in the EEG features (e.g. EEG alpha power) during the experimental task in comparison with the baseline were computed for each participant by [45]: (2) Where AEB is the increase/decrease in the EEG alpha power (positive/negative values) during the experimental task as compared to that of the eyes-open baseline, SE is the EEG alpha power during the experimental task, and SB is the EEG alpha power in the eyes-open condition.
Statistical analysis.
As mentioned, two personality tests (i.e. the EPI and BFI) were used for personality assessment. The extraversion scores of all participants were computed by converting the EPI and BFI scores to percentages and averaging them. Pearson correlation was used to determine the correlation between the averaged extraversion score (E-score) and both personality tests. The E-score was used throughout the analysis.
Subsequently, the mean ± standard deviation of the EEG features and self-assessment survey items of two possible groups—more and less extraverted—were computed. The significant results of these two possible groups were measured using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Additionally, ANOVA was used to identify the optimal EEG features and self-assessment survey items of the extraversion personality trait. These optimal features and items were selected based on the highest F-value of the ANOVA.
Finally, the main correlation of extraversion personality with the optimal EEG feature and self-assessment survey item were explored using the linear regression method. Two linear regression scatterplots were analysed to demonstrate the possible correlation. For all statistical analyses, the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) software platform was used, in which p-values smaller than 0.05 indicate statistically significant data. The data were represented in mean ± standard deviation, unless otherwise specified.
Results
In this section, we discuss the results of all tested data in this study. First, we discuss the results of the two personality tests, including the proposed E-score. Second, we discuss the results of the EEG features—EEG alpha coherence and EEG alpha power—in relation to the extraversion personality trait. Third, we discuss the results of the self-assessment survey related to the face-to-face interaction task. The statistical tests mentioned in the ‘Statistical analysis’ section were used to determine the significance of all data representing extraversion.
Personality tests
In this study, two personality tests—the EPI and BFI—were used to evaluate the level of extraversion in each participant. The EPI and BFI are self-report questionnaires where the EPI contained 57 “YES” or “NO” items and the BFI contained 44 5-point scale items (1 = disagree strongly…5 = agree strongly). Of the 57 items in the EPI, 24 were used to assess the extraversion personality trait, while for BFI, 8 out of 44 items were used for the same. As stated in the ‘Statistical analysis’ section, the averaged E-score of each participant was computed by averaging the scores of their EPI and BFI after converting them to percentages. More extraverted participants scored more than 50%, while less extraverted participants scored less than 50%. We proposed to employ the E-score as there was consistency between the extraversion facet scales of the EPI and BFI [46]. Moreover, the Pearson correlation revealed that the EPI was significantly correlated with the BFI (r = 0.896, p < 0.001). The mean E-scores for the more and less extraverted participants were found to be 68.18 ± 8.80 and 35.22 ± 7.60, respectively. For validation of the proposed E-score, the Pearson correlation showed that it was significantly correlated with both the EPI (r = 0.982, p < 0.001) and BFI (r = 0.964, p < 0.001). These results indicated that the E-score could be used to represent each participant's level of extraversion in the statistical analysis [47].
EEG features
Possible group differences of the extraversion personality trait—with respect to two EEG features (i.e. EEG alpha coherence and EEG alpha power) in the occipital region during the face-to-face interaction task and in the eyes-open condition—were analysed. The mean EEG alpha coherence in the occipital region in the eyes-open condition for both groups were similar (more extraverted = 0.66 ± 0.15; less extraverted = 0.65 ± 0.09), as illustrated in Fig 3A. For the mean EEG alpha power in the eyes-open condition, the less extraverted participants exhibited higher levels (7.80 ± 6.91) than the more extraverted participants (5.93 ± 5.51), as illustrated in Fig 3B. However, as can be seen in Table 1, the differences between the group means of both EEG features in the eyes-open condition were not statistically significant (p > 0.05). Thus, for these cases, null and alternative hypotheses cannot be rejected and accepted, respectively, for the EEG features in the eyes-open condition.
(a) Mean EEG alpha coherence and (b) mean EEG alpha power in the occipital region in the eyes-open condition and face-to-face interaction task.
