Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Use and validity of child neurodevelopment outcome measures in studies on prenatal exposure to psychotropic and analgesic medications – A systematic review

  • Sarah Hjorth ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    s.h.andersen@farmasi.uio.no

    Affiliation PharmacoEpidemiology and Drug Safety Research Group, Department of Pharmacy, Faculty of Mathematics and Natural Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

  • Rebecca Bromley,

    Roles Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Evolution and Genomic Science, School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Biology, Medicine and Health, University of Manchester, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, Manchester, England, Royal Manchester Children's Hospital, Central Manchester University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, England

  • Eivind Ystrom,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations PharmacoEpidemiology and Drug Safety Research Group, Department of Pharmacy, Faculty of Mathematics and Natural Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway, Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway, PROMENTA Research Center, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

  • Angela Lupattelli,

    Roles Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation PharmacoEpidemiology and Drug Safety Research Group, Department of Pharmacy, Faculty of Mathematics and Natural Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

  • Olav Spigset,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Clinical Pharmacology, St. Olav's University Hospital, Trondheim, Norway, Department of Clinical and Molecular Medicine, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

  • Hedvig Nordeng

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations PharmacoEpidemiology and Drug Safety Research Group, Department of Pharmacy, Faculty of Mathematics and Natural Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway, Department of Child Health and Development, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway

Abstract

In recent years there has been increased attention to child neurodevelopment in studies on medication safety in pregnancy. Neurodevelopment is a multifactorial outcome that can be assessed by various assessors, using different measures. This has given rise to a debate on the validity of various measures of neurodevelopment. The aim of this review was twofold. Firstly we aimed to give an overview of studies on child neurodevelopment after prenatal exposure to central nervous system acting medications using psychotropics and analgesics as examples, giving special focus on the use and validity of outcome measures. Secondly, we aimed to give guidance on how to conduct and interpret medication safety studies with neurodevelopment outcomes. We conducted a systematic review in the MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, Web of Science, Scopus, and Cochrane databases from inception to April 2019, including controlled studies on prenatal exposure to psychotropics or analgesics and child neurodevelopment, measured with standardised psychometric instruments or by diagnosis of neurodevelopmental disorder. The review management tool Covidence was used for data-extraction. Outcomes were grouped as motor skills, cognition, behaviour, emotionality, or “other”. We identified 110 eligible papers (psychotropics, 82 papers, analgesics, 29 papers). A variety of neurodevelopmental outcome measures were used, including 27 different psychometric instruments administered by health care professionals, 15 different instruments completed by parents, and 13 different diagnostic categories. In 23 papers, no comments were made on the validity of the outcome measure. In conclusion, establishing neurodevelopmental safety includes assessing a wide variety of outcomes important for the child’s daily functioning including motor skills, cognition, behaviour, and emotionality, with valid and reliable measures from infancy through to adolescence. Consensus is needed in the scientific community on how neurodevelopment should be assessed in medication safety in pregnancy studies.

Review registration number: CRD42018086101 in the PROSPERO database.

Introduction

Traditionally, studies on medication safety in pregnancy have mainly focused on immediate pregnancy outcomes such as the risk of malformations, low birth weight, and prematurity. In recent years, there has been an increased call for studies on the longer-term safety of prenatal exposure to medications, including studies on child neurodevelopment [1,2].

Psychotropic and analgesic medications share the biological plausibility to affect child neurodevelopment if used prenatally, as they can pass the placenta and bind to targets in the developing brain [3,4]. For these relatively common medications, even small increases in risk of neurodevelopmental delays in offspring could have public health impact [5]. Analgesics have a high frequency of use, with mild analgesics as the most common over the counter medication group in pregnancy with frequencies from 50% to over 70% [6,7]. There has been a marked increase in the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants in pregnancy over the last ten years, with the most recent prevalence data ranging from 2–4% in Europe [8,9] to 8% in North America [10]. Other psychotropics are less frequently used in pregnancy, but are important to study given the potential negative impact of a suboptimally treated maternal disorder on the health of mother and child [11,12].

Neurodevelopment comprises a wide range of traits and includes intelligence, language and motor skills, and attentional and executive functioning [13], which all are important for everyday life. When measuring neurodevelopment, some studies use the presence or absence of medical diagnoses, while others use psychometric instruments (questionnaires or tests) completed by parents, teachers, or health care professionals. Recent initiatives have suggested that it is important to consider a spectrum of neurodevelopment, not just diagnostic categories [14]. The question of how to measure neurodevelopmental outcomes in medication safety studies was recently raised in an editorial by our research group [2]. There we further call for consensus on how to conduct neurodevelopmental safety studies as part of a future pharmacovigilance framework [2].

Attempts have been made to summarise the literature on specific medication classes as the antidepressants [15], and paracetamol in relation to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD, corresponding to the ICD-10 diagnosis hyperkinetic disorder) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [16]. A review on antidepressants chose not to present pooled effect estimates given the heterogeneity of the outcome measures [15], whereas a review on paracetamol presented pooled effect estimates for risk of ASD and ADHD, despite clear heterogeneity of outcome measures [16]. In summaries of the evidence, there has been little discussion about the validity and reliability of outcome measures.

Independent of which outcome measures are used, these should be reliable and valid in order for research data to be of value. Evaluations of reliability and validity are important for both medical diagnoses [17] and psychometric instruments [18,19], as both methods to some extent rely on subjective assessments [20]. Reliability is the degree to which the assessment is free from measurement error [18] (see also S1 File for definitions of different types of validity and reliability). Low reliability in an outcome measure mainly introduces random error and can therefore affect the precision of the results. This is particularly problematic in studies with smaller sample sizes. Validity is defined as the extent to which the outcome measure truly measures the construct (e.g. shyness) it is intended to measure [19]. An outcome measure that is not valid in the context where it is used, will give systematically erroneous results, which is problematic regardless of sample size.

To our knowledge, the use and validity of neurodevelopmental outcome measures in studies on medication safety in pregnancy have not yet been systematically evaluated. Hence the primary aim of this review was to provide an overview of the use and validity of child neurodevelopment outcome measures employed in completed medication safety studies on prenatal exposure to psychotropic or analgesic medications. The secondary aim was aid researchers and clinicians in conducting and interpreting studies on maternal prenatal use of psychotropics and analgesics, and child neurodevelopment.

Methods

A systematic review was conducted in the MEDLINE, Embase, PsycInfo, Scopus, Cochrane, and Web of Science databases from inception to January 17th 2018. The search was updated on April 30th 2019. Reference lists of relevant reviews and included studies were screened to ensure complete coverage of the published literature. The search strategies were developed by the first author and research librarians, with inputs from all authors. Search terms and an example of the search strategy for the MEDLINE database can be found in S2 File. This systematic review was registered in the PROSPERO database (registration number CRD42018086101) and reported according to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews (PRISMA).

Studies were considered eligible for inclusion if they fulfilled the criteria for participants, exposures, comparators, outcomes, and study design as described below. Participants were children born to mothers who used psychotropic or analgesic medication in pregnancy. For this review, children were defined as individuals under the age of 18 years. Assessments of neurodevelopment before the child was one month old were not considered, as we wished to exclude transient effects of prenatal exposure to medication. Exposures were maternal antenatal use of analgesic medication (ATC-codes N02 and M01A), antipsychotic medication (ATC-code N05A), anxiolytic medication (ATC-code N05B), hypnotic and sedative medication (ATC-code N05C), or antidepressants (ATC-code N06A) [21]. Antiepileptic medication (ATC-code N03) was not included in the review, as it has been thoroughly investigated in recent reviews [22,23]. Comparators were children born to mothers who did not use the specified medications in pregnancy. Outcomes were all neurodevelopment outcomes that had been assessed either by psychiatric diagnoses, or by standardised psychometric instruments filled in by parents, teachers, or health care professionals. In order to provide an overview, we divided the neurodevelopment outcomes in the following domains:

  • Motor skills: Including ICD-10 code F82, specific developmental disorder of motor skills [24]
  • Cognition: Including ICD-10 codes F70-79, mental retardation, F80, specific developmental disorder of speech and language, F81, specific developmental disorder of scholastic skills, and F84, pervasive developmental disorders (ASD)
  • Behaviour: Including ICD-10 codes F90, hyperkinetic disorders, and F91, conduct disorders
  • Emotionality: Including ICD-10 codes F30-39, mood disorders, F40-49, neurotic, stress-related and somatoform disorders (including anxiety), and F93, emotional disorders with onset specific to childhood
  • Other: Including sleep disorders (ICD-10 code F51), and tic disorders (ICD-10 code F95)

For all ICD-10 codes, corresponding DSM-5 codes were likewise eligible. Autosomal genetic disorders (Rett syndrome, ICD-10 code F84.2) and unspecific disorders (Other childhood disintegrative disorder, ICD-10 code F84.3) were excluded. As the population for this review was children, we also excluded mental disorders that have their onset in adolescence [25]: bipolar disorder (ICD-10 code F31), schizophrenia (ICD-10 codes F20-29), and substance use disorder (ICD-10 codes F10-19). Randomised controlled trials (RCTs), cohort studies, register-based studies, and case-control studies were considered eligible for inclusion, whereas non-original studies (eg. reviews and editorials), original studies without a comparator group, cross-sectional studies, ecological studies, and animal studies were excluded. No date restrictions were applied, but for resource reasons, the search was limited to peer-reviewed publications in English, French, Italian, Spanish, or one of the Scandinavian languages.

