Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Not all electric shark deterrents are made equal: Effects of a commercial electric anklet deterrent on white shark behaviour

  • Channing A. Egeberg ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Channing A. Egeberg, Ryan M. Kempster

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors are joint first authors on this work.

    Affiliation The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia

  • Ryan M. Kempster ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Channing A. Egeberg, Ryan M. Kempster

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ryankempster@supportoursharks.com

    ‡ These authors are joint first authors on this work.

    Affiliation The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia

  • Nathan S. Hart,

    Roles Supervision

    Affiliations The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia, Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, New South Wales, Australia

  • Laura Ryan,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia, Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, New South Wales, Australia

  • Lucille Chapuis,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia

  • Caroline C. Kerr,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia

  • Carl Schmidt,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia

  • Enrico Gennari,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Oceans Research, Mossel Bay, South Africa, South African Institute for Aquatic Biodiversity, Grahamstown, South Africa

  • Kara E. Yopak,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia, Department of Biology and Marine Biology, UNCW Center for Marine Science, University of North Carolina Wilmington, Wilmington, North Carolina, United States of America

  • Shaun P. Collin

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations The UWA Oceans Institute and the Oceans Graduate School, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australia, School of Life Sciences, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia

Abstract

Personal shark deterrents offer the potential of a non-lethal solution to protect individuals from negative interactions with sharks, but the claims of effectiveness of most deterrents are based on theory rather than robust testing of the devices themselves. Therefore, there is a clear need for thorough testing of commercially available shark deterrents to provide the public with information on their effectiveness. Using a modified stereo-camera system, we quantified behavioural interactions between Carcharodon carcharias (white sharks) and a baited target in the presence of a commercially available electric anklet shark deterrent, the Electronic Shark Defense System (ESDS). The stereo-camera system enabled accurate assessment of the behavioural responses of C. carcharias when approaching an ESDS. We found that the ESDS had limited meaningful effect on the behaviour of C. carcharias, with no significant reduction in the proportion of sharks interacting with the bait in the presence of the active device. At close proximity (< 15.5 cm), the active ESDS did show a significant reduction in the number of sharks biting the bait, but this was countered by an increase in other, less aggressive, interactions. The ESDS discharged at a frequency of 7.8 Hz every 5.1 s for 2.5 s, followed by an inactive interval of 2.6 s. As a result, many sharks may have encountered the device in its inactive state, resulting in a reduced behavioural response. Consequently, decreasing the inactive interval between pulses may improve the overall effectiveness of the device, but this would not improve the effective deterrent range of the device, which is primarily a factor of the voltage gradient rather than the stimulus frequency. In conclusion, given the very short effective range of the ESDS and its unreliable deterrent effect, combined with the fact that shark-bite incidents are very rare, it is unlikely that the current device would significantly reduce the risk of a negative interaction with C. carcharias.

Introduction

As human populations increase, more people continue to enter the ocean for leisure, resulting in an increase in human-shark interactions globally [1, 2]. Although negative interactions between humans and sharks are extremely rare, each incident attracts a high level of interest, as they often result in serious consequences for those involved. Despite the worldwide media attention that shark bite incidents receive, over 80% of them have occurred in just 6 regions: The United States, Australia, South Africa, Brazil, The Bahamas, and Réunion Island [2]. In response, all of these regions (except The Bahamas) have, at some point, instituted some form of government-controlled mitigation strategy in an attempt to reduce the number of shark bite incidents in their waters [36]. Unfortunately, most of these strategies have involved the removal of sharks in order to reduce the local population, yet no evidence has been presented to support the effectiveness of such programs in reducing the risk of a negative encounter with a shark [3, 7]. Furthermore, these programs are at odds with the important ecological role that sharks play in ocean ecosystems [8, 9]. Since these control programs do not discriminate by species or size, they place increased pressure on non-target and potentially vulnerable species [1013], including elasmobranchs and marine mammals, the effects of which could be ecologically and economically damaging [9, 1418]. There is, therefore, a clear need for alternative non-lethal shark mitigation solutions that will allow humans and sharks to safely co-exist.