For the face-to-face interaction task, the mean EEG alpha coherence of the more extraverted participants (0.68 ± 0.08) was higher than that of the less extraverted participants (0.60 ± 0.08), as illustrated in Fig 3A. Additionally, the difference between the group means of EEG alpha coherence was statistically significant (see p-value in Table 1). Thus, null and alternative hypotheses could be rejected and accepted, respectively, for EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task. For the mean EEG alpha power during the interaction task, the less extraverted participants exhibited higher EEG alpha power levels (4.98 ± 3.50) than the more extraverted participants (4.25 ± 4.45), as illustrated in Fig 3B. However, the difference between the group means of EEG alpha power during the interaction task was not statistically significant (p > 0.05; see Table 1).
Additionally, the differences between the EEG features during the interaction task and eyes-open condition were calculated. The results revealed that the mean EEG alpha coherence of the more extraverted participants was increased (0.10 ± 0.40) during the interaction task as compared to that of the eyes-open condition. In comparison, the mean EEG alpha coherence of the less extraverted participants (-0.07 ± 0.11) was decreased during the interaction task compared to the eyes-open condition. Although there was dissimilarity in the mean EEG alpha coherence between the more and less extraverted participants, there was no statistically significant difference between them: F(1,30) = 2.678, p > 0.05. The same lack of significance was found for differences between the mean EEG alpha power of the more (-0.09 ± 1.05) and less extraverted (-0.16 ± 0.56) participants: F(1,30) = 0.052, p > 0.05.
Additionally, ANOVA was then used to identify the optimal EEG features. Based on the results of the ANOVA for each of the EEG features (see Table 1), EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task demonstrated the highest F-value (F = 8.934). After obtaining the optimal EEG feature, a linear regression was employed to determine the influence of the extraversion personality trait on EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task. To evaluate the linearity, a scatterplot was made of EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task against the extraversion personality trait (E-score), and a superimposed regression line was plotted. As illustrated in Fig 4, the linear regression established that the extraversion personality trait could be used to predict the EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task with statistical significance: F(1,30) = 9.946, p < 0.05, with R2 = 0.249; a large effect size according to Cohen [48]. The regression equation was: predicted EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task = 0.52 + 0.002x (E-score).
Self-assessment survey
As stated in the ‘Procedure’ section, after performing the face-to-face interaction task, each participant needed to fill in a self-assessment survey related to the task. All items in the self-assessment were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). This self-assessment had an acceptable internal consistency, as determined by a Cronbach's alpha of 0.76 [49]. The results of the survey showed that the more extraverted participants reported that they felt more comfortable sitting in front of a stranger and sharing ideas, were more spontaneous in voicing those ideas, and had a greater preference for giving feedback verbally compared to the less extraverted participants (see Table 2). The difference between the group means for all items in the self-assessment survey were found to be statistically significant (p < 0.05), as presented in Table 1.
As mentioned in the ‘Statistical analysis’ section, ANOVA was used to identify the optimal self-assessment survey item by referring to the highest F-value of all items. Based on the ANOVA results in Table 1, item (a) demonstrated the highest F-value (F = 20.589). After obtaining the optimal self-assessment survey item, a linear regression was employed to determine the correlation between the extraversion personality trait and the comfortableness of the participants sitting in front of the strangers (item (a)). To evaluate the linearity, a scatterplot was made of item (a) against the extraversion personality trait (E-score), and a superimposed regression line was plotted. As illustrated in Fig 5, the linear regression established that the extraversion personality trait could be used to predict the item (a) with statistical significance: F(1,30) = 42.044, p < 0.0001, with R2 = 0.584; a large effect size according to Cohen [48]. The regression equation was: predicted item (a) = 1.40 + 0.04x (E-score).
Linear regression relation between extraversion personality trait (E-score) and item (a): comfortableness while sitting in front of a stranger; (F(1,30) = 42.044, p < 0.0001).
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate correlates between neural signals in a specific brain region and the extraversion personality trait. Specifically, the investigation focused on an EEG-based study of extraversion correlates with eye contact during a face-to-face interaction task with a stranger. We mainly focused on eye contact as it is a crucial signal during social interactions. We also analysed the consistency of the EEG features with a self-assessment survey of the interaction task. The findings of this study regarding the EEG features and self-assessment survey are believed to have provided a reliable outcome in the investigation of the extraversion personality trait during the face-to-face interaction task.