References were imported to the systematic review data management platform Covidence [26]. Title and abstract screening, full text screening, and data extraction were all performed independently by two reviewers (SH and AL). Disagreement was solved by involving a third (HN) and, in case of doubt, a fourth reviewer (RB). When necessary, the authors of the original studies were contacted to provide additional information. Data items extracted from included studies were study design, inclusion and exclusion criteria, study population, duration of follow-up, definition of exposure in pregnancy (including whether gestational age at birth was known, or duration of gestation was estimated), timing, duration and dose of medication, outcome measure used, validity and reliability of outcome measure, covariates and how these were handled, study power, statistical analysis, and effect size. The data extraction forms were developed a priori with inputs from all authors.

Risk of bias in individual studies was assessed using Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) guidelines [27]. In addition to the items specified in the GRADE guidelines (failure to develop appropriate eligibility criteria, flawed measurement of both exposure and outcome, failure to adequately control confounding, and incomplete follow-up) [27], we assessed how missing data was handled in the studies. The risk of bias assessment was used in determining whether there were other explanations for heterogeneity between study findings than the psychometric properties of the chosen outcome measure. The psychometric properties of the outcome measures in the included studies were assessed on the domains internal consistency, inter-rater and test-retest reliability, construct, content and criterion validity (umbrella term for concurrent and predictive validity) as defined by the COnsensus-based Standards for the selection of health Measurement Instruments (COSMIN) group and following their recommendations [28]. Risk of publication bias for each medication group was assessed according to GRADE guidelines [29]. Due to the expected heterogeneity between the studies in terms of age of the child at measurement and method of assessment, no meta-analysis was planned. However, to ease comparisons of results from different papers, effect sizes (Cohen’s d) [30] were calculated using the metaeff package [31] for Stata [32]. Traditionally, a Cohen’s d with an absolute value of 0.2 is considered a small effect, 0.5 a medium effect and 0.8 or above a large effect [33]. The decision to calculate Cohen’s d was made post hoc as we became aware of how many different effect measures were used in the different studies.

Data was grouped by type of assessment (diagnoses or psychometric instruments), assessor for psychometric instruments (health care professional, parents, or teachers), and by age of the child.

Results

The literature search yielded 7,527 studies. After removal of duplicate records, 4,331 studies were left for title and abstract screening. Of these, 206 were relevant for full text assessment, and 101 were eligible for inclusion. See Fig 1 for PRISMA flowchart. A further 9 studies [3442] were identified from reference lists of included studies and relevant reviews. Of the eligible papers, 82 focused on psychotropics (S1, S2, S5 and S6 Tables) and 29 on analgesics (S3 and S7 Tables). Some papers studied more than one medication group. Across all 110 papers, 26 papers used information from databases or national health registries. Neurodevelopment was assessed using 27 different psychometric instruments completed by health care professionals, 15 different psychometric instruments completed by parents, and five different psychometric instruments completed by teachers, not counting different versions of the same instrument (S4 Table). In addition, 13 different diagnostic categories were used. The most commonly used psychometric instrument completed by health care professionals was the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, used in 17 papers. The most common psychometric instrument completed by parents was the Childhood Behaviour Checklist, used in 22 papers. The most common diagnostic category was ASD (ICD-10 code F84) [24], used in 18 papers. However, the outcome measures differed for psychotropics and analgesics. For psychotropics, the most common outcome measure was diagnosis of ASD, used in 16 papers, whereas the most common outcome measures for analgesics were the Ages and Stages Questionnaire and the Child Behaviour Checklist, both used in six papers.

In the following, the use of outcome measures will be described by medication group. Only results for the oldest age band are presented here (and in S1S3 Tables), if a paper had assessed children at multiple time points using the same outcome measure.

Antidepressants

Antidepressants was the most studied medication group with 66 papers [34, 3638, 41,43103]. Children had been assessed from the age of one month to 19 years. Diagnostic codes were used in 23 papers, while psychometric instruments were used in the remaining 43 papers (S1 Table). Most work has been done in the cognitive domain, where focus has been divided between intelligence (IQ), language and risk of ASD diagnosis, and most assessments have been done by health care professionals using psychometric instruments (Fig 2). Contrary to other medications, for antidepressants an additional domain of neurodevelopment other than motor skills, cognition, behaviour, and emotionality was assessed, as two papers reported parent assessed sleep problems.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Domains of neurodevelopment evaluated and data sources used in medication safety papers on psychotropics.

Some papers had outcomes from more than one domain and assessments from more than one type of assessor. HCP: Health care professionals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.g002

Anxiolytics

Twenty papers studied neurodevelopment after prenatal exposure to anxiolytics [35,38,40,56,69,7981,87,92,94,104112], with ten of the papers published ten or more years ago. Follow-up was available from two months to 15 years (S2 Table). Most papers had used assessment from a health care professional, and only one paper had investigated risk of psychiatric diagnosis [56]. Most work has been done on the cognitive domain; the least has been done on emotionality (Fig 2).

Antipsychotics

Antipsychotics were investigated in seven papers [69,72,73,81,94,113,114]. Children had been followed from two months to 18 years. All papers had used psychometric instruments completed by health care professionals, except one that used ASD diagnosis [71] (Fig 2). Most papers, five of seven, studied motor skills. Only one paper had investigated a behavioural outcome [113] and none had investigated emotionality.

Hypnotics and sedatives

Six papers studied hypnotics and sedatives [94,104,109,115117], and three of these papers looked at overdoses taken for suicide attempts. Follow-up was available from 1 year to 5 years. Three papers did not specify age at follow-up, but other papers from the same study have assessed toddlers. Most papers, four of six, had used psychometric instruments completed by health care professionals (Fig 2). Cognition was studied in four, and behaviour in five papers. One paper investigated motor skills [108], none studied emotionality.

Paracetamol

Due to a surge in papers since 2010, paracetamol was the most investigated analgesic with 18 papers [39,42,72,118132]. Studies had follow-up between 18 months and 18 years (S3 Table). Most papers studied cognition and behaviour, with motor skills and emotionality investigated by two papers each (Fig 3). The most common data source was parent reporting.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Domains of neurodevelopment evaluated and data sources used in medication safety papers on analgesics.

Some papers had outcomes from more than one domain and assessments from more than one type of assessor. HCP: Health care professionals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.g003

NSAIDs

Five papers investigated NSAIDs [120,133136]. Three of the papers were more than ten years old. Children were followed from 6 months to 6 years. Three papers studied exposure to indomethacin for treatment of preterm contractions, so the populations consisted of a higher proportion of children born prematurely than the general population. Four papers reported assessments by health care professionals using psychometric instruments, two papers used parent reporting, and one teacher reporting [135]. The papers covered motor skills, cognition and behaviour domains of neurodevelopment, but not emotionality (Fig 3).

Acetylsalicylic acid

Of the five papers on acetylsalicylic acid [39,127,130,137,138], three were more than 20 years old. Children were assessed from 4 to 11 years of age. Three papers used assessments by health care professionals using psychometric instruments. The newest paper used parent and teacher reporting [130]. One paper investigated the motor -, two the cognition -, and two the behaviour domain of neurodevelopment (Fig 3).

Triptans

In four papers from the same research group [139142], the same cohort of children was followed from 18 months to 5 years of age. All domains of neurodevelopment were assessed and assessors were the children’s parents (Fig 3). A fifth paper from a different context had follow-up until age 18 and assessed risk of ASD diagnosis [72].

Analgesic opioids

Many papers have been written on use of illicit opioids, but for this review only analgesic opioids were included, yielding two papers [128,143] (Fig 3). In one paper, children’s language development was assessed at 3 years of age by the parents, and in the other, the risk of diagnoses of ASD and developmental delay were assessed in pre-schoolers.

Validity and reliability of neurodevelopmental outcome measures

In 23 of the 110 eligible papers, no comment was made on either reliability or validity of any of the chosen outcome measures (Tables 13). The majority, 60 papers, commented qualitatively on reliability and/or validity of at least one of their chosen outcome measures, for instance by writing that the outcome measure was validated in their country. The remaining 27 papers also provided at least one quantitative measure of reliability and/or validity, such as Cronbach’s α for internal consistency. Of the papers that commented on specific types of validity and/or reliability, most, 28 papers, commented on concurrent validitywhile content validity was only mentioned in one paper (Fig 4, see Tables 13 for more detail). In 37 papers, it was not mentioned what type of validity the authors commented on, but it was rather stated, for example, that the outcome measure was well-validated. Reliability was mentioned in 36 papers, and of these 17 did not specify what type of reliability that was referred to. We have provided an overview of the reliability and validity of the used outcome measures based on information from the psychometric literature (See S4 Table). In the following, the reporting of validity and reliability of outcome measures will be described by medication group.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Psychometric properties of the neurodevelopment outcome measure mentioned in medication safety papers on psychotropics and analgesics.

*Some papers commented on both validity and reliability, and those papers that commented on specific types of validity or reliability could comment on more than on type. One study used both diagnoses and psychometric instruments. Therefore the numbers add up to more than the total number of papers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.g004

thumbnail
Table 1. Validity and reliability of outcome measures as reported by the authors of the study, papers on antidepressants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Validity and reliability of outcome measures as reported by the authors of the study, papers on psychotropics except antidepressants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Validity and reliability of outcome measures as reported by the authors of the study, papers on analgesics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.t003

Antidepressants

In 66 papers on antidepressants, 53 had comments on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. It was most common to report on concurrent validity, done in 22 papers, or not to specify the type of validity that was commented on, which was the case for another 22 papers. One paper mentioned content validity, and none of the papers mentioned structural validity.

Anxiolytics

Ten of the 20 papers on anxiolytics had comments on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. The type of validity that was mentioned most often was construct validity in the form of correlation to the full scale of the instrument. None of the papers mentioned content validity.