Previous research suggests that there are a variety of methods that could be used to deter sharks from an area, based purely on manipulation of their sensory cues [1921]. Personal shark deterrents offer the potential of a non-lethal solution to protect individuals from negative interactions with sharks, and vice versa. The most well studied form of non-lethal deterrent to date, the Shark Shield [2225] targets a shark’s electroreceptive organs, known as the ampullae of Lorenzini, which can detect minute electric field gradients (≤1 nV/cm) via an array of small pore openings on the surface of the head [26]. The electrosensory system is known to facilitate the passive detection of bioelectric stimuli produced by potential prey [2629], predators [30, 31], and conspecifics [31, 32]. Electric deterrents are designed to over-stimulate the electrosensory system [4, 24, 25, 33], while causing minimal or no effect on non-target species that do not possess this sensory modality [23].

Some electric shark deterrents have been shown to effectively deter Carcharodon carcharias (white shark) from biting stationary bait presented in the water column [22, 25], and from interacting with mobile seal decoys at the surface [24]. Specific electric field characteristics, such as voltage gradient and frequency, have been shown to be key factors that influence how an electric deterrent will affect a shark’s behaviour [22, 30]. This aspect of deterrent technology is particularly important, given that sharks are also attracted to certain types of electric stimuli [2628, 34]. Currently, there are a number of electric deterrents commercially available to the public (Table 1), all of which claim to be effective shark deterrents, yet most of them have not undergone robust and independent scientific scrutiny. Furthermore, given that the design and electrode configuration of each of these devices is different, the effectiveness of each device, or lack thereof, will likely reflect these differences. Therefore, studying responses of sharks to different devices with varying electric field properties can help determine optimal deterrent thresholds.

thumbnail
Table 1. Commercially available shark deterrents that target the electrosensory system.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.t001

In field tests with C. carcharias, the Shark Shield was shown to be an effective deterrent [2225] capable of reducing interactions with bait by an average of 82.7%, with a minimum effective deterrent range of 82–131 cm (equivalent to 9.7–15.7 V/m) [22]. The minimum effective deterrent range was described as the shortest distance/highest voltage gradient that a shark would appropriate toward an active device. The combination of the steep voltage gradient and an electric pulse frequency of 1.67Hz produced by the Shark Shield, likely overwhelmed the shark’s electrosensory system resulting in an avoidance response. Kempster, Hart [30] observed a greater deterrent (‘freeze’) response by shark embryos when the voltage gradient increased and frequencies ranged between 0.1 and 2Hz. [22], therefore, concluded that as voltage gradient is a limiting factor in the development of an electric deterrent (due to the potentially negative effects on the users wearing them, i.e. causing involuntary muscle spasms), it may be possible to increase effectiveness by altering the frequency of the electric field discharge.

In the present study, we set out to test the effectiveness of another commercially available electric shark deterrent, the Electric Shark Defense System (ESDS), which is known to utilise different electric field characteristics to the Shark Shield. We aimed to measure the electric field gradient and frequency of the ESDS to determine if, in theory, it would be capable of deterring C. carcharias based on the known electrosensory deterrent threshold of this species [22]. This would allow a greater understanding of how differences in voltage gradient and frequency may affect the behaviour of C. carcharias. In addition, we aimed to behaviourally test the effective deterrent radius of the ESDS by measuring the closest distance that C. carcharias would approach a bait protected by the active device compared to a visually-identical (but electrically inactive) control. Overall, this study aimed to determine the effectiveness of the ESDS, and provide more information on the electric field characteristics necessary to deter white sharks.

Methods

Ethics statement

This project was approved by The University of Western Australia Animal Ethics Committee (Permit No. RA/3/100/1193), and by the South African Department of Environmental Affairs: Biodiversity and Coastal Research, Oceans and Coasts Branch (Permit No. RES2014/91). All work was carried out in strict accordance with the guidelines of the Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes (8th Edition 2013).

Study site

Experiments were conducted on consecutive days in July 2014 off Seal Island, Mossel Bay, in the Western Cape region of South Africa (Fig 1). This site was chosen due to its calm conditions and the large population of pinnipeds that frequent Seal Island, which has resulted in a reliably high abundance of C. carcharias periodically throughout the year [35]. Testing was conducted simultaneously at four locations on the eastern side of the island (Fig 1A) and repeated four times each day between 8 am and 4 pm.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Map of Seal Island (A) in Mossel Bay, South Africa (B), highlighting the specific location of testing sites around the island (A1-4). Testing site locations are not exact, but, instead, mark the approximate area that trials were concurrently conducted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.g001

Remote Monitoring Research Apparatus (ReMoRA)

