Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does viral load alter behavior of the bee parasite Varroa destructor?

  • Carl Giuffre ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Carl Giuffre, Sharon R. Lubkin, David R. Tarpy

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    cjgiuffre@smcm.edu

    Affiliation Department of Mathematics and Computer Sciences, St. Mary’s College of Southern Maryland, St. Mary’s City, Maryland, United States of America

  • Sharon R. Lubkin ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Carl Giuffre, Sharon R. Lubkin, David R. Tarpy

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mathematics, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, North Carolina, United States of America

  • David R. Tarpy

    Contributed equally to this work with: Carl Giuffre, Sharon R. Lubkin, David R. Tarpy

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Entomology and Plant Pathology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, North Carolina, United States of America, W.M. Keck Center for Behavioral Biology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, North Carolina, United States of America

Abstract

The invasive mite Varroa destructor has negatively impacted global apiculture, by being a vector for many viruses of the honey bee (Apis mellifera). Until now, most studies have been limited to varroa-honey bee or virus-honey bee interactions. The aim of this study is to bridge the important research gap of varroa-virus interactions by correlating varroa behavior with viral load. Ten-minute video recordings of 200 varroa mites were analyzed, and average speeds of the mites were compared to individual qPCR viral loads for deformed wing virus (DWV) and sacbrood virus (SBV). Statistically significant models reveal that colony, DWV, and SBV all might play a role in mite behavior, suggesting that the varroa-virus interaction needs to be an integral part of future studies on honey bee pathogens.

Introduction

The invasive mite Varroa destructor has negatively impacted apiculture worldwide [1]. Varroa experienced an evolutionary host-shift from the Asian honey bee (Apis cerana) to the European honey bee (Apis mellifera) as early as 1960 and has been strongly implicated for playing a role in Colony Collapse Disorder and reduced health of bees in general [1,2]. These ectoparasites go through two major phases in their life cycle—the reproductive and phoretic stages. During the reproductive stage, a single varroa female infests the cell of an immature honey bee (pupa), feeding on the hemolymph of the developing bee. In doing so, the parasite can directly vector several viruses within honey bee colonies [24]. During the phoretic stage, varroa mites emerge with the enclosed bee and continue to feed on adult honey bee hemolymph for sustenance [2] and continue to spread viral pathogens horizontally among nestmates [5]. Honey bee colonies can exceed 50,000 bees, with only one female reproductive (the queen) [2]. When a queen is infected with viral pathogens, the health of the entire colony can be compromised as she then has the potential to vertically transmit virus to her offspring through oviposition [5].

The Vector Manipulation Hypothesis suggests that pathogens can modify the motility behavior or host preference for vector organisms, aiding in the spread of pathogens to the target hosts [5]. There are many known examples in arthropods of viruses or parasites altering vector behavior [57]. The Vector Manipulation Hypothesis supports the possibility that infected mite vectors could exhibit higher motility than uninfected individuals. Such behavioral modifications could determine the success or failure for certain viruses to spread within and among honey bee colonies, and it could have significant consequences for global bee health and the means to mitigate disease.

The study of varroa is important for the future success of honey bee management. The introduction of varroa to the United States has heavily impacted colony health, honey bee economics, and integrated pest management [1,8,9]. The mode of action for many pesticides is similar for both insects and arachnids, both belonging to the same phylum, Arthropoda [10]. Thus, treating for varroa can have negative implications on honey bee colony health, even leading to mortality within or of the colony [10]. On the other hand, mites themselves impact colony health by vectoring pathogens [11]. This tradeoff forces beekeepers to make difficult decisions, and optimal varroa treatment strategy is not always clear.

As a further complication, varroa have developed resistance to common acaricide treatments [8,9], which has influenced modern studies emphasizing behavioral treatments over chemical ones. Since the introduction of varroa to honey bees has presumably modified bee behavior, most previous studies focus on how honey bees behave toward varroa or bee grooming [1214]. Most of these assays examine some indirect artifact of honey bee behavior, such as the freeze-brood and sticky-board assays. One assay more directly measured the ability for a honey bee to bite and damage the exoskeleton or legs of varroa, rendering them dead or useless [15].