Antipsychotics

Of seven papers on antipsychotics, five had comments on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. However, four of these did not mention what type of validity their comment regarded. None of the papers mentioned construct validity, content validity or internal consistency.

Hypnotics and sedatives

Half of the six papers on hypnotics and sedative commented on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. No type of validity or reliability was mentioned in more than one paper. None of the papers mentioned content validity, inter-rater–or test-retest reliability.

Paracetamol

All but one of the 18 papers on paracetamol had comments on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. Eight papers did not specify what type of validity the comment regarded. None of the papers mentioned content validity.

NSAIDs

Three of five papers on NSAIDs had comments on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. All three mentioned cross-cultural validity (a subtype of construct validity), none mentioned criterion validity, content validity, inter-rater–or test-retest reliability.

Acetylsalicylic acid

All five papers commented on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. In three papers, the type of validity was not specified, yet four of the papers commented on a specified type of reliability. None of the papers mentioned construct validity or content validity.

Triptans

Of five papers on triptans, four had comments on validity and/or reliability of at least one of their outcome measures. Most frequently mentioned was predictive validity and cross-cultural validity. None of the papers mentioned structural validity or content validity.

Opioids

Both papers on opioids commented on the validity of at least one of their outcome measures, one on concurrent validity, the other did not specify type of validity. None of the papers commented on reliability, construct validity or content validity.

Discussion

In the 110 papers on neurodevelopment after prenatal exposure to psychotropics (66 papers on antidepressants, 27 papers on other psychotropics) or analgesics (29 papers) identified from a systematic review of the literature, 47 different psychometric instruments and 13 different diagnostic categories were used to measure neurodevelopment. Twenty-three papers did not mention the reliability or validity of any of the neurodevelopment outcome measures. Among the papers that did mention psychometric properties, 37 papers did not specify on what type of validity they were commenting.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this review include a comprehensive search in six different databases, an interdisciplinary research team, compliance with PRISMA guidelines, and assessment of study eligibility and quality, as well as data extraction, done in double.

Limitations include that only published papers in predefined languages were included in the review, though no papers in other languages that fulfilled the remaining eligibility criteria turned up in our search. Further, search strategy could have been optimised by inclusion of the names of maternal illnesses that are indications for use of the studied medications. Publication bias could not be ruled out for most of the medication groups. Although this may affect the interpretation of effect sizes and risks of using the medications in pregnancy, we consider it unlikely to affect how authors report the psychometric properties of the instruments they use. Finally, the reviewers were not blinded to study authors when assessing study eligibility and quality. This could be a limitation as many of the included studies were done in our research group. However, the use of predefined criteria for eligibility and quality assessment should decrease the risk of bias.

Points for consideration

There are many factors influencing study design in this area, however a lack of current consensus leads to incompatible data across studies which undoubtedly prolongs the period of time it takes to confirm safety or risks to the foetus. Based on the systematic literature review and our experiences as researchers and clinicians, we provide five points that should be considered for the conduct and interpretation of studies on maternal prenatal use of medications, and child neurodevelopment.

A wide variety of outcomes must be assessed to establish neurodevelopmental safety.

The human brain is complex and its functions are diverse. Whilst there is comorbidity between neurodevelopmental disorders [24,144], all domains of neurodevelopment (e.g. IQ, language efficiency, attention etc.) are important for children’s daily living, and it should be considered that they may be differentially impacted upon by teratogen exposure. Therefore a call for complete consensus on how to measure neurodevelopment, for instance by selecting one domain of neurodevelopment as the priority in medication safety research, is not reasonable. A complete consensus on choice of outcome measure may also be difficult to obtain, as a single outcome measure does not exist which can reliably measure all diverse aspect of neurodevelopment, and different populations may require different measures due to variables such as age and geographical region. All outcome measures have strengths and weaknesses that we will elaborate on below, and validation of psychometric instruments and diagnoses is done using other psychometric instruments or diagnostic categories as references, making the discussion of validity relativistic. Despite these challenges, to be able to build on each other’s research and pool results in meta-analyses, it is necessary that some agreement should be reached regarding a core outcome set for teratology studies investigating neurodevelopment, so studies assessing the same domain of neurodevelopment and using the same data source also would use compatible outcome measures and report results in a uniform manner.

Previous literature, both animal and human, should inform choice of outcomes to be measured.

In order to bring about a more uniform approach to the study of central nervous system acting medications in pregnancy and the potential impact on the developing brain, new research should select primary outcomes guided by previous literature from both animal and human studies. Reviews of the pre-clinical research, or knowledge of the literature on in-vitro or animal studies, are necessary along with those of the already available human literature. Prenatal exposure to the antiepileptic medication valproate demonstrates this point. Despite early case reports noting impaired human neurodevelopment alongside major congenital malformations, Pregnancy Registers around the world were established to assess major congenital malformation risk but not neurodevelopment [145]. Further, an early review of the pre-clinical research data would have added further weight for the requirement to study neurodevelopment following valproate exposure with the same gravitas as major congenital malformations. As early as 1996 an association between valproate and ASD like behaviours was noted [146], whilst ASD in exposed children was not the focus of a prospective investigation until much later [22].

Data sources have different strengths and weaknesses and should complement each other.

Standardised psychometric instruments completed blinded to exposure status by health care professionals are considered the gold standard to assess certain areas of neurodevelopment, e.g. Bayley Scales of Infant Development to assess early development [57], or Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence to assess child IQ [58]. Such clinical assessments are often detailed, providing comprehensive information on a number of neurodevelopmental outcomes. A strength of assessment by health care professionals for research purposes is that blinding of the assessors can reduce unconscious bias. It is therefore important that blinding takes place when possible and that authors state whether the assessors were blinded when reporting a study. In this review, blinding status was reported in half of the 52 papers that used assessments by health care professionals (S5S7 Tables). A limitation to the use of licenced psychometric instruments is costliness, training of assessors, and the amount of time spent on each assessment. In addition, families will be required to give up time for the assessment. Therefore we see in this review that studies using psychometric instruments completed by health care professionals often are small in size and in some cases only powered to detect the largest of group differences. In small studies, where random error may impact results, it is important to report on the reliability of the outcome measure used. For all psychometric instruments, the concurrent and predictive validity should be considered, as well as content validity to enable clinicians and researchers to determine whether the symptoms or traits evaluated by the scales are in fact clinically relevant to the specific population being investigated.

There are limited opportunities for health care professionals to measure child behaviour and emotionality in addition to cognitive development, as a clinic will rarely provide natural settings to observe these domains. In addition, emotionality is dependent on both situation and relation to the assessor. Therefore these domains will often rely on parent, teacher, or child reporting. In settings where few children are looked after in day care centres, parents will often be the only ones who see preschool children on a sufficiently regular basis to provide assessments of behaviour or emotionality.

Often less burdensome for families, and, in some cases, available to research groups that do not have access to licenced psychologists, psychometric instruments completed by parents or teachers can be used in large samples. There are two main weaknesses in parent reporting. One is the lack of blinding to exposure status, which is particularly important for medications that have received media attention as having unfavourable effects on the developing brain. The other is specific to psychotropics, namely distortion bias, the influence of maternal mood on the assessment. Whether maternal mood will affect reporting is at present disputed [64]. Some studies indicate that mothers with no emotional disorders will underrate child problems, while mothers with emotional disorders will overrate [147,148], whereas other studies do not find clinically significant effects of maternal emotional disorder [149,150].

Teacher reporting may be blinded to exposure status. However, the expectations of children in a classroom setting may be different from what is expected from children elsewhere. One review concluded that teacher reporting results in a higher prevalence of ADHD than if the disease is classified according to diagnostic criteria [151]. In addition, not all domains of neurodevelopment may be assessed with equal ease in a classroom setting. Hence in a meta-analysis of 119 studies, parent reporting of emotional problems correlated better than teacher reporting with children’s own assessments [152]. When parent and teacher reporting only show moderate correlations, it is important to consider that they represent assessment of the child in very different settings. Some children have problems at school that are not present in the home-environment. Hence one assessment is not necessarily more correct than the other. In both parent and teacher reporting, the concurrent and predictive validity should be considered, as well as content validity to enable clinicians and researchers to determine whether the symptoms or traits evaluated by the scales are in fact clinically relevant problems.

When using the presence or absence of a diagnosis (i.e. ASD or ADHD) to assess neurodevelopment, we clearly only examine the most affected individuals. In countries or regions with health registries, this data source is comparatively cheap and fast. However, detection bias is always possible, and blinding of assessors rating the outcome is not possible. For instance, women exposed to a suspected teratogen or women with a history of mental illness may be more likely to get their children examined for mental illnesses. Further, the presence or absence of diagnoses can be a somewhat crude measure and may differ by region, country, or version of the diagnostic manual in their criteria. Often registries contain data from public secondary services, wherefore children managed in primary care or in private hospitals may be misclassified. In addition, not all children with clinically relevant problems will fulfil the diagnostic criteria for a certain disorder. As an example, the known developmental neurobehavioural teratogen valproate increases the prevalence of ASD in children from 1.8% in general population controls to 8–15% in prenatally exposed children [22]. However, the clinical picture in these cases of ASD is atypical [153]. It is possible that the medications we investigate will increase the risk of a syndrome that is not caught by common diagnostic criteria. Lessons learned from congenital malformations show that minor deviations should not be overlooked as they might be part of specific diagnosis [154]. When using the presence or absence of diagnoses as a neurodevelopmental outcome, the authors should address both the validity of the diagnostic criteria, and the validity of the recording of diagnoses in the registries the data stem from. The specificity of diagnoses from registries can be increased by requiring that a diagnosis should be present at least twice in a child’s medical records before the child is considered as having that diagnosis [155]. This will exclude the instances where a child is evaluated to rule out a diagnosis.