Stereo-video recordings were made using a modified Baited Remote Underwater Video System (BRUVS) called a Remote Monitoring Research Apparatus (ReMoRA). Stereo-BRUVS have been used extensively to characterise fish assemblages and allow for the recording of events at precise distances [36]. The design and setup of the ReMoRA are detailed by [22]. In brief, the ReMoRA included two downward-facing GoPro Hero 3 high-definition cameras (in waterproof housings), positioned 0.7 m apart on a horizontal aluminium square bar affixed perpendicularly to a vertical stainless steel pole (Fig 2). GoPro cameras were chosen due to their low cost, and ability to generate accurate length measurements from stereo video footage [37]. The cameras were inwardly converged by eight degrees to gain a maximum field-of-view and to allow for three-dimensional calibration used for distance and length measurements [37, 38] (Fig 2B). A PVC container (approximate volume of 4.5 litres), holding approximately 0.5 kg of sardines and locally-sourced fish heads, was securely suspended 1 m in front of the cameras to act as a controlled attractant and, despite sharks interacting with it, the bait canister was never removed from the ReMoRA.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Diagram of a Remote Monitoring Research Apparatus (ReMoRA).

(A) shows the ReMoRA in its deployed configuration with downward-facing cameras. (B) shows the measurements recorded to calculate proximity of C. carcharias to the ESDS electrode closest to the bait canister. Using Event Measure software, the closest part of a shark’s head to the electrode is marked via the left and right cameras (a), and then the centre of the ESDS electrode is also marked (b), which accurately calculates the closest observable proximity of the shark in three-dimensional space (c), taking into account both the vertical and horizontal axis. For clarity, the electrodes of the ESDS are displayed in white to highlight their position.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.g002

Electric shark deterrent

The source of the electric deterrent in this study was the commercially available Electronic Shark Defense System (ESDS). The ESDS is a portable electronic device, patented by Wilson Vinano [39, 40], which emits an electric field and is used by recreational water users to repel sharks. The device is designed to wrap around the ankle, and consists of a small electronic control unit connected to two square electrodes separated by 10 cm. The device is automatically activated when the electrodes are submerged in seawater, completing the electric circuit, which results in the generation of an electric field thought to be repellent to sharks, as outlined in the original patent [40]. Since the completion of this study, the ESDS has been rebranded as No Shark. It is not clear whether the newly-branded device differs from the one used in this study.

Electric field gradient measurements

To estimate the electric field gradient that a shark experienced when encountering an active ESDS, a voltage gradient probe was constructed and connected to an oscilloscope to record the electric field gradient at set distances, and angles, relative to an active ESDS, following the same protocol outlined by Kempster, Egeberg [22]. Measurements were recorded in a sheltered bay with a bottom depth of 4 m, at a temperature and salinity similar to Mossel Bay (15°C; 37 ppt). The shallow depth was necessary to allow the probe to be accurately positioned by an investigator and to minimise wave disturbance. However, the proximity of the ESDS to the seabed and the surface may have had an effect on the spatial distribution and strength of the electric field. Therefore, electric field measurements presented in this study should only be used as an estimate and not absolute, as they are likely to vary depending on the conditions in which the device is used. Comparable measurements were also recorded from an inactive device to confirm the lack of detectable signal.

Experimental design

Each ReMoRA was deployed with either an inactive ESDS (control treatment) or an active ESDS (active treatment). As the ESDS automatically activates upon contact with water, we used a device with no charge for the control treatment. Each rig was suspended from the surface via a large float, with the bait positioned at approximately 4 m depth (1 m below the cameras), and anchored to the seabed at approximately 20 m depth (Fig 2). Four ReMoRAs were deployed simultaneously across four locations on the eastern side of Seal Island (two control and two active), which were each separated by at least 300 m (Fig 1A). After each ReMoRA was deployed, the vessel moved to the other side of the island to avoid interference with the experiment. Once deployed, the cameras attached to each ReMoRA (active and control) recorded continuously for 90 minutes to complete one trial. Each ReMoRA was then retrieved and redeployed at a different site (after replacing camera batteries, SD cards, and bait), rotating between all four sites throughout a day of testing, with the starting location randomly allocated. Potential temporal and spatial influences were limited by deploying control and active treatments evenly between locations and during the same time period each day.

Individual sharks were identified from distinct markings, scars, and fin shapes using a catalogue of known individuals provided by local researchers at Oceans Research (www.oceans-research.com). Accurate assessments of sex were not possible, but, based on local knowledge and previous research [41], the population around Seal Island is thought to be comprised of predominantly females. Furthermore, due to low visibility, the shark total length could not always be precisely measured from the ReMoRA stereo-video footage. However, based on information from local researchers, all sharks included in this investigation were considered to be between 2 and 4 m in total length [35].