More than 18 honey bee viruses have been identified, with six major viruses at the center of global scientific interest: deformed wing virus, sacbrood virus, black queen cell virus, Kashmir bee virus, acute bee paralysis virus, and chronic bee paralysis virus [3,16,17]. We here focus on two of the more common and economically important viruses in honey bees: deformed wing virus (DWV) and sacbrood virus (SBV). DWV has been of scientific interest because of its connection with varroa transmission. Although DWV can be spread to larvae by vectored mites, physiological differences in infected individuals are not apparent until the adult stage of the honey bee [1619]. The host bee pupates and often develops with deformed wings, rendering adults unable to contribute to foraging duties in the colony [16]. SBV targets the brood cycle of honey bees, preventing a brood from pupating and thus resulting in larval death. Both these viruses have been found in varroa, though it appears varroa are only vectors for DWV and not SBV [3,16]. The low mortality and virulence of DWV benefits both virus prevalence and varroa dispersal, whereas SBV increases the chance of varroa mortality alongside the dead honey bee brood [20]. The aim of this study is to test the Vector Manipulation Hypothesis by correlating mite behavioral phenotypes to their viral status, identifying the role this interaction plays in the entire honey bee system.

Methods

Mite collection

Mites were collected at the Lake Wheeler Honey Bee Research facility in Raleigh, NC. Once varroa infestations were identified in a colony, mites were gathered using the sugar-shake method, a process that safely dislodges live mites off their honey bee hosts [21]. The mites were subsequently gently rinsed in phosphate-buffered solution, removing excess sugar from their exoskeleton [21]. Mites were then placed in a 60 mm-diameter petri dish, creating a small arena for the mite to explore over the course of the experiment. Fifty mites were gathered from each of four unrelated colonies, yielding 200 experimental subjects.

Video recording

All recordings were taken in a dark room with one Sony Handycam mounted approximately 27 centimeters above the dishes containing the mites (Fig 1). Two utility light clamps (Coleman) were installed with 10W (60W-equivalent) LED light bulbs (Lighting Science) and placed on the perimeter of the recording arena to provide uniform lighting. Beneath the dishes was a sheet of white paper. Individual mites were recorded for 10 minutes at 30 frames per second with 1 minute of buffering at the beginning of each video, for a total recording time of 11 minutes. A total of 40 videos were recorded over the span of three days, with ten dishes per recording.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental setup.

A camera was mounted 27 cm directly above a collection of 10 petri dishes, with one varroa mite per dish. Two utility lamps were mounted in a square surrounding the recording area, angled to minimize glare off the dish and maximize illumination of the recording area (figure not to scale). Once the footage was recorded, mites were individually transferred to micropipette tubes and placed in a -80°C freezer for subsequent analysis of viral loads.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217975.g001

Quantitative PCR

RNA was extracted from individual varroa mites [3] using the BioBasic EZ-10 Spin Column Total RNA Mini-Preps Kit, resuspended in water, and tested for β-actin and Apo28s as reference genes [22] and for the same viral targets via real-time PCR on the same machine using KAPA SYBR FAST One-Step qRT-PCR ABIPrism Kit. The reaction mix contained 2.5 μL of SYBR, 0.25 μL of primer, 0.2 μL of KAPA RT Mix, 1.25 μL of water and 1μL of sample for a final reaction volume of 5.2 μL. The qPCR program ran at 42°C for 5 min, 52°C for 3 min, 95°C for 3 min, then cycled 40 times through 95°C for 3 sec, 58°C for 25 sec, and 72°C for 1 sec, then performed a melt curve step. Results were verified by melt curve temperature and normalized [23] via GeNorm.

Video analysis

Videos were processed using a custom algorithm written in MATLAB R2105b (The Math Works Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States). This algorithm follows four steps, requiring minimal user intervention only on the first step: (1) Dish partitioning, (2) Frame extraction and thresholding, (3) Centroid calculation, and (4) Metric calculation. Steps 1–3 of the algorithm are visualized in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Visualization of the steps for the mite detection algorithm.

For each video frame, (A) Petri dish edges were determined then (B) the images were converted to grayscale and binary images were produced using a threshold value of 0.255 on a 0–1 scale. Finally, the convex hull of the mite was used to compute a centroid in the Cartesian plane.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217975.g002

Because multiple dishes were filmed simultaneously, individual dishes had to be identified and segmented from the videos. The manual partitioning step forces the user to define boundaries on each dish using the first frame from the video and the MATLAB R2015b Image Processing Toolbox command imcircle. This step serves a dual purpose in the context of this experiment. First, it enables the centroids calculated in step 3 to be joined and identified as belonging to the same varroa mite, then centered at the origin, irrespective of where the dish was placed in the recording area. Second, it allows for quick conversion between pixel and metric data, using the diameter of the petri dish as a scale.