As the different data sources have different strengths and weaknesses, they can be used to complement each other. So far, a minority of studies have used a mixture of data sources, and only one used assessment by both diagnoses and psychometric instruments [122]. Future studies should to a greater extent use more than one data source to measure neurodevelopment if the expertise within the research group allows. For an example of how this could be done, see the paper by Liew and colleagues [122].

In meta-analyses, the use of different data sources can be challenging. Currently, review authors differ on whether to combine outcome measures from different data sources in meta-analyses [16,156158], or summarise the evidence qualitatively [15,159]. Until a consensus is reached on which outcome measures to use and which to combine, we would like to caution over the combining of data from different research methodologies. Further, given the diversity of neurodevelopmental outcomes and their measurement there is a requirement of in-depth knowledge of the various outcome measures, as they are often based on different constructs reflecting different neurodevelopmental domains at different ages. Finally, standard approaches to meta-analysis of data including publication bias and heterogeneity in outcome measures between studies should be taken into account per outcome using standardised methods such as funnel plots.

The outcome measure should be age appropriate.

Standardised tests and questionnaires are often validated for a certain age group. If the outcome measure is to be used in a different age group, it should first be validated for use in that age group [19]. Researchers using diagnoses as outcomes should be aware that certain diagnoses are not valid below a certain age. For example, the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends that children should be 4 years old before DSM-IV diagnostic criteria of ADHD can be used [144]. Children should not be considered as having a diagnosis, if they only have a diagnosis recorded at an age where it is considered implausible that a correct diagnosis can be made. Length of follow-up should be guided by the average age at diagnosis for the specific disorder in the country where the research is carried out.

When planning new research on medication safety for neurodevelopment, it should also be considered that brain development continues into early adulthood [25], and that some difficulties in certain cognitive domains will not be detectable until the teenage years, when more complex cognitive processing is required [13]. For example, very different levels of inhibition or reasoning ability are expected from a 3-year-old and a 13-year-old. Longer follow-up would also allow investigation into mental disorders that may have developmental origins, but that have their onset in adolescence, such as schizophrenia. Another way to take into account the continuing development of the child brain is to investigate trajectories of development. This method is common in psychology [160], and could be employed in medication safety studies as well, if children are assessed at several points in time.

Reliability and validity of the outcome measure should be reported.

The use of several different outcome measures across studies makes it difficult for readers to be familiar with all the different measures. This increases the responsibility of authors to provide information on validity and reliability. Many of the studies in this review were large, including several hundred exposed pregnancies, thus limiting the risk of random error. In these studies, the most important psychometric property to report is validity, as invalid measures may introduce bias.

In psychology, construct validity is often considered the most important form of validity, as there are no objective criteria or “gold standards” to compare to [161]. Quite surprisingly, only three papers mentioned structural validity, one of which provided quantitative measures, and no papers explicitly mentioned hypothesis testing. Construct validity can be evaluated using statistical methods, and therefore numbers ought to be reported.

Criterion validity can also be evaluated using statistical methods. About a third of the papers provided a comment on criterion validity for at least one of their outcome measures, however only 15 reported a quantitative measure of criterion validity. Concurrent validity, the performance of the psychometric instrument or diagnosis against a gold standard, was reported for 28 papers. Specifically for diagnoses in registry-based studies, concurrent validity can both refer to the validity of the diagnostic criteria, and to the validity of the registration of the investigated diagnoses in the particular registry the data stems from. However, only the latter was mentioned in the papers identified in this review. Predictive validity is mainly relevant to psychometric instruments, and was only mentioned by 11 papers. Predictive validity is important for measures used in children, as children may grow into or out of difficulties [22].

Only one paper mentioned content validity [55], the degree to which the questions or tasks that make up a psychometric instrument, or the criteria that make up a diagnosis, are relevant and comprehensive measures of the domain of neurodevelopment that the outcome measure is used to investigate [19]. Content validity cannot be evaluated with the use of statistical methods. As such it is more subjective than the other forms of validity, which may make authors hesitant to comment on it. However, expert group evaluation of content validity has been done for diagnostic criteria and for some psychometric instruments [162,163], and could be reported. Another option is to make the questions or tasks of an outcome measure available to readers in a supplement (if copyright allows), so readers can assess content validity.

If the content or criterion validity is low or unknown, this should be reflected in the language used in the paper. For example, if a study uses a psychometric instrument to assess the presence of ADHD, and the instrument has not shown acceptable validity when tested against diagnostic interviews for ADHD, authors should write that they have assessed “symptoms of ADHD” and not “ADHD” as such.

In studies where a psychometric instrument is used in a different language or for another population than that in which it was developed, cross-cultural validity will tell us the extent to which the instrument measures the same as the original instrument. Without validation it cannot be assumed that the outcome measure is valid in a different population from the one for which it was developed [19]. Yet, cross-cultural validity alone will not be a sufficient measure of validity, as it does not provide any information on whether the original psychometric instrument measures what is intended.

We recognise that many journals have word limits for articles, making it difficult to include detailed information on reliability and validity. However, given the importance of this information it is suggested that this is prioritised. Today many journals allow online supplements, where psychometric properties can be described. For an example of how this could be done, see the online supplement to the paper by Avella-Garcia and colleagues [118]. Many papers identified in this review only referred to the manual of the outcome measure they use, which is often not accessible to the readers.

Finally, other methodological issues than use and validity of outcome measure can introduce between-study heterogeneity and should be considered. Some of these issues are the study limitations in observational studies according to GRADE [27], as assessed in S5S7 Tables. Other examples include choice of appropriate comparator group to handle confounding by indication of medication use [156], analyses of direct and indirect medication effects by taking into account postnatal factors [164], and analysis of medication use by timing, dose and/or duration [165]. Interested readers are referred to a recent review on these methodological issues in medication safety studies with central nervous system outcomes [166].

Conclusions

Studies have used several outcome measures including diagnoses and psychometric instruments completed by health care professionals, parents, or teachers to assess child neurodevelopment, yet few studies reported adequately on the reliability and validity of their outcomes. In order to establish neurodevelopmental safety of prenatal exposure to a medication, it is necessary to assess several domains of neurodevelopment until adolescence using age appropriate outcome measures. For medications where an animal model exists, this should inform which outcomes are assessed first. Authors should use reliable and valid outcome measures to assess neurodevelopment. We encourage reporting on the validity and reliability of the outcome measures used. In addition, results should be interpreted in light of the reliability and validity of the outcome measure that is used. Consensus is required on which outcome measure to use for each age group and data source in each domain of neurodevelopment. Until such consensus is in place, researchers should to a larger extent combine different data sources in one study, and authors of meta-analyses should be aware that in-depth knowledge of the various outcome measures is necessary when deciding which outcomes can be combined.

Supporting information

S1 File. Definitions of different types of reliability and validity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s001

(PDF)

S2 File. Search terms and search strategy, example for the MEDLINE database.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s002

(PDF)

S1 Table. Study characteristics, papers on antidepressants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s004

(PDF)

S2 Table. Study characteristics, papers on psychotropics except for antidepressants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s005

(PDF)

S3 Table. Study characteristics, papers on analgesics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s006

(PDF)

S4 Table. Reliability and validity of the outcome measures used in studies on neurodevelopmental safety of prenatal exposure to psychotropics and analgesics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s007

(PDF)

S5 Table. Risk of bias assessments, papers on antidepressants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s008

(PDF)

S6 Table. Risk of bias assessments, papers on psychotropics except for antidepressants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s009

(PDF)

S7 Table. Risk of bias assessments, papers on analgesics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219778.s010

(PDF)