Video calibration and analysis

The program CAL (SeaGIS Pty. Ltd.) was used to calibrate the ReMoRA’s cameras before and after completion of the field work in order to make accurate proximity measurements from the footage. This process is described in detail by Harvey and Shortis [38]. Xilisoft video conversion software (Xilisoft Corporation) was then used to merge and convert the collected GoPro footage from MP4 to AVI format to facilitate image analysis using the program EventMeasure (SeaGIS Pty. Ltd.). EventMeasure was used to identify and count the number of individuals, estimate individual lengths (where possible), measure time spent in the area, and quantify minimum distance (proximity) to the deterrent during encounters. The software synchronizes stereo-video footage to allow accurate measurements of distance to be recorded in three-dimensional space. Time spent in the area was measured between the first and last appearance of individual sharks within the field-of-view of the cameras. An individual’s proximity to the deterrent was measured from the closest part of a shark’s head to the center of the closest ESDS electrode during each encounter. A single proximity measurement was calculated for each encounter and defined as the closest observable distance a shark approached during an encounter with the control and active treatments, regardless of whether they interacted with the treatment or not. Therefore, even when a shark interacted by biting a bait canister, their closest proximity to the center of the electrode was still calculated. This allowed for the calculation of the highest electric field strength that a shark experienced during each encounter with an active ESDS.

Data analysis

All encounters of C. carcharias with the ReMoRA (appearance on the stereo-camera video footage) were classified at three levels of interaction. If a shark passed by (within the field-of-view of the cameras) without interacting, then it was categorized as a Type 0 interaction (Pass). If a shark touched the bait, ESDS or any other part of the rig with any part of its body (other than its mouth), then it was categorized as a Type 1 interaction (Bump) (Fig 3A). Finally, if a shark bit the bait or ESDS, its behaviour was categorized as a Type 2 interaction (Bite) (Fig 3B). No individual sharks were identified as appearing in multiple trials, although we cannot be absolutely certain that this did not occur, as identification of specific individuals was difficult for some encounters. Nevertheless, for statistical purposes, data from different trials were not considered to reflect repeated measures on individual animals. Where relevant, statistical tests were weighted by encounter number or shark ID to detect any affect that individual sharks and/or the number of encounters had on each treatment. All statistical tests were performed using the statistics software Minitab (Minitab Inc.), and, unless otherwise stated, data is presented as mean ± std. error throughout.

thumbnail
Fig 3.

Screenshots of C. carcharias encountering an active ESDS: (A) C. carcharias interacting with the bait (Type 1 interaction); (B) C. carcharias biting the bait (Type 2 interaction).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.g003

Results

A total of 17 control deployments (inactive ESDS) and 17 active deployments (active ESDS) were conducted (totalling 51 hours of video footage), which resulted in 395 encounters (238 control; 157 active) from 44 individual C. carcharias.

Interactions

The presence of an active ESDS did not result in a reduction or increase in the number of C. carcharias individuals observed (appearance within the camera’s field-of-view within a distance of ≤ 3 m) when compared with the control (Table 2: #1). Upon their first encounter with a ReMoRA, 43.5 ± 10.6% of C. carcharias individuals interacted with the bait during control trials, and 33.3 ± 10.5% of sharks interacted during active trials (Table 2: #2). When considering all encounters, an equal proportion of sharks interacted (Bumps and Bites, i.e.: Type 1 and 2 interactions) at least once (Table 2: #3; Fig 4) during control (95.7 ± 4.4%) and active trials (85.7 ± 7.8%). In contrast, when only Bites (Type 2 interactions) were considered, significantly fewer individuals were observed interacting (Table 2: #4; Fig 4) during active trials (52.4%) than during control trials (87.0%). On average, the number of times individuals of C. carcharias encountered a ReMoRA during a single trial (appeared on camera, whether interacting or not) did not differ significantly (Table 2: #5) between control (10.35 ± 1.86) and active (7.14 ± 1.31) treatments. However, the number of interactions per trial did differ significantly Table 2: #6) between the control (7.65 ± 1.53) and active (4.14 ± 1.27) treatments.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Proportion of interactions (Type 0, 1, and 2) by C. carcharias during control and active trials. n refers to individual sharks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparison of the behavioural response of C. carcharias when encountering an inactive (control) or active ESDS.