Video frames were converted to grayscale. ImageJ (National Institutes of Health, Bethesda MD–USA) was used to estimate the proper binary thresholding value for the mites (65 of 255 or ≈ 0.26, 255 = white), which was applied uniformly across every image sequence. It was important to ensure that the thresholding value was low enough to distinguish the mite from the background, but high enough to avoid tracking shadows cast by mites or dish edges. The result was a discrete sequence of images containing an isolated cluster of pixels, representative of a single varroa mite. On occasion, pixel values were misinterpreted in the thresholding step. The MATLAB command regionprops (The Math Works Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States) computed the convex hull for candidate clusters of pixels that may have been the mite. If the cluster size was lower than the expected pixel area of a mite, it was eliminated as background noise. The centroid of the remaining pixel cluster was then interpreted as the Euclidean coordinate location of the mite, which was used to determine behavioral parameters that might be of interest.

Standard measurements—such as velocity and average speed—are useful in analyzing complete, continuous, and smooth data. However, recorded mite tracks appeared to be discontinuous at some points for the following reason. Varroa are extremely flat organisms, and therefore they appear as an ellipse when viewed dorsally but only as a sliver when viewed laterally. Since any mite can be hard to detect if it turns edge-on, careful consideration had to be made regarding how behavioral metrics were calculated in the presence of discontinuities in mite tracks. Furthermore, centroid calculation can introduce small perturbations in coordinate data. These perturbations improperly skewed typical rate-of-change calculations such as instantaneous speed and velocity.

To compensate, a surrogate metric for average speed was calculated in step 4 of the processing algorithm. Because the mites are approximately 1 mm in width, dividing the total track area covered by a mite per unit time by the mite width yields a reasonable estimate of the mite average speed. Each dish was gridded with 1 mm2 squares and treated as a binary 60 by 60 “visitation matrix” (Fig 3). If a mite centroid was found in any given grid square, it was assigned a value of one, otherwise zero. The total area traveled, in square millimeters, could then be divided by the amount of time the mite was successfully tracked, resulting in the following estimate of average speed S: (1)

thumbnail
Fig 3. Cartoon for average speed metric.

The petri dish image space was gridded with 1 mm by 1 mm squares, corresponding to the typical width of a varroa mite. Grid positions containing a mite centroid are assigned a value of 1, otherwise 0. Average speed is calculated from the fraction of positions filled divided by the recording time.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217975.g003

All data files used in this study, including raw video footage, qPCR, and mite metric data, are freely available, and can be found at https://osf.io/ehdr3/.

Results

The behavioral and qPCR data were statistically analyzed using JMP Pro 11.0 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). Mites were tested for a suite of seven common honey bee viruses [24] although the collected mites only tested positive for DWV and SBV. Of the 200 total mites, 194 remained for statistical analysis after accounting for various technical errors or small representation in categories (Table 1). Of those 194, whose tracks are displayed in Fig 4A–4C, 120 tested negative for infection (Fig 4A). Colonies 1 and 2 were primarily uninfected, with only two mites from each colony testing positive for both DWV and SBV. In contrast, colonies 3 and 4 displayed a variety of infection patterns: uninfected (Fig 4A), infected with DWV only (Fig 4B), and infected with both DWV and SBV (Fig 4C).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Visualization of mite tracks.

Mite tracks grouped by colony and viral status. (A) Uninfected mites grouped by colony and sorted by average speed. (B) Mites only infected by DWV, grouped by colony and sorted by average speed. (C) Mites infected with both DWV and SBV, sorted by SBV viron count.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217975.g004

Of 200 mites, 6 were excluded from statistical analysis: 1 mite was only infected by SBV, 3 mites were not analyzed by qPCR, and 2 mites could not be tracked for any portion of the ten-minute video. Infection types for the remaining 194 mites (uninfected/DWV-only/both) are also provided.