The present study found that the mean EEG alpha coherence (i.e. the connectivity between brain regions) during the face-to-face interaction task was the optimal EEG feature for extraversion. This result suggests that the mean EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task could expose extraversion differences better than EEG features in the eyes-open condition (baseline/resting state). Our findings support that the EEG resting state cannot be used to recognise the extraversion personality trait [50]. During the interaction task, the mean EEG alpha coherence in the occipital region of the more extraverted participants was significantly higher than that of the less extraverted participants. The EEG alpha coherence results indicate a stronger connectivity between the left (O1) and right (O2) occipital regions in the more extraverted participants during the interaction task. As the occipital region involves visual processing [51], we deduced that this high connectivity in the occipital region occurred in the more extraverted participants as a result of their consistent eye contact during the interaction task. This presumption is in line with studies that described the extraversion personality trait as being directly proportional to the attention or eye contact one commits to the eyes of another [18, 52]. In further support of this, a linear regression in this study demonstrated a significant positive correlation between the extraversion personality trait and mean EEG alpha coherence in the occipital region during the interaction task (see Fig 4). Therefore, we believe the more extraverted participants had more consistent eye contact with others than the less extraverted participants.
Moreover, the interpretation of the EEG alpha coherence is also supported by behavioural data. The linear regression demonstrated a significant positive correlation between the extraversion personality trait and the comfort of the participants sitting in front of a stranger (see Fig 5). This comfortableness is believed to enable the more extraverted participants to maintain better eye contact than the less extraverted participants [53, 54]. Due to this comfortableness, the mean EEG alpha coherence of more extraverted participants was increased during the interaction task. Besides an increased level of comfort, the more extraverted participants were also found to be more comfortable and spontaneous in sharing their ideas than the less extraverted participants. They also reported a preference for giving feedback verbally. All results from the self-assessment survey were predictable, as it is well known that more extraverted individuals are more outgoing and sociable, while less extraverted individuals are the opposite [1, 6, 55, 56]. These results provided further evidence that more extraverted individuals are easily adapted to response as compared to less extraverted individuals [57, 58]. Furthermore, the results support the notion that more extraverted individuals build up their characteristic of low arousal from response organisation, while less extraverted individuals' characteristic of high arousal means they tend to derive inhibition from response organisation and prefer stimulus analysis [58]. In this case, the interaction task was believed to cause the more extraverted participants to be more comfortable than the less extraverted participants.
Besides mean EEG alpha coherence, we also observed a decrease in mean EEG alpha power between the more extraverted and less extraverted participants during the interaction task compared to the baseline. As illustrated in Fig 3A, the decrease in EEG alpha power during the interaction task compared to the eyes-open condition was greater in the less extraverted participants than the more extraverted participants. Based on our review, there is inconsistency in the reasoning for this decrease in EEG alpha oscillations. Some studies claim that a decrease in EEG alpha power in the occipital region is due to the attention of an individual on the location of a visual target [51, 59]. Another study claims that the decrease is due to a wandering mind [60]. Similarly, we were unable to reach a solid conclusion regarding the relationship between the extraversion personality trait and EEG alpha power during the interaction task. This likely contributed to difficulty in achieving statistically significant results for EEG alpha power correlates of the extraversion personality trait during the face-to-face interaction. In comparison, the EEG alpha coherence provided more substantial evidence representing the extraversion personality trait. Presumably, this is because coherence is measured by considering the signals from two electrodes, while power was measured from a single electrode [44]. Additional research remains necessary to clarify the relationship between EEG alpha power in the occipital region and the extraversion personality trait during the interaction task.