For more detailed data, see S1 Table. Justification for the statistical tests used is provided below.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.t002

Time taken to arrive and interact

The time taken for C. carcharias to first arrive on screen during each trial did not differ significantly (Table 2: #7) between the control (32:55 ± 6:19 mins) and active (26:03 ± 8:46 mins) treatments. After first arrival on screen, the time taken for individuals to interact also did not differ significantly (Table 2: #8) between control (0:24 ± 0:13 mins) and active (0:21 ± 0:04 mins) treatments. Furthermore, the total time that sharks spent in the area during each trial did not differ significantly (Table 2: #9) between the control (2:34 ± 0:34 mins) and active (1:43 ± 0:25 mins) treatments. Following a previous encounter, the time taken for C. carcharias individuals to reappear on screen occurred over a short time frame (18–25 s between encounters), with no significant time difference observed between encounters with the control or active treatments (Table 2: #10 and #11).

Proximity

The mean proximity of the first C. carcharias individuals to encounter a ReMoRA during each trial was not significantly different (Table 2: #12; Fig 5) between the control (47.4 ± 8.5 cm) and active (35.1 ± 7.3 cm) treatments. When considering all encounters of C. carcharias individuals, mean proximity was still not significantly different (Table 2: #13) between the control (27.0 ± 3.1 cm) and active (26.8 ± 3.1 cm) treatments. Furthermore, no significant difference was observed in the mean proximity per encounter (Table 2: #14; Fig 5) between control (23.6 ± 3.2 cm) and active (23.5 ± 1.8 cm) treatments. Despite significantly fewer sharks biting the bait (Type 2 interactions) during active trials (Table 2: #4), no significant difference was observed in the mean proximity per individual (Table 2: #15), or the mean proximity per encounter (Table 2: #16), during Type 2 interactions with the control (17.2 ± 1.7 cm and 17.0 ± 1.1 cm, respectively) and active (13.7 ± 2.5 cm and 15.5 ± 1.2 cm, respectively) treatments.

thumbnail
Fig 5.

Proportion of sharks that interacted per encounter during control (A) and active (B) ESDS trials. Overlaid is the average proximity of sharks to the ESDS during each encounter. Proximity trend line (Control): y = -22.083x + 346.6; Proximity trend line (Active): y = -10.753x + 288.27.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.g005

Habituation

Based on an individual shark’s first nine encounters (i.e. the maximum number of encounters per trial in which there are data available for both control and active treatments), when only considering interactions (not proximity), there was no significant evidence of habituation between encounters during control (Table 3A: #1; Fig 5A) or active (Table 3B: #1; Fig 5B) trials. There was also no relationship between the proportion of sharks interacting per encounter and the total number of sharks (Control: Table 3A: #2; Active: Table 3B: #2), or between the proportion of sharks interacting per encounter and the number of encounters (Control: Table 3A: #3; Active: Table 3B: #3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Comparison of the behavioural response of C. carcharias between individuals, and between encounters, during control (A) and active (B) trials.

Justification for the tests used is provided below.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.t003

Conversely, based on the same nine encounters, when considering only proximity (not interactions), there was some evidence of habituation, as significant differences were observed in how close individual sharks would approach during control (Table 3A: #4; Fig 5A) and active (Table 3B: #4; Fig 5B) trials. Furthermore, during control trials, the average proximity of all sharks combined decreased significantly with each subsequent approach (Table 3A: #5; Fig 5A). However, this relationship was not observed during active trails (Table 3B: #5; Fig 5B). Nevertheless, during control trials, there was no evidence that the observed differences in proximity between individual sharks, or all sharks combined, was influenced by the number of encounters each shark experienced (Table 3A: #6 and #7) or by the total number of sharks included (Table 3A: #8). Whereas, during active trials, there was evidence that the observed difference in proximity between individual sharks was significantly negatively correlated with the number of times individuals encountered the device (Table 3B: #6). However, the average proximity of all sharks combined per encounter was not correlated with the number of encounters each shark experienced or the total number of sharks included (Table 3B: #7 and #8).