We tested a variety of statistical models using the input variables {colony ID, kDWV, SBV} to predict the response variable AS (average speed, mm/min, determined from video analysis). SBV were in units of virions, and kDWV were in units of kilovirions. The general form of the statistical model used was (2) Where α, β, γ, δ, θ, D0, and S0 are free fitting parameters in the appropriate units. This general model form includes model terms for intercept, colony ID (C), kDWV (D), SBV (S), and a nonlinear interaction (N) between viruses. We combinatorially tested models containing or omitting each of these model terms, e.g. Model CD contains only the intercept, colony, and kDWV dependence (thus setting parameters β, δ, and θ to 0). Additionally, we tested models with nonlinear dependence on kDWV or SBV (not shown). In general, models were rejected at a level of α = 0.05. Parameter fits for the simplest models are shown in Fig 5. Additional rejected models are not shown.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Statistical models.

Models were combinatorially tested across multiple levels. The leading row of the table provides quick identification for the model and parameters of interest for that model (i.e., Model CD uses parameters α, β, and γ). Poor parameter estimates are highlighted (pink). AICc and BIC metrics were normalized according to the expression ΔIC = IC/max(IC)– 1, then color-coded on a scale from 0 (green, strong information criterion model selection) to max(ΔIC) (red, unfavorable information criterion model selection) so that model selection could be quickly verified. Models with ΔIC = 0 are boldfaced. Additional rejected models (p > .05) not shown.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217975.g005

Colony effects are not the major effects of interest in this study. However, it was important to establish whether colony-level effects were present before building up more complicated models. Model C was accepted with a p-value < 0.01 (numerator d.f. = 3), indicating that the colony of origin influences average speed of the mite.

The main effects of viral loads (kDWV, SBV) on average speed were explored in Models D and S, ignoring colony-level effects. Model D (p = 0.66, d.f. = 1) and Model S (p = 0.82, d.f. = 1) were both rejected. However, addition of the nonlinear viral interaction yielded Model DSN, which was accepted with a p-value of 0.04 (d.f. = 3). Finally, Model CDS (p = 0.02, d.f. = 5) and Model CDSN (p < 0.01, d.f. = 6) were both accepted.

Selection of appropriate and parsimonious statistical models was done using Bayesian information criterion (BIC) and corrected Akaike information criterion (AICc), two philosophically different approaches for model selection. Both metrics attempt to maximize goodness-of-fit, while minimizing the number of parameters used to avoid overfitting the data. Model C had the smallest BIC, and Model CSN had the smallest AICc. BIC and AICc are reported in Fig 5.

Discussion

BIC favors selection of Model C (smallest BIC), which suggests that mite viral load seems to be unrelated to mite behavior. At the colony level, phoretic mites may modify their behavior based on environmental conditions, such as bee population, brood availability, hive temperature, or even bee grooming. Although none of these variables were measured in the current study, environmental conditions clearly play a direct role in virus’ ability to spread, by impacting vector motility. It could be argued that colonies have different viral profiles responsible for these global effects. Unfortunately, four colonies were not sufficient to determine such effects.

AICc selects Model CSN, which also has the lowest p-value. The estimates of γ (mm/min/kDWV) have high variability between models, are nearly centered at zero, and have high p-values, bringing to question the kind of impact DWV has on mite behavior. However, the selection of statistical models that include cross-terms without also including the related linear terms has been a topic of debate in the statistical community [25].

The most complicated Model CDSN also performs very well, but its AICc is higher than Model CSN's AICc. Nonetheless, Model CDSN is of interest, because while DWV is known to replicate in varroa, SBV is not [3,16]. SBV should not be modifying varroa RNA, and therefore, should have no interactive relationship with their behavior [3]. One possible explanation for this might be that SBV plays a more important role in colony-level effects, which indirectly changes their behavior, however the Model CS(C*S) with colony and SBV crossed was rejected at level α = 0.05. Recent studies show that viruses can interact with one another in nontrivial ways, including altered host susceptibility due to a breakdown in physical barriers in both plants and animals, which may be true for vectors, as well [2628]. Although SBV does not modify varroa RNA, it could be that SBV-bearing mites are more susceptible to effects from DWV, or that SBV replication occurs in the presence of DWV, but this possibility will require further study. Our data indicate that the DWV-SBV interaction generally causes mites to move more slowly. SBV seems is correlated with faster mite movement, except in Models S, CS and CDS, where the p-values for δ (mm/min/SBV) estimation are high (Fig 5).

Many questions remain, such as how or why DWV and SBV interact. This study had mite selection and tracking performed blind (a priori) to the types and levels of infection: it was possible that none of the mites were infected, or that they had a suite of viruses with even more complicated levels of cross-infection. The limited results of this study suggest the need for a more controlled experiment, where varroa are inoculated with specific virus levels and types, or where varroa are gathered from more than four colonies over a longer timeframe, with greater knowledge of colony phenotypes or environments.