There are certain limitations to this study which deserve further investigation. First, the sample was limited to right-handed male students aged 18–23 years old. Therefore, the assumptions in this study could not be used to generalise about female, non-students, left-handed or middle-aged adult groups. Second, this study was limited to two groups—i.e. more and less extraverted. As such, multiple comparisons could not be made. Third, this study employed a cross-sectional design. Longitudinal studies are required for further insight into neural mechanism correlates of the extraversion personality trait during the interaction task. For instance, less extraverted participants could repeat the experiment after a few months to determine whether their neural signals have changed. Such longitudinal neuroimaging studies are needed to objectively investigate personality changes throughout the lifespan to support subjective study which claimed personality could be changed—particularly in adolescence and old age [61]. Fourth, this study only mentioned the general relation between the extraversion personality trait and eye contact using EEG signals. Further analysis—mainly on the degree of eye contact and extraversion—should be added and thorough measurements should be conducted. Fifth, the self-assessment survey in this study was limited to only four items. We believe adding more items to the survey could assist us in justifying the EEG results. In general, this study was limited to only an EEG investigation and self-assessment survey of the interaction task. Other physiological measurements, such as functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), electrocardiography (ECG) or electrodermal response, could assist us in gaining further insight into the extraversion personality trait during the face-to-face interaction task.
In conclusion, this study revealed that the extraversion personality trait was positively correlated with EEG alpha coherence in the occipital region representing neural correlates of eye contact during a face-to-face verbal interaction. The resulting EEG alpha coherence during the interaction task was found to be in agreement with the self-assessment survey collected in this study. This emphasises that an EEG-based investigation related to social interaction can reveal the extraversion personality trait, thus providing an alternative to EEG resting state investigations. Contradictory to those of the EEG alpha coherence, the EEG alpha power findings in this study showed that further investigation is required to clarify inconsistencies in the literature. Overall, the results obtained in this study have laid the foundation upon which to develop our understanding of neural mechanism correlates of the extraversion personality trait in face-to-face interactions or social activities.
Acknowledgments
This research work was supported by the Ministry of Higher Education, Malaysia, under the Higher Institution Centre of Excellence (HICoE) Scheme (Ref No. 015MA0-001) and an FRGS Grant (Ref No. FRGS/1/2016/TK04/UTP/03/3). The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. For data collection, we thank Muhamad Hafiz Abd Latif, Rodney Petrus Balandong, and Azimah Ajam for their assistance.
References
- 1. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five‐factor model and its applications. Journal of personality. 1992;60(2):175–215.
- 2. Oshio A, Taku K, Hirano M, Saeed G. Resilience and Big Five personality traits: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;127:54–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.048
- 3. Barrick MR, Mount MK, Li N. The theory of purposeful work behavior: The role of personality, higher-order goals, and job characteristics. Academy of management review. 2013;38(1):132–53.
- 4. Nussbaum EM. How introverts versus extroverts approach small-group argumentative discussions. The Elementary School Journal. 2002:183–97.
- 5. Davidson B, Gillies RA, Pelletier AL. Introversion and Medical Student Education: Challenges for Both Students and Educators. Teaching and Learning in Medicine. 2015;27(1):99–104. pmid:25584478
- 6. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: happy and unhappy people. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1980;38(4):668. pmid:7381680
- 7. Duffy KA, Helzer EG, Hoyle RH, Helzer JF, Chartrand TL. Pessimistic expectations and poorer experiences: The role of (low) extraversion in anticipated and experienced enjoyment of social interaction. PloS one. 2018;13(7):e0199146. pmid:29975736
- 8. Amichai-Hamburger Y, Wainapel G, Fox S. " On the Internet no one knows I'm an introvert": Extroversion, neuroticism, and Internet interaction. Cyberpsychology & behavior. 2002;5(2):125–8.
- 9. Ebeling-Witte S, Frank ML, Lester D. Shyness, Internet use, and personality. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2007;10(5):713–6.
- 10. Hietanen JK. Affective Eye Contact: An Integrative Review. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1587-. pmid:30210410.
- 11.
Harper RG, Wiens AN, Matarazzo JD. Nonverbal communication: The state of the art1978.
- 12. Kleinke CL. Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin. 1986;100(1):78–100. pmid:3526377
- 13. George N, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Seen Gaze-Direction Modulates Fusiform Activity and Its Coupling with Other Brain Areas during Face Processing. NeuroImage. 2001;13(6):1102–12. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0769.
- 14. Hietanen JK. Does your gaze direction and head orientation shift my visual attention? Neuroreport. 1999;10(16):3443–7. pmid:10599859
- 15. Hietanen JK, Leppänen JM, Peltola MJ, Linna-aho K, Ruuhiala HJ. Seeing direct and averted gaze activates the approach–avoidance motivational brain systems. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(9):2423–30. pmid:18402988
- 16. Hains SM, Muir DW. Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. Child development. 1996;67(5):1940–51. pmid:9022223
- 17.