ESDS electric field characteristics and predicted effective range

The electric field voltage gradient of the ESDS was greatest at close proximity to the electrodes and dissipated rapidly with distance (Fig 6). A maximum voltage gradient of >200 V/m was measured within 5 cm of the electrodes. The ESDS discharged every 5.1 s (0.2 Hz) and consisted of 20 pulses (10 positive and 10 negative, with sequential pulses of alternating polarity) over a 2.5 s period (7.8 Hz) with an inter-pulse period of inactivity of 2.6 s. For consistent measurements, the electric field gradient was measured along the same axis, parallel to the end of the electrode.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Plot to show the voltage gradient decline of the ESDS with increasing distance.

The short-dashed arrows indicate the average deterrent threshold of C. carcharias (15.7 V/m [22]) and the corresponding estimated effective deterrent range of the ESDS (11.6 cm). The long-dashed arrows indicate the average deterrent threshold of C. carcharias during their first encounter with an electric field (9.7 V/m [22]) and the corresponding estimated effective deterrent range of the ESDS (16.9 cm). Red dots depict actual voltage gradient measurements recorded for the ESDS. Voltage gradient curve plotted using Harris model: y = 1/(-0.06s82+0.0239x^0.6961).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.g006

The mean proximity of C. carcharias during the first encounter with an active ESDS (35.1 ± 7.3 cm), which was not significantly different from the control (Table 2: #12; Fig 5), equated to an estimated voltage gradient of just 4.6 (± 5.1) V/m (Fig 6) experienced by a shark. Even when considering the mean proximity of all encounters with an active ESDS (23.5 ± 1.8 cm), which was also not significantly different from the control (Table 2: #14; Fig 5), the estimated voltage gradient experienced was just slightly higher at 6.8 (± 0.5) V/m (Fig 6). However, when only considering interactions that resulted in a Bite (Type 2 interactions), the mean proximity per individual (13.7 ± 2.5 cm) and per encounter (15.5 ± 1.2 cm) equated to much greater estimated average voltage gradients of 10.7 V/m and 12.5 V/m, respectively.

Discussion

Initial observation of C. carcharias interactions with an active ESDS might suggest that the device was having a repellent effect, as significantly fewer individuals were observed biting (Type 2 Interactions) the active device compared with the control (Fig 4). Furthermore, when only considering interactions (not proximity), the observed effect remained constant even after multiple encounters, suggesting that a shark’s behaviour was not changing over time in the presence of the active device. However, when considering proximity, sharks did show evidence of habituation as they would approach closer with each subsequent encounter (Fig 5B). When you also account for sharks bumping the device as well as biting (Type 1 and 2 Interactions), there was no significant difference in the effectiveness of the active ESDS over the inactive control (Fig 4). Thus, any effect that the active ESDS may of been having was at such a short range that the sharks would likely have only experienced it when they were about to bite.

Based on the electrical output of the ESDS (Fig 6) and the currently accepted electric deterrent range of C. carcharias (9.7–15.7 V/m [22]), it was predicted that individuals would show a deterrent response when they approached within 11.6 to 16.9 cm of an active device (Fig 6). However, most encounters and interactions observed during active trials fell outside of this range (Table 2: #12–14), and were not significantly different from the control trials. Therefore, the active ESDS was unlikely to be having any meaningful effect on the behaviour of C. carcharias, particularly when you compare these results with those of the Shark Shield [22]. The active ESDS did, however, significantly reduce, but not prevent, Bites (Type 2 interactions) (Table 2: #4; Fig 4). There was a 34.6% reduction in the proportion of Bites in the presence of the active ESDS, but a corresponding increase (24.6%) in Bumps (Type 1 interactions) (Fig 4). As the proximity of Bites (Type 2 interactions) fell within the predicted effective deterrent range of the ESDS (Table 2: # 15 and #16), a significant behavioural response was observed, but the active device was not sufficiently effective to prevent interactions all together.

During prior testing of an alternative electric deterrent, the Shark Shield, almost all interactions by C. carcharias were prevented at a voltage gradient of 9.7–15.7 V/m [22]. Yet, despite experiencing a similar voltage gradient upon close encounters with an active ESDS (equivalent of 12.5 V/m), 52% of sharks still interacted by biting the bait (Type 2 interaction) (Table 2: #4; Fig 4). Furthermore, when encountering an active ESDS, sharks had to approach within 15.5 ± 1.2 cm (Table 2: #15) of the device to experience a voltage gradient high enough to cause a behavioural response. In contrast, when encountering a Shark Shield, sharks only had to approach within 131 ± 10.3 cm to exhibit a behavioural response [22]. Based on previously reported electric deterrent thresholds for a range of shark species, it is estimated that the Shark Shield will produce an effective deterrent range, on average, seven times larger than that produced by the ESDS (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Estimated effective deterrent range of the Shark Shield and ESDS for five shark species, based on their highest reported deterrent threshold (V/m).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.t004