References

  1. 1. EUROmediCAT Steering Group. EUROmediCAT Recommendations for European Pharmacovigilance concerning safety of medication use in pregnancy: ABSTRACT. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2015 Oct;24:3–7. pmid:26395593
  2. 2. Nordeng H, Lupattelli A, Wood M. Prenatal exposure to antidepressants and increased risk of psychiatric disorders. BMJ. 2017 Sep 6;j3950. pmid:28877910
  3. 3. Oberlander TF, Gingrich JA, Ansorge MS. Sustained neurobehavioral effects of exposure to SSRI antidepressants during development: molecular to clinical evidence. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2009;86(6):672–7. pmid:19890255
  4. 4. de Fays L, Van Malderen K, De Smet K, Sawchik J, Verlinden V, Hamdani J, et al. Use of paracetamol during pregnancy and child neurological development. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2015;57(8):718–24. pmid:25851072
  5. 5. Broussard CS, Frey MT, Hernandez-Diaz S, Greene MF, Chambers CD, Sahin L, et al. Developing a systematic approach to safer medication use during pregnancy: summary of a Centers for Disease Control and Prevention–convened meeting. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2014 Sep;211(3):208–214.e1. pmid:24881821
  6. 6. Lupattelli A, Spigset O, Twigg MJ, Zagorodnikova K, Mårdby AC, Moretti ME, et al. Medication use in pregnancy: a cross-sectional, multinational web-based study. BMJ Open. 2014 Feb;4(2):e004365. pmid:24534260
  7. 7. Werler MM, Mitchell AA, Hernandez-Diaz S, Honein MA, the National Birth Defects Prevention Study. Use of over-the-counter medications during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2005 Sep;193(3):771–7.
  8. 8. Bakker MK, Kölling P, van den Berg PB, de Walle HEK, de Jong van den Berg LTW. Increase in use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in pregnancy during the last decade, a population-based cohort study from the Netherlands. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2008 Apr;65(4):600–6. pmid:17953715
  9. 9. Jimenez-Solem E, Andersen JT, Petersen M, Broedbaek K, Andersen NL, Torp-Pedersen C, et al. Prevalence of antidepressant use during pregnancy in Denmark, a nation-wide cohort study. PloS One. 2013;8(4):e63034. pmid:23638179
  10. 10. Mitchell AA, Gilboa SM, Werler MM, Kelley KE, Louik C, Hernández-Díaz S. Medication use during pregnancy, with particular focus on prescription drugs: 1976–2008. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011 Jul;205(1):51.e1-51.e8.
  11. 11. Jarde A, Morais M, Kingston D, Giallo R, MacQueen GM, Giglia L, et al. Neonatal Outcomes in Women With Untreated Antenatal Depression Compared With Women Without Depression: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016 Aug 1;73(8):826. pmid:27276520
  12. 12. Tosato S, Albert U, Tomassi S, Iasevoli F, Carmassi C, Ferrari S, et al. A Systematized Review of Atypical Antipsychotics in Pregnant Women: Balancing Between Risks of Untreated Illness and Risks of Drug-Related Adverse Effects. J Clin Psychiatry. 2017 May;78(5):e477–89. pmid:28297592
  13. 13. Bromley RL, Baker GA. Fetal antiepileptic drug exposure and cognitive outcomes. Seizure. 2017 Jan;44:225–31. pmid:27784632
  14. 14. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. 2013 May 14;11:126. pmid:23672542
  15. 15. El Marroun H, White T, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H. Maternal use of antidepressant or anxiolytic medication during pregnancy and childhood neurodevelopmental outcomes: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(10):973–92. pmid:24863148
  16. 16. Masarwa R, Levine H, Gorelik E, Reif S, Perlman A, Matok I. Prenatal Exposure to Acetaminophen and Risk for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Autistic Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression Analysis of Cohort Studies. Am J Epidemiol. 2018 Aug;187(8):1817–27. pmid:29688261
  17. 17. Kraemer HC, Kupfer DJ, Clarke DE, Narrow WE, Regier DA. DSM-5: How Reliable Is Reliable Enough? Am J Psychiatry. 2012 Jan;169(1):13–5. pmid:22223009
  18. 18. de Vet HCW, Terwee CB, Mokkink LB, Knol DL. Reliability. In: Measurement in Medicine: A Practical Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2011. p. 96–149.
  19. 19. de Vet HCW, Terwee CB, Mokkink LB, Knol DL. Validity. In: Measurement in Medicine: A Practical Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2011. p. 150–201.
  20. 20. de Vet HCW, Terwee CB, Mokkink LB, Knol DL. Concepts, theories and models, and types of measurements. In: Measurement in Medicine: A Practical Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2011. p. 7–29.
  21. 21. WHO. WHOCC—ATC/DDD Index [Internet]. WHO Collaborating Centre for Drug Statistics Methodology. 2017 [cited 2018 Jul 6]. Available from: https://www.whocc.no/atc_ddd_index/
  22. 22. Bromley R, Weston J, Adab N, Greenhalgh J, Sanniti A, McKay AJ, et al. Treatment for epilepsy in pregnancy: neurodevelopmental outcomes in the child. In: The Cochrane Collaboration, editor. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2014.
  23. 23. Veroniki AA, Rios P, Cogo E, Straus SE, Finkelstein Y, Kealey R, et al. Comparative safety of antiepileptic drugs for neurological development in children exposed during pregnancy and breast feeding: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017 Jul;7(7):e017248. pmid:28729328
  24. 24. WHO. ICD-10 Version:2016 [Internet]. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision. 2016 [cited 2017 Sep 19]. Available from: http://apps.who.int/classifications/icd10/browse/2016/en
  25. 25. Andersen SL. Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability or window of opportunity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003 Jan;27(1–2):3–18. pmid:12732219
  26. 26. Covidence. Covidence—Accelerate your systematic review [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2017 Oct 11]. Available from: https://www.covidence.org/
  27. 27. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist G, Kunz R, Brozek J, Alonso-Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 4. Rating the quality of evidence—study limitations (risk of bias). J Clin Epidemiol. 2011 Apr;64(4):407–15. pmid:21247734
  28. 28. Mokkink LB, Terwee CB, Patrick DL, Alonso J, Stratford PW, Knol DL, et al. The COSMIN checklist for assessing the methodological quality of studies on measurement properties of health status measurement instruments: an international Delphi study. Qual Life Res Int J Qual Life Asp Treat Care Rehabil. 2010 May;19(4):539–49.
  29. 29. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Montori V, Vist G, Kunz R, Brozek J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 5. Rating the quality of evidence—publication bias. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011 Dec;64(12):1277–82. pmid:21802904
  30. 30. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. 567 p.
  31. 31. Kontopantelis E, Reeves D. METAEFF: Stata module to perform effect sizes calculations for meta-analyses [Internet]. 2011 [cited 2018 Jul 6]. Available from: http://EconPapers.repec.org/RePEc:boc:bocode:s457072
  32. 32. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2017.
  33. 33. Fritz CO, Morris PE, Richler JJ. Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2012;141(1):2–18. pmid:21823805
  34. 34. Boukhris T, Sheehy O, Bérard A. Antidepressant Use in Pregnancy and the Risk of Attention Deficit with or without Hyperactivity Disorder in Children. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2017 Jul;31(4):363–73. pmid:28640459
  35. 35. Hartz SC, Heinonen OP, Shapiro S, Siskind V, Slone D. Antenatal Exposure to Meprobamate and Chlordiazepoxide in Relation to Malformations, Mental Development, and Childhood Mortality. N Engl J Med. 1975 Apr 3;292(14):726–8. pmid:1113782
  36. 36. Heikkinen T, Ekblad U, Palo P, Laine K. Pharmacokinetics of fluoxetine and norfluoxetine in pregnancy and lactation. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2003 Apr;73(4):330–7. pmid:12709723
  37. 37. Laugesen K, Olsen MS, Telén Andersen AB, Frøslev T, Sørensen HT. In utero exposure to antidepressant drugs and risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a nationwide Danish cohort study. BMJ Open. 2013 Sep 20;3(9):e003507. pmid:24056487
  38. 38. Reebye PN, Ng TWC, Misri S, Stikarovska I. Affect Expression and Self-Regulation Capacities of Infants Exposed in utero to Psychotropics. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3.
  39. 39. Streissguth AP, Treder RP, Barr HM, Shepard TH, Bleyer WA, Sampson PD, et al. Aspirin and acetaminophen use by pregnant women and subsequent child IQ and attention decrements. Teratology. 1987 Apr;35(2):211–9. pmid:3603404
  40. 40. Viggedal G, Hagberg BS, Laegreid L, Aronsson M. Mental development in late infancy after prenatal exposure to benzodiazepines—a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993 Mar;34(3):295–305. pmid:8463369
  41. 41. Viktorin A, Uher R, Reichenberg A, Levine SZ, Sandin S. Autism risk following antidepressant medication during pregnancy. Psychol Med. 2017 Dec;47(16):2787–96. pmid:28528584
  42. 42. Vlenterie R, Wood ME, Brandlistuen RE, Roeleveld N, van Gelder MMHJ, Nordeng H. Neurodevelopmental problems at 18 months among children exposed to paracetamol in utero: a propensity score matched cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2016 Aug 31;dyw192.
  43. 43. Austin M-P, Karatas JC, Mishra P, Christl B, Kennedy D, Oei J. Infant neurodevelopment following in utero exposure to antidepressant medication. Acta Paediatr Oslo Nor 1992. 2013;102(11):1054–9.
  44. 44. Batton B, Batton E, Weigler K, Aylward G, Batton D. In utero antidepressant exposure and neurodevelopment in preterm infants. Am J Perinatol. 2013;30(4):297–301. pmid:22893558
  45. 45. Boukhris T, Sheehy O, Mottron L, Berard A. Antidepressant Use During Pregnancy and the Risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Children. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(2):117–24. pmid:26660917
  46. 46. Brandlistuen RE, Ystrom E, Eberhard-Gran M, Nulman I, Koren G, Nordeng H. Behavioural effects of fetal antidepressant exposure in a Norwegian cohort of discordant siblings. Int J Epidemiol. 2015 Aug;44(4):1397–407. pmid:25873178