Ruhland K, Zibrek K, McDonnell R, editors. Perception of personality through eye gaze of realistic and cartoon models. Proceedings of the ACM SIGGRAPH Symposium on Applied Perception; 2015: ACM.
- 18. Wu DWL, Bischof WF, Anderson NC, Jakobsen T, Kingstone A. The influence of personality on social attention. Personality and Individual Differences. 2014;60:25–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.017.
- 19. Jiang J, Borowiak K, Tudge L, Otto C, von Kriegstein K. Neural mechanisms of eye contact when listening to another person talking. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2017;12(2):319–28. pmid:27576745
- 20. Burra N, Kerzel D, George N. Early left parietal activity elicited by direct gaze: A high-density EEG study. PloS one. 2016;11(11):e0166430. pmid:27880776
- 21. Alessandri G, Caprara GV, De Pascalis V. Relations among EEG-alpha asymmetry and positivity personality trait. Brain and Cognition. 2015;97:10–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.04.003 pmid:25956140
- 22. Roslan NS, Izhar LI, Faye I, Saad MNM, Sivapalan S, Rahman MA. Review of EEG and ERP studies of extraversion personality for baseline and cognitive tasks. Personality and Individual Differences. 2017;119(Supplement C):323–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.040.
- 23. Maksimenko VA, Runnova AE, Zhuravlev MO, Protasov P, Kulanin R, Khramova MV, et al. Human personality reflects spatio-temporal and time-frequency EEG structure. PloS one. 2018;13(9):e0197642. pmid:30192756
- 24.
Sanei S, Chambers JA. EEG signal processing: John Wiley & Sons; 2013.
- 25.
Eysenck HJ. The biological basis of personality: Transaction publishers; 1967.
- 26. Tran Y, Craig A, McIsaac P. Extraversion–introversion and 8–13 Hz waves in frontal cortical regions. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30(2):205–15.
- 27. Fink A, Neubauer AC. Eysenck meets Martindale: The relationship between extraversion and originality from the neuroscientific perspective. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44(1):299–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.08.010.
- 28. Savage RD. ELECTRO-CEREBRAL ACTIVITY, EXTRAVERSION AND NEUROTICISM. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science. 1964;110:98–100. Epub 1964/01/01. pmid:14103379.
- 29. O'Gorman JG, Lloyd JEM. Extraversion, impulsiveness, and EEG alpha activity. Personality and Individual Differences. 1987;8(2):169–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(87)90171-1.
- 30. Wall TL, Schuckit MA, Mungas D, Ehlers CL. EEG alpha activity and personality traits. Alcohol. 1990;7(5):461–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/0741-8329(90)90032-8. pmid:2222849
- 31. O'Gorman JG, Mallise LR. Extraversion and the EEG: II. A test of Gale's hypothesis. Biological Psychology. 1984;19(2):113–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(84)90050-4. pmid:6518221
- 32. Gale A, Edwards J, Morris P, Moore R, Forrester D. Extraversion–introversion, neuroticism–stability, and EEG indicators of positive and negative empathic mood. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30(3):449–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00036-2.
- 33. Fink A, Neubauer AC. Extraversion and cortical activation: effects of task complexity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2004;36(2):333–47.
- 34. Fink A, Grabner RH, Neuper C, Neubauer AC. Extraversion and cortical activation during memory performance. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2005;56(2):129–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.11.002 pmid:15804448
- 35. Chavanon M-L, Wacker J, Leue A, Stemmler G. Evidence for a dopaminergic link between working memory and agentic extraversion: An analysis of load-related changes in EEG alpha 1 activity. Biological Psychology. 2007;74(1):46–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.07.001 pmid:16904812
- 36. Fink A. Event-related desynchronization in the EEG during emotional and cognitive information processing: Differential effects of extraversion. Biological Psychology. 2005;70(3):152–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.01.013 pmid:16198045
- 37. Wacker J. Effects of positive emotion, extraversion, and dopamine on cognitive stability-flexibility and frontal EEG asymmetry. Psychophysiology. 2018;55(1):e12727. pmid:28306164
- 38. Volf NV, Razumnikova OM. Sex differences in EEG coherence during a verbal memory task in normal adults. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1999;34(2):113–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00067-7. pmid:10576396
- 39. Christakou A, Halari R, Smith AB, Ifkovits E, Brammer M, Rubia K. Sex-dependent age modulation of frontostriatal and temporo-parietal activation during cognitive control. NeuroImage. 2009;48(1):223–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.070 pmid:19580877
- 40.