Varroa mites not only vector honey bee-specific viruses, but they also carry their own viruses that do not infect A. mellifera [2931]. The role that these varroa-specific viruses play on varroa movement and behavior is still unknown, and how they potentially interact with economically important bee viruses remains unclear. Both of these avenues warrant further empirical research and are intriguing to fully understand varroa as a noxious parasite of honey bee colonies worldwide. However, because varroa-specific pathogens would not be selectively favored to alter the behavior of their vectors to increase infection rates of their main host, they are unlikely to have significant bearing on the Vector Manipulation Hypothesis, and thus, were not measured as part of this preliminary study in lieu of focusing on the honey bee-specific viruses (DWV and SBV). Based on our preliminary findings here, it is clear that additional research is warranted both in vitro as well as in vivo to focus on possible behavioral changes of varroa mites as pathogenic vectors.

This assay is a novel attempt at exploring varroa behavior, in addition to tying results to the Vector Manipulation Hypothesis. The AICc values for all models suggest that viral loads seem to play an important role in how varroa explore their surroundings, supporting this hypothesis and akin to findings in other areas of entomology [5,6]. Varroa behavior should be studied more for the role that it plays in the host-vector-disease interaction, and this study provides a framework for such future studies.

The bioassay developed in this study attempts to facilitate data gathering by making it high-throughput and easy-to-track. It could be modified, e.g. by using particle image velocimetry, or restricting the mite to the upper or lower surface of the dish. Moreover, this study opens the door for a host-preference study on varroa. Honey bee pheromones are notably vital for communication in the dark environment of a hive [2]. Using a similar experimental design, one could test viral effects on host preference for A. mellifera drones, versus workers, versus A. cerana. Perhaps mite viral ethology could better explain the evolutionary jump from A. cerana to A. mellifera, or further support it. Testing and measuring mite behavior in vitro may help untangle the more complicated vector-disease dynamics of the hive.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Jennifer Keller for her dedicated work at the Lake Wheeler honey bee facility, Paulina Spencer for her contributions in data collection, and Alex Cruz for his art contribution. This project was supported by USDA-APHIS grant number 14-8130-0360-CA, as well as a grant from the NC Department of Agriculture & Consumer Services, awarded to DRT.

References

  1. 1. Oldroyd BP. What's killing American honey bees?. PLoS biology. 2007 Jun 12;5(6):e168. pmid:17564497
  2. 2. Winston ML. The biology of the honey bee. Harvard University Press; 1991.
  3. 3. Rosenkranz P, Aumeier P, Ziegelmann B. Biology and control of Varroa destructor. Journal of invertebrate pathology. 2010 Jan 1;103:S96–119. pmid:19909970
  4. 4. de Miranda JR, Bailey L, Ball BV, Blanchard P, Budge GE, Chejanovsky N, et al. Standard methods for virus research in Apis mellifera. Journal of apicultural research. 2013 Jan 1;52(4):1–56.
  5. 5. Chen Y, Evans J, Feldlaufer M. Horizontal and vertical transmission of viruses in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Journal of invertebrate pathology. 2006 Jul 1;92(3):152–9. pmid:16793058
  6. 6. Ingwell LL, Eigenbrode SD, Bosque-Pérez NA. Plant viruses alter insect behavior to enhance their spread. Scientific reports. 2012 Aug 15;2:578. pmid:22896811
  7. 7. Cator LJ, Lynch PA, Read AF, Thomas MB. Do malaria parasites manipulate mosquitoes?. Trends in Parasitology. 2012 Nov 30;28(11):466–70. pmid:23044288
  8. 8. Spreafico M, Eördegh F, Bernardinelli I, Colombo M. First detection of strains of Varroa destructor resistant to coumaphos. Results of laboratory tests and field trials. Apidologie. 2001;32(1):49–55.
  9. 9. Pettis JS. A scientific note on Varroa destructor resistance to coumaphos in the United States. Apidologie. 2004 Jan 1;35(1):91–2.
  10. 10. Thrasyvoulou AT, Pappas N. Contamination of honey and wax with malathion and coumaphos used against the Varroa mite. Journal of Apicultural Research. 1988 Jan 1;27(1):55–61.
  11. 11. Tentcheva D, Gauthier L, Zappulla N, Dainat B, Cousserans F, Colin ME, et al. Prevalence and seasonal variations of six bee viruses in Apis mellifera L. and Varroa destructor mite populations in France. Applied and Environmental Microbiology. 2004 Dec 1;70(12):7185–91. pmid:15574916
  12. 12. Ibrahim A, Spivak M. The relationship between hygienic behavior and suppression of mite reproduction as honey bee (Apis mellifera) mechanisms of resistance to Varroa destructor. Apidologie. 2006;37(1):31–40.
  13. 13. Harris JW. Bees with Varroa Sensitive Hygiene preferentially remove mite infested pupae aged ≤ five days post capping. Journal of Apicultural Research. 2007 Jan 1;46(3):134–9.
  14. 14. Giuffre C, Lubkin SR, Tarpy DR. Automated assay and differential model of western honey bee (Apis mellifera) autogrooming using digital image processing. Computers and Electronics in Agriculture. 2017 Apr 1;135:338–44.
  15. 15. Hunt G, Given JK, Tsuruda JM, Andino GK. Breeding Mite-Biting Bees to Control Varroa. Bee Culture. 2016 Mar 23.
  16. 16. Chen YP, Siede R. Honey bee viruses. Advances in Virus Research. 2007 Dec 31;70:33–80. pmid:17765703
  17. 17. Genersch E, Aubert M. Emerging and re-emerging viruses of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Veterinary Research. 2010 Nov 1;41(6):54. pmid:20423694
  18. 18. Gisder S, Aumeier P, Genersch E. Deformed wing virus: replication and viral load in mites (Varroa destructor). Journal of General Virology. 2009 Feb 1;90(2):463–7.
  19. 19. de Miranda JR, Genersch E. Deformed wing virus. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology. 2010 Jan 31;103:S48–61. pmid:19909976
  20. 20. Mondet F, de Miranda JR, Kretzschmar A, Le Conte Y, Mercer AR. On the front line: Quantitative virus dynamics in honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) colonies along a new expansion front of the parasite Varroa destructor. PLoS Pathog. 2014 Aug 21;10(8):e1004323. pmid:25144447
  21. 21. Dietemann V, Ellis JD, Neumann P. Standard methods for Apis mellifera pest and pathogen research: Introduction. Journal of Apicultural Research. 30 Jul 2013;52(4):1–4.
  22. 22. Evans JD. Beepath: an ordered quantitative-PCR array for exploring honey bee immunity and disease. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology. 2006 Oct 31;93(2):135–9. pmid:16737710
  23. 23. Pfaffl MW. A new mathematical model for relative quantification in real-time RT–PCR. Nucleic Acids Research. 2001 May 1;29(9):e45. pmid:11328886
  24. 24. Alburaki M, Chen D, Skinner J, Meikle W, Tarpy D, Adamczyk J, Stewart S. Honey bee survival and pathogen prevalence: from the perspective of landscape and exposure to pesticides. Insects. 2018 Jun;9(2):65.
  25. 25. Rindskopf D. Nonstandard log-linear models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990 Jul;108(1):150.
  26. 26. DaPalma T, Doonan BP, Trager NM, Kasman LM. A systematic approach to virus–virus interactions. Virus Research. 2010 Apr 30;149(1):1–9. pmid:20093154
  27. 27. Syller J. Facilitative and antagonistic interactions between plant viruses in mixed infections. Molecular Plant Pathology. 2012 Feb 1;13(2):204–16. pmid:21726401
  28. 28. Celum C, Levine R, Weaver M, Wald A. Genital herpes and human immunodeficiency virus: double trouble. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2004 Jun;82(6):447–53. pmid:15356938
  29. 29. Levin S, Sela N, Chejanovsky N. Two novel viruses associated with the Apis mellifera pathogenic mite Varroa destructor. Scientific reports. 2016 Nov 24;6:37710. pmid:27883042
  30. 30. Levin S, Galbraith D, Sela N, Erez T, Grozinger CM, Chejanovsky N. Presence of Apis rhabdovirus-1 in populations of pollinators and their parasites from two continents. Frontiers in microbiology. 2017 Dec 12;8:2482. pmid:29312191
  31. 31. Grozinger CM, Flenniken ML. Bee viruses: Ecology, pathogenicity, and impacts. Annual review of entomology. 2019 Jan 7;64:205–26. pmid:30629896