  47. 47. Brown AS, Gyllenberg D, Malm H, McKeague IW, Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki S, Artama M, et al. Association of Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Exposure During Pregnancy With Speech, Scholastic, and Motor Disorders in Offspring. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(11):1163–70. pmid:27732704
  48. 48. Brown HK, Ray JG, Wilton AS, Lunsky Y, Gomes T, Vigod SN. Association Between Serotonergic Antidepressant Use During Pregnancy and Autism Spectrum Disorder in Children. JAMA. 2017;317(15):1544–52. pmid:28418480
  49. 49. Casper RC, Fleisher BE, Lee-Ancajas JC, Gilles A, Gaylor E, DeBattista A, et al. Follow-up of children of depressed mothers exposed or not exposed to antidepressant drugs during pregnancy. J Pediatr. 2003;142(4):402–8. pmid:12712058
  50. 50. Castro VM, Kong SW, Clements CC, Brady R, Kaimal AJ, Doyle AE, et al. Absence of evidence for increase in risk for autism or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder following antidepressant exposure during pregnancy: a replication study. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(101562664):e708.
  51. 51. Clements CC, Castro VM, Blumenthal SR, Rosenfield HR, Murphy SN, Fava M, et al. Prenatal antidepressant exposure is associated with risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder but not autism spectrum disorder in a large health system. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(6):727–34. pmid:25155880
  52. 52. Croen LA, Grether JK, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Hendrick V. Antidepressant use during pregnancy and childhood autism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(11):1104–12. pmid:21727247
  53. 53. de Vries NKS, van der Veere CN, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Early Neurological Outcome of Young Infants Exposed to Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors during Pregnancy: Results from the Observational SMOK Study. Scott JG, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013 May 28;8(5):e64654.
  54. 54. El Marroun H, White TJH, van der Knaap NJF, Homberg JR, Fernandez G, Schoemaker NK, et al. Prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and social responsiveness symptoms of autism: population-based study of young children. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2014;205(2):95–102.
  55. 55. El Marroun H, White TJ, Fernandez G, Jaddoe VW, Verhulst FC, Stricker BH, et al. Prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and non-verbal cognitive functioning in childhood. J Psychopharmacol Oxf Engl. 2017;31(3):346–55.
  56. 56. Figueroa R. Use of antidepressants during pregnancy and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the offspring. J Dev Behav Pediatr JDBP. 2010;31(8):641–8. pmid:20613624
  57. 57. Galbally M, Lewis AJ, Buist A. Developmental outcomes of children exposed to antidepressants in pregnancy. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011;45(5):393–9. pmid:21314237
  58. 58. Galbally M, Lewis AJ, Buist A. Child developmental outcomes in preschool children following antidepressant exposure in pregnancy. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2015;49(7):642–50. pmid:25698806
  59. 59. Gidaya NB, Lee BK, Burstyn I, Yudell M, Mortensen EL, Newschaffer CJ. In utero exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(10):2558–67. pmid:24803368
  60. 60. Grzeskowiak LE, Morrison JL, Henriksen TB, Bech BH, Obel C, Olsen J, et al. Prenatal antidepressant exposure and child behavioural outcomes at 7 years of age: a study within the Danish National Birth Cohort. BJOG Int J Obstet Gynaecol. 2016;123(12):1919–28.
  61. 61. Gustafsson HC, Goodman SH, Feng T, Choi J, Lee S, Newport DJ, et al. Major depressive disorder during pregnancy: Psychiatric medications have minimal effects on the fetus and infant yet development is compromised. Dev Psychopathol. 2018 Aug;30(3):773–85. pmid:30068426
  62. 62. Handal M, Skurtveit S, Furu K, Hernandez-Diaz S, Skovlund E, Nystad W, et al. Motor development in children prenatally exposed to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: a large population-based pregnancy cohort study. BJOG Int J Obstet Gynaecol. 2016;123(12):1908–17.
  63. 63. Handal M, Skurtveit S, Roth C, Hernandez-Diaz S, Selmer R. Prenatal Exposure to Folic Acid and Antidepressants and Language Development: A Population-Based Cohort Study. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2016;36(4):333–9. pmid:27285658
  64. 64. Hanley GE, Brain U, Oberlander TF. Infant developmental outcomes following prenatal exposure to antidepressants, and maternal depressed mood and positive affect. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89(8):519–24. pmid:23384962
  65. 65. Hanley GE, Brain U, Oberlander TF. Prenatal exposure to serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants and childhood behavior. Pediatr Res. 2015;78(2):174–80. pmid:25897539
  66. 66. Harrington RA, Lee L-C, Crum RM, Zimmerman AW, Hertz-Picciotto I. Prenatal SSRI use and offspring with autism spectrum disorder or developmental delay. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):e1241–8. pmid:24733881
  67. 67. Heikkinen T, Ekblad U, Kero P, Ekblad S, Laine K. Citalopram in pregnancy and lactation. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2002;72(2):184–91. pmid:12189365
  68. 68. Hermansen TK, Roysamb E, Augusti E-M, Melinder A. Behavior and inhibitory control in children with prenatal exposure to antidepressants and medically untreated depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233(8):1523–35.
  69. 69. Hurault-Delarue C, Damase-Michel C, Finotto L, Guitard C, Vayssiere C, Montastruc J-L, et al. Psychomotor developmental effects of prenatal exposure to psychotropic drugs: a study in EFEMERIS database. Fundam Clin Pharmacol. 2016;30(5):476–82. pmid:27324073
  70. 70. Hutchison SM, Masse LC, Glier MB, Brain U, Devlin AM, Oberlander TF. Impact of Prenatal Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Antidepressant Exposure and Maternal Mood on Physical Activity, Dietary Intake, and Markers of Adiposity at Age 6 Years. J Dev Behav Pediatr JDBP. 2019 Mar;
  71. 71. Hviid A, Melbye M, Pasternak B. Use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy and risk of autism. N Engl J Med. 2013;369(25):2406–15. pmid:24350950
  72. 72. Janecka M, Kodesh A, Levine SZ, Lusskin S, Viktorin A, Rahman R, et al. Association of Autism Spectrum Disorder With Prenatal Exposure to Medication Affecting Neurotransmitter Systems. Jama Psychiatry. 2018 Dec;75(12):1217–24. pmid:30383108
  73. 73. Johnson KC, LaPrairie JL, Brennan PA, Stowe ZN, Newport DJ. Prenatal antipsychotic exposure and neuromotor performance during infancy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(8):787–94. pmid:22474072
  74. 74. Johnson KC, Smith AK, Stowe ZN, Newport DJ, Brennan PA. Preschool outcomes following prenatal serotonin reuptake inhibitor exposure: differences in language and behavior, but not cognitive function. J Clin Psychiatry. 2016;77(2):e176–82. pmid:26930533
  75. 75. Liu X, Agerbo E, Ingstrup KG, Musliner K, Meltzer-Brody S, Bergink V, et al. Antidepressant use during pregnancy and psychiatric disorders in offspring: Danish nationwide register based cohort study. Bmj-Br Med J. 2017 Sep 6;358:j3668.
  76. 76. Lupattelli A, Wood M, Ystrom E, Skurtveit S, Handal M, Nordeng H. Effect of Time-Dependent Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Antidepressants During Pregnancy on Behavioral, Emotional, and Social Development in Preschool-Aged Children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018 Mar;57(3):200–8. pmid:29496129
  77. 77. Malm H, Brown AS, Gissler M, Gyllenberg D, Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki S, McKeague IW, et al. Gestational Exposure to Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors and Offspring Psychiatric Disorders: A National Register-Based Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55(5):359–66. pmid:27126849
  78. 78. Man KKC, Chan EW, Ip P, Coghill D, Simonoff E, Chan PKL, et al. Prenatal antidepressant use and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring: population based cohort study. BMJ. 2017;357(8900488, bmj, 101090866):j2350.
  79. 79. Mattson SN, Calarco KE, Chambers CD, Jones KL. Interaction of maternal smoking and other in-pregnancy exposures: analytic considerations. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24(3):359–67. pmid:12009491
  80. 80. Misri S, Reebye P, Kendrick K, Carter D, Ryan D, Grunau RE, et al. Internalizing behaviors in 4-year-old children exposed in utero to psychotropic medications. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(6):1026–32. pmid:16741203
  81. 81. Mortensen JT, Olsen J, Larsen H, Bendsen J, Obel C, Sorensen HT. Psychomotor development in children exposed in utero to benzodiazepines, antidepressants, neuroleptics, and anti-epileptics. Eur J Epidemiol. 2003;18(8):769–71. pmid:12974552
  82. 82. Netsi E, Evans J, Wulff K, O’Mahen H, Ramchandani PG. Infant outcomes following treatment of antenatal depression: Findings from a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Affect Disord. 2015 Dec;188:252–6. pmid:26372945
  83. 83. Nulman I, Rovet J, Stewart DE, Wolpin J, Gardner HA, Theis JGW, et al. Neurodevelopment of Children Exposed in Utero to Antidepressant Drugs. N Engl J Med. 1997 Jan 23;336(4):258–62. pmid:8995088
  84. 84. Nulman I, Rovet J, Stewart DE, Wolpin J, Pace-Asciak P, Shuhaiber S, et al. Child development following exposure to tricyclic antidepressants or fluoxetine throughout fetal life: a prospective, controlled study. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(11):1889–95. pmid:12411224
  85. 85. Nulman I, Koren G, Rovet J, Barrera M, Pulver A, Streiner D, et al. Neurodevelopment of children following prenatal exposure to venlafaxine, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, or untreated maternal depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(11):1165–74. pmid:23128923