John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL. The big five inventory—Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley. CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research. 1991;54.
- 41. Eysenck HJ, Eysenck S. The Eysenck personality inventory. 1965.
- 42. Berg P, Scherg M. A multiple source approach to the correction of eye artifacts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1994;90(3):229–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(94)90094-9. pmid:7511504
- 43. Hanouneh S, Amin HU, Saad NM, Malik AS. EEG Power and Functional Connectivity Correlates with Semantic Long-Term Memory Retrieval. IEEE Access. 2018;6:8695–703.
- 44. Thatcher RW, North D, Biver C. EEG and intelligence: relations between EEG coherence, EEG phase delay and power. Clinical neurophysiology. 2005;116(9):2129–41. pmid:16043403
- 45. Pfurtscheller G, Da Silva FL. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clinical neurophysiology. 1999;110(11):1842–57. pmid:10576479
- 46.
Goldberg LR. The Big Five factor structure as an integrative framework: An empirical comparison with Eysenck’s PEN model. The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood.7-35.
- 47. Uusberg H, Allik J, Hietanen JK. Eye contact reveals a relationship between Neuroticism and anterior EEG asymmetry. Neuropsychologia. 2015;73:161–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.008 pmid:25980386
- 48.
Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behaviors science.(2nd). New Jersey: Laurence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers, Hillsdale. 1988.
- 49. Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. International Journal of Medical Education. 2011;2:53–5. pmid:28029643 PubMed PMID: PMC4205511.
- 50. Korjus K, Uusberg A, Uusberg H, Kuldkepp N, Kreegipuu K, Allik J, et al. Personality cannot be predicted from the power of resting state EEG. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9(63). pmid:25762912
- 51. Feng W, Störmer VS, Martinez A, McDonald JJ, Hillyard SA. Involuntary orienting of attention to a sound desynchronizes the occipital alpha rhythm and improves visual perception. Neuroimage. 2017;150:318–28. pmid:28213117
- 52. Mobbs N. Eye‐contact in relation to social introversion/extraversion. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1968;7(4):305–6.
- 53. Kleck RE, Nuessle W. Congruence between the indicative and communicative functions of eye contact in interpersonal relations. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1968;7(4):241–6. pmid:5706460
- 54. Binetti N, Harrison C, Coutrot A, Johnston A, Mareschal I. Pupil dilation as an index of preferred mutual gaze duration. Royal Society Open Science. 2016;3(7):160086. pmid:27493767
- 55. Guo M, Liu R-D, Ding Y, Hu B, Zhen R, Liu Y, et al. How are extraversion, exhibitionism, and gender associated with posting selfies on WeChat friends' circle in Chinese teenagers? Personality and Individual Differences. 2018;127:114–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.042.
- 56. Duffy KA, Chartrand TL. The extravert advantage: How and when extraverts build rapport with other people. Psychological Science. 2015;26(11):1795–802. pmid:26408038
- 57. Gale A, Coles M, Blaydon J. EXTRAVERSION—INTROVERSION AND THE EEG. British Journal of Psychology. 1969;60(2):209–23. pmid:5787680
- 58. Brebner J, Cooper C. The effect of a low rate of regular signals upon the reaction times of introverts and extraverts. Journal of Research in Personality. 1974;8(3):263–76.
- 59. Thut G, Nietzel A, Brandt SA, Pascual-Leone A. α-Band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(37):9494–502. pmid:16971533
- 60. Braboszcz C, Delorme A. Lost in thoughts: neural markers of low alertness during mind wandering. Neuroimage. 2011;54(4):3040–7. pmid:20946963
- 61. Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC. Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2011;101(4):862. pmid:21859226