As suggested by Kempster et al. [22], it is likely that the time between pulses of an electric deterrent will play an important role in the effectiveness of the device. The ESDS, for example, pulsed at a rate of 7.8 Hz for 2.5 s, but was then inactive for a period of 2.6 s between pulse bursts, whereas, the Shark Shield pulsed continuously at a rate of 1.67 Hz. Therefore, the ESDS was actually inactive for 2.6 s between every 2.5 s burst of pulses (i.e. the device was inactive 51% of the time), whereas the Shark Shield was only inactive for approximately 0.6 s between pulses. When we consider that the time taken between encounters can be as short as 18 s (Table 2: #10), it is very likely that individuals may have encountered an active ESDS during the 2.6 s inactive period between pulses. This likely explains why so many sharks interacted during active ESDS trials (Fig 4), as many of those interactions may have occurred during the 2.6 s inactive period. Therefore, the ESDS may be improved by reducing the inter-pulse interval, but this is unlikely to have any significant impact on the effective deterrent range of the device, as this is a factor of the strength of the voltage gradient and electrode spacing, rather than pulse frequency. Due to the compact size of the ESDS, the electrodes are spaced very close to one another (10 cm apart), which will limit its potential deterrent range because of the exponential decay in field strength with distance beyond the dipoles. Previous studies have suggested that an electric deterrent will likely be most effective if it imitates the frequency of biological organisms (1–2 Hz) [44]. Although technically correct, rather than sharks showing a natural aversion to biologically familiar signals, it is more likely that a repetition rate of 1–2 Hz will ensure that an approaching shark experiences the voltage gradient during their encounter. The effective deterrent range is, therefore, simply a matter of the strength of the voltage gradient produced, which, in this case, is limited by the small size of the ESDS, and in the case of larger devices like the Shark Shield, is limited by the potential negative effects that a strong electric field may have on the user wearing the device.

The results of this study showed that the ESDS did have an effect on C. carcharias behaviour in very close proximity (≤ 15.5 ± 1.2 cm; Table 2: #15), but the deterrent effect was not sufficient to completely prevent interactions with a static bait. Given the very short effective range of the ESDS (Table 2: #15 and #16) and the unreliable deterrent effect (Fig 4), it is doubtful that this device would dramatically reduce the risk of a negative shark encounter for the person wearing it. Ocean users should be very critical of shark deterrent claims, as the use of untested devices may actually put lives at risk by giving users a false sense of security. Future research should compare the behavioural responses of a range of shark species with other electric shark deterrents on the market, to determine species-specific differences in the effectiveness of these devices.

Supporting information

S1 Table.

Behavioural response of C. carcharias when encountering an inactive/control (A) or active (B) ESDS.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212851.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by The University of Western Australia, the West Australian State Government Shark Hazard Mitigation Applied Research Program, and through the very generous support of Mr Craig Burton and Mrs Katrina Burton. The authors wish to thank the South African Department of Environmental Affairs: Biodiversity and Coastal Research, Oceans and Coasts Branch for their support and help in conducting this research. The authors also wish to thank Jessica Meeuwig, Charlie Huveneers, Lauren Peel and all of the interns at the Oceans Research campus in Mossel Bay for all of their help and hard work during this investigation.