  86. 86. Nulman I, Koren G, Rovet J, Barrera M, Streiner DL, Feldman BM. Neurodevelopment of children prenatally exposed to selective reuptake inhibitor antidepressants: Toronto sibling study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2015;76(7):e842–7. pmid:26231010
  87. 87. Oberlander TF, Misri S, Fitzgerald CE, Kostaras X, Rurak D, Riggs W. Pharmacologic factors associated with transient neonatal symptoms following prenatal psychotropic medication exposure. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65(2):230–7. pmid:15003078
  88. 88. Oberlander TF, Reebye P, Misri S, Papsdorf M, Kim J, Grunau RE. Externalizing and attentional behaviors in children of depressed mothers treated with a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressant during pregnancy. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(1):22–9. pmid:17199063
  89. 89. Oberlander TF, Papsdorf M, Brain UM, Misri S, Ross C, Grunau RE. Prenatal effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants, serotonin transporter promoter genotype (SLC6A4), and maternal mood on child behavior at 3 years of age. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(5):444–51. pmid:20439795
  90. 90. Pedersen LH, Henriksen TB, Bech BH, Licht RW, Kjaer D, Olsen J. Prenatal antidepressant exposure and behavioral problems in early childhood—a cohort study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;127(2):126–35. pmid:23126521
  91. 91. Rai D, Lee BK, Dalman C, Newschaffer C, Lewis G, Magnusson C. Antidepressants during pregnancy and autism in offspring: population based cohort study. BMJ. 2017;358(8900488, bmj, 101090866):j2811.
  92. 92. Reebye PN, Morison SJ, Panikkar H, Misri S, Grunau RE. Affect expression in prenatally psychotropic exposed and nonexposed mother-infant dyads. Infant Ment Health J. 2002 Jul;23(4):403–16.
  93. 93. Santucci AK, Singer LT, Wisniewski SR, Luther JF, Eng HF, Dills JL, et al. Impact of prenatal exposure to serotonin reuptake inhibitors or maternal major depressive disorder on infant developmental outcomes. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014;75(10):1088–95. pmid:25373117
  94. 94. Schechter JC, Brennan PA, Smith AK, Stowe ZN, Newport DJ, Johnson KC. Maternal Prenatal Psychological Distress and Preschool Cognitive Functioning: the Protective Role of Positive Parental Engagement. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017 Feb;45(2):249–60. pmid:27150387
  95. 95. Simon GE, Cunningham ML, Davis RL. Outcomes of prenatal antidepressant exposure. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(12):2055–61. pmid:12450956
  96. 96. Skurtveit S, Selmer R, Roth C, Hernandez-Diaz S, Handal M. Prenatal exposure to antidepressants and language competence at age three: results from a large population-based pregnancy cohort in Norway. BJOG Int J Obstet Gynaecol. 2014 Dec;121(13):1621–31.
  97. 97. Sorensen MJ, Gronborg TK, Christensen J, Parner ET, Vestergaard M, Schendel D, et al. Antidepressant exposure in pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders. Clin Epidemiol. 2013;5(101531700):449–59.
  98. 98. Sujan AC, Rickert ME, Oberg S, Quinn PD, Hernandez-Diaz S, Almqvist C, et al. Associations of Maternal Antidepressant Use During the First Trimester of Pregnancy With Preterm Birth, Small for Gestational Age, Autism Spectrum Disorder, and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Offspring. Jama-J Am Med Assoc. 2017 Apr 18;317(15):1553–62.
  99. 99. Suri R, Hellemann G, Stowe ZN, Cohen LS, Aquino A, Altshuler LL. A prospective, naturalistic, blinded study of early neurobehavioral outcomes for infants following prenatal antidepressant exposure. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72(7):1002–7. pmid:21672498
  100. 100. Viktorin A, Uher R, Kolevzon A, Reichenberg A, Levine SZ, Sandin S. Association of Antidepressant Medication Use During Pregnancy With Intellectual Disability in Offspring. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(10):1031–8. pmid:28700807
  101. 101. Weikum WM, Mayes LC, Grunau RE, Brain U, Oberlander TF. The impact of prenatal serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SRI) antidepressant exposure and maternal mood on mother-infant interactions at 3 months of age. Infant Behav Dev. 2013;36(4):485–93. pmid:23728194
  102. 102. Weikum WM, Brain U, Chau CMY, Grunau RE, Boyce WT, Diamond A, et al. Prenatal serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SRI) antidepressant exposure and serotonin transporter promoter genotype (SLC6A4) influence executive functions at 6 years of age. Front Cell Neurosci. 2013;7(101477935):180.
  103. 103. Wibroe MA, Mathiasen R, Pagsberg AK, Uldall P. Risk of impaired cognition after prenatal exposure to psychotropic drugs. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017;136(2):177–87. pmid:28561934
  104. 104. Brandlistuen RE, Ystrom E, Hernandez-Diaz S, Skurtveit S, Selmer R, Handal M, et al. Association of prenatal exposure to benzodiazepines and child internalizing problems: A sibling-controlled cohort study. PloS One. 2017;12(7):e0181042. pmid:28746341
  105. 105. Gidai J, Acs N, Banhidy F, Czeizel AE. An evaluation of data for 10 children born to mothers who attempted suicide by taking large doses of alprazolam during pregnancy. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008;24(1–2):53–60. pmid:18818181
  106. 106. Gidai J, Acs N, Banhidy F, Czeizel AE. A study of the effects of large doses of medazepam used for self-poisoning in 10 pregnant women on fetal development. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008;24(1–2):61–8. pmid:18818182
  107. 107. Gidai J, Ács N, Bánhidy F, Czeizel A. A study of the teratogenic and fetotoxic effects of large doses of chlordiazepoxide used for self-poisoning by 35 pregnant women. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008 Feb;24(1–2):41–51. pmid:18818180
  108. 108. Laegreid L, Hagberg G, Lundberg A. Neurodevelopment in late infancy after prenatal exposure to benzodiazepines—a prospective study. Neuropediatrics. 1992;23(2):60–7. pmid:1351263
  109. 109. Lupattelli A, Chambers CD, Bandoli G, Handal M, Skurtveit S, Nordeng H. Association of Maternal Use of Benzodiazepines and Z-Hypnotics During Pregnancy With Motor and Communication Skills and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms in Preschoolers. JAMA Netw Open. 2019 Apr 5;2(4):e191435. pmid:30951155
  110. 110. Odsbu I, Skurtveit S, Selmer R, Roth C, Hernandez-Diaz S, Handal M. Prenatal exposure to anxiolytics and hypnotics and language competence at 3 years of age. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2015;71(3):283–91. pmid:25547568
  111. 111. Radojcic MR, El Marroun H, Miljkovic B, Stricker BHC, Jaddoe VWV, Verhulst FC, et al. Prenatal exposure to anxiolytic and hypnotic medication in relation to behavioral problems in childhood: A population-based cohort study. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2017 Jun;61:58–65. pmid:28259732
  112. 112. Timmermann G, Acs N, Banhidy F, Czeizel AE. A study of teratogenic and fetotoxic effects of large doses of meprobamate used for a suicide attempt by 42 pregnant women. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008;24(1–2):97–107. pmid:18818186
  113. 113. Peng M, Gao K, Ding Y, Ou J, Calabrese JR, Wu R, et al. Effects of prenatal exposure to atypical antipsychotics on postnatal development and growth of infants: a case-controlled, prospective study. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;228(4):577–84.
  114. 114. Platt JE, Friedhoff AJ, Broman SH, Bond R, Laska E, Lin SP. Effects of prenatal neuroleptic drug exposure on motor performance in children. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp. 1989 Sep;4(3):205–13.
  115. 115. Petik D, Ács N, Bánhidy F, Czeizel A. A study of the effects of large doses of glutethimide that were used for self-poisoning during pregnancy on human fetuses. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008 Feb;24(1–2):69–78. pmid:18818183
  116. 116. Petik D, Timmermann G, Czeizel AE, Acs N, Banhidy F. A study of the teratogenic and fetotoxic effects of large doses of amobarbital used for a suicide attempt by 14 pregnant women. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008;24(1–2):79–85. pmid:18818184
  117. 117. Timmermann G, Czeizel A, Bánhidy F, Ács N. A study of the teratogenic and fetotoxic effects of large doses of barbital, hexobarbital and butobarbital used for suicide attempts by pregnant women. Toxicol Ind Health. 2008 Feb;24(1–2):109–19. pmid:18818187
  118. 118. Avella-Garcia CB, Julvez J, Fortuny J, Rebordosa C, Garcia-Esteban R, Galan IR, et al. Acetaminophen use in pregnancy and neurodevelopment: attention function and autism spectrum symptoms. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(6):1987–96. pmid:27353198
  119. 119. Bornehag C-G, Reichenberg A, Hallerback MU, Wikstrom S, Koch HM, Jonsson BA, et al. Prenatal exposure to acetaminophen and children’s language development at 30 months. Eur Psychiatry. 2018 Jun;51:98–103. pmid:29331486
  120. 120. Brandlistuen RE, Ystrom E, Nulman I, Koren G, Nordeng H. Prenatal paracetamol exposure and child neurodevelopment: a sibling-controlled cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2013 Dec 1;42(6):1702–13. pmid:24163279
  121. 121. Laue HE, Cassoulet R, Abdelouahab N, Serme-Gbedo YK, Desautels A-S, Brennan KJM, et al. Association Between Meconium Acetaminophen and Childhood Neurocognitive Development in GESTE, a Canadian Cohort Study. Toxicol Sci. 2019 Jan;167(1):138–44. pmid:30202886
  122. 122. Liew Z, Ritz B, Rebordosa C, Lee P-C, Olsen J. Acetaminophen use during pregnancy, behavioral problems, and hyperkinetic disorders. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(4):313–20. pmid:24566677
  123. 123. Liew Z, Ritz B, Virk J, Olsen J. Maternal use of acetaminophen during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders in childhood: A Danish national birth cohort study: Acetaminophen and Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism Res. 2016 Sep;9(9):951–8. pmid:26688372
  124. 124. Liew Z, Bach CC, Asarnow RF, Ritz B, Olsen J. Paracetamol use during pregnancy and attention and executive function in offspring at age 5 years. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(6):2009–17. pmid:28031314