References

  1. 1. West J. Changing patterns of shark attacks in Australian waters. Mar Freshwater Res. 2011;62:744–54.
  2. 2. McPhee D. Unprovoked Shark Bites: Are They Becoming More Prevalent? Coastal Management. 2014;42(5):478–92.
  3. 3. Wetherbee BM, Lowe C., G. C. A Review of Shark Control in Hawaii with Recommendations for Future Research. Pac Sci. 1994;4(2):95–115.
  4. 4. Cliff G, Dudley SFJ. Protection against shark attack in South Africa, 1952–90. Mar Freshwater Res. 1992;43(1):263–72.
  5. 5. Hazin FHV, Afonso AS. A green strategy for shark attack mitigation off Recife, Brazil. Animal Conservation. 2014;17(4):287–96.
  6. 6. Crossley R, Collins CM, Sutton SG, Huveneers C. Public Perception and Understanding of Shark Attack Mitigation Measures in Australia. Human Dimensions of Wildlife. 2014;19(2):154–65.
  7. 7. House D. Western Australian Shark Hazard Mitigation Drum Line Program 2014–17: Public Environmental Review. Western Australia: The Department of the Premier and Cabinet, 2014.
  8. 8. Ferretti F, Worm B, Britten GL, Heithaus MR, Lotze HK. Patterns and ecosystem consequences of shark declines in the ocean. Ecology Letters. 2010;13(8):1055–71. pmid:20528897
  9. 9. Ruppert JLW, Travers MJ, Smith LL, Fortin M-J, Meekan MG. Caught in the Middle: Combined Impacts of Shark Removal and Coral Loss on the Fish Communities of Coral Reefs. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(9):e74648. pmid:24058618
  10. 10. Cliff G, Dudley SFJ. Sharks caught in the protective gill nets off Natal, South Africa. 4. The bull shark Carcharhinus leucas Valenciennes. South African Journal of Marine Science. 1991;10(1):253–70.
  11. 11. Dudley SFJ, Cliff G. Sharks caught in the protective gill nets off Natal, South Africa. 7. The blacktip shark Carcharhinus limbatus (Valenciennes). South African Journal of Marine Science. 1993;13(1):237–54.
  12. 12. Cliff G. Sharks caught in the protective gill nets off Kwazulu-Natal, South Africa. 8. The great hammerhead shark Sphyrna mokarran (Ruppell). South African Journal of Marine Science. 1995;15:105–14.
  13. 13. Cliff G, Dudley S, Jury M. Catches of white sharks in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa and environmental influences. Great white sharks: The biology of Carcharodon carcharias. 1996:351–62.
  14. 14. Vianna GMS, Meekan MG, Pannell DJ, Marsh SP, Meeuwig JJ. Socio-economic value and community benefits from shark-diving tourism in Palau: A sustainable use of reef shark populations. Biological Conservation. 2012;145(1):267–77.
  15. 15. Gallagher AJ, Hammerschlag N. Global shark currency: the distribution, frequency, and economic value of shark ecotourism. Current Issues in Tourism. 2011;14(8):797–812.
  16. 16. Cisneros-Montemayor AM, Barnes-Mauthe M, Al-Abdulrazzak D, Navarro-Holm E, Sumaila UR. Global economic value of shark ecotourism: implications for conservation. Oryx. 2013;47(3):1–8.
  17. 17. Kempster RM, Collin SP. Iconic Species: Great White Sharks, Basking Sharks and Whale Sharks In: Klein EJTaN, editor. Sharks: Conservation, Governance and Management: Taylor and Francis Group; 2014. p. 352.
  18. 18. Atwood TB, Connolly RM, Ritchie EG, Lovelock CE, Heithaus MR, Hays GC, et al. Predators help protect carbon stocks in blue carbon ecosystems. Nature Clim Change. 2015;advance online publication.
  19. 19. O'Connell CP, Stroud EM, He P. The emerging field of electrosensory and semiochemical shark repellents: Mechanisms of detection, overview of past studies, and future directions. Ocean & Coastal Management. 2014;97:2–11.
  20. 20. Hart NS, Collin SP. Sharks senses and shark repellents. Integrative Zoology. 2015;10(1):38–64. pmid:24919643
  21. 21. Jordan LK, Mandelman JW, McComb DM, Fordham SV, Carlson JK, Werner TB. Linking sensory biology and fisheries bycatch reduction in elasmobranch fishes: a review with new directions for research. Conservation Physiology. 2013;1(1):1–20. pmid:27293586
  22. 22. Kempster RM, Egeberg CA, Hart NS, Ryan L, Chapuis L, Kerr CC, et al. How Close is too Close? The Effect of a Non-Lethal Electric Shark Deterrent on White Shark Behaviour. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(7):e0157717. pmid:27368059
  23. 23. Broad A, Knott N, Turon X, Davis AR. Effects of a shark repulsion device on rocky reef fishes: no shocking outcomes. Marine Ecology Progress Series. 2010;408:295–8.
  24. 24. Huveneers C, Rogers PJ, Semmens JM, Beckmann C, Kock AA, Page B, et al. Effects of an Electric Field on White Sharks: In Situ Testing of an Electric Deterrent. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(5):e62730. pmid:23658766
  25. 25. Smit CE, Peddemors V. Estimating the probability of a shark attack when using an electric repellent: applications2003; 37(1):[59–78 pp.].
  26. 26. Kalmijn AJ. Bioelectric