  125. 125. Liew Z, Ritz B, Virk J, Arah OA, Olsen J. Prenatal Use of Acetaminophen and Child IQ: A Danish Cohort Study. Epidemiology. 2016 Nov;27(6):912–8. pmid:27479646
  126. 126. Liew Z, Kioumourtzoglou M-A, Roberts AL, O’Reilly EJ, Ascherio A, Weisskopf MG. Use of Negative Control Exposure Analysis to Evaluate Confounding: An Example of Acetaminophen Exposure and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Nurses’ Health Study II. Am J Epidemiol. 2019 Apr;188(4):768–75. pmid:30923825
  127. 127. Ruisch IH, Buitelaar JK, Glennon JC, Hoekstra PJ, Dietrich A. Pregnancy risk factors in relation to oppositional-defiant and conduct disorder symptoms in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. J Psychiatr Res. 2018 Jun;101:63–71. pmid:29550610
  128. 128. Skovlund E, Handal M, Selmer R, Brandlistuen RE, Skurtveit S. Language competence and communication skills in 3-year-old children after prenatal exposure to analgesic opioids. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2017;26(6):625–34. pmid:28168770
  129. 129. Stergiakouli E, Thapar A, Davey Smith G. Association of Acetaminophen Use During Pregnancy With Behavioral Problems in Childhood: Evidence Against Confounding. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(10):964–70. pmid:27533796
  130. 130. Thompson JMD, Waldie KE, Wall CR, Murphy R, Mitchell EA, ABC study group. Associations between acetaminophen use during pregnancy and ADHD symptoms measured at ages 7 and 11 years. Robinson E BD Wild C, editor. PloS One. 2014;9(9):e108210. pmid:25251831
  131. 131. Tovo-Rodrigues L, Schneider BC, Martins-Silva T, Del-Ponte B, de Mola CL, Schuler-Faccini L, et al. Is intrauterine exposure to acetaminophen associated with emotional and hyperactivity problems during childhood? Findings from the 2004 Pelotas birth cohort. Bmc Psychiatry. 2018 Nov 20;18:368. pmid:30458756
  132. 132. Ystrom E, Gustavson K, Brandlistuen RE, Knudsen GP, Magnus P, Susser E, et al. Prenatal Exposure to Acetaminophen and Risk of ADHD. Pediatrics. 2017 Nov;140(5):e20163840. pmid:29084830
  133. 133. al-Alaiyan S, Seshia MM, Casiro OG. Neurodevelopmental outcome of infants exposed to indomethacin antenatally. J Perinat Med. 1996;24(4):405–11. pmid:8880639
  134. 134. Amin SB, Kamaluddeen M, Sangem M. Neurodevelopmental outcome of premature infants after exposure to antenatal indomethacin. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2008;199(1):41.e1–8.
  135. 135. Markovic M, Swanson SA, Stricker BH, Jaddoe VWV, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H, et al. Prenatal exposure to non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) and neurodevelopmental outcomes in children. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2019 Apr;28(4):452–9. pmid:30838712
  136. 136. Salokorpi T, Eronen M, von Wendt L. Growth and development until 18 months of children exposed to tocolytics indomethacin or nylidrin. Neuropediatrics. 1996;27(4):174–7. pmid:8892364
  137. 137. Barr HM, Streissguth AP, Darby BL, Sampson PD. Prenatal exposure to alcohol, caffeine, tobacco, and aspirin: Effects on fine and gross motor performance in 4-year-old children. Dev Psychol. 1990;26(3):339–48.
  138. 138. Klebanoff MA, Berendes HW. Aspirin exposure during the first 20 weeks of gestation and IQ at four years of age. Teratology. 1988;37(3):249–55. pmid:3368878
  139. 139. Harris G-ME, Wood M, Ystrom E, Nordeng H. Prenatal triptan exposure and neurodevelopmental outcomes in 5-year-old children: Follow-up from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2018 May;32(3):247–55. pmid:29569251
  140. 140. Wood ME, Frazier JA, Nordeng HME, Lapane KL. Longitudinal changes in neurodevelopmental outcomes between 18 and 36 months in children with prenatal triptan exposure: findings from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(9):e011971. pmid:27625061
  141. 141. Wood ME, Lapane K, Frazier JA, Ystrom E, Mick EO, Nordeng H. Prenatal Triptan Exposure and Internalising and Externalising Behaviour Problems in 3-Year-Old Children: Results from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study: Prenatal triptan exposure and externalising symptoms. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2016 Mar;30(2):190–200. pmid:26525300
  142. 142. Wood ME, Frazier JA, Nordeng HME, Lapane KL. Prenatal triptan exposure and parent-reported early childhood neurodevelopmental outcomes: an application of propensity score calibration to adjust for unmeasured confounding by migraine severity. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2016;25(5):493–502. pmid:26554750
  143. 143. Rubenstein E, Young JC, Croen LA, DiGuiseppi C, Dowling NF, Lee L-C, et al. Brief Report: Maternal Opioid Prescription from Preconception Through Pregnancy and the Odds of Autism Spectrum Disorder and Autism Features in Children. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019 Jan;49(1):376–82. pmid:30132098
  144. 144. Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management. ADHD: Clinical Practice Guideline for the Diagnosis, Evaluation, and Treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adolescents. PEDIATRICS. 2011 Nov 1;128(5):1007–22. pmid:22003063
  145. 145. Tomson T, Battino D, French J, Harden C, Holmes L, Morrow J, et al. Antiepileptic drug exposure and major congenital malformations: The role of pregnancy registries. Epilepsy Behav. 2007 Nov;11(3):277–82. pmid:17996635
  146. 146. Rodier PM, Ingram JL, Tisdale B, Nelson S, Romano J. Embryological origin for autism: developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 1996 Jun 24;370(2):247–61. pmid:8808733
  147. 147. Müller JM, Furniss T. Correction of distortions in distressed mothers’ ratings of their preschool children’s psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 2013 Nov;210(1):294–301. pmid:23648281
  148. 148. Najman JM, Williams GM, Nikles J, Spence S, Bor W, O’Callaghan M, et al. Bias influencing maternal reports of child behaviour and emotional state. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2001 Apr;36(4):186–94. pmid:11518032
  149. 149. van der Toorn SLM, Huizink AC, Utens EMWJ, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, Ferdinand RF. Maternal depressive symptoms, and not anxiety symptoms, are associated with positive mother-child reporting discrepancies of internalizing problems in children: a report on the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010 Apr;19(4):379–88. pmid:19823897
  150. 150. Sawyer MG, Streiner DL, Baghurst P. The influence of distress on mothers’ and fathers’ reports of childhood emotional and behavioral problems. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1998 Dec;26(6):407–14. pmid:9915648
  151. 151. Polanczyk GV, Willcutt EG, Salum GA, Kieling C, Rohde LA. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2014 Apr;43(2):434–42. pmid:24464188
  152. 152. Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT. Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychol Bull. 1987;101(2):213–32. pmid:3562706
  153. 153. Rasalam A, Hailey H, Williams J, Moore S, Turnpenny P, Lloyd D, et al. Characteristics of fetal anticonvulsant syndrome associated autistic disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2005 Jul 14;47(8):551–5. pmid:16108456
  154. 154. Pinsky L. Informatic Morphogenetic Variants. In: Issue and reviews in teratology: 3. New York: Plenum Press; 1985. p. 135–70.
  155. 155. Bateman BT, Hernandez-Diaz S, Fischer MA, Seely EW, Ecker JL, Franklin JM, et al. Statins and congenital malformations: cohort study. BMJ. 2015 Mar 17;350(mar17 10):h1035–h1035.
  156. 156. Morales DR, Slattery J, Evans S, Kurz X. Antidepressant use during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: systematic review of observational studies and methodological considerations. BMC Med. 2018 Dec;16(1).
  157. 157. Mezzacappa A, Lasica P-A, Gianfagna F, Cazas O, Hardy P, Falissard B, et al. Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorders According to Period of Prenatal Antidepressant Exposure: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. [Review]. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(6):555–63. pmid:28418571
  158. 158. Kobayashi T, Matsuyama T, Takeuchi M, Ito S. Autism spectrum disorder and prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Reprod Toxicol Elmsford N. 2016;65(be4, 8803591):170–8.
  159. 159. Hermansen TK, Melinder A. Prenatal SSRI exposure: Effects on later child development. Child Neuropsychol. 2015 Sep 3;21(5):543–69. pmid:25089614
  160. 160. Nagin DS, Odgers CL. Group-Based Trajectory Modeling in Clinical Research. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010 Mar;6(1):109–38.
  161. 161. Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Construct validity and external validity. In: Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 2001. p. 64–102.
  162. 162. Regier DA, Kaelber CT, Roper MT, Rae DS, Sartorius N. The ICD-10 clinical field trial for mental and behavioral disorders: results in Canada and the United States. Am J Psychiatry. 1994 Sep;151(9):1340–50. pmid:8067491
  163. 163. Brown T, Lalor A. The Movement Assessment Battery for Children—Second Edition (MABC-2): A Review and Critique. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 2009 Jan;29(1):86–103. pmid:19197761
  164. 164. Ananth CV, VanderWeele TJ. Placental abruption and perinatal mortality with preterm delivery as a mediator: disentangling direct and indirect effects. Am J Epidemiol. 2011 Jul 1;174(1):99–108. pmid:21430195
  165. 165. Grzeskowiak LE, Gilbert AL, Morrison JL. Investigating outcomes associated with medication use during pregnancy: A review of methodological challenges and observational study designs. Reprod Toxicol. 2012 Jun;33(3):280–9. pmid:22329969
  166. 166. Wang Z, Ho PWH, Choy MTH, Wong ICK, Brauer R, Man KKC. Advances in Epidemiological Methods and Utilisation of Large Databases: A Methodological Review of Observational Studies on Central Nervous System Drug Use in Pregnancy and Central Nervous System Outcomes in Children. Drug Saf. 2018 Nov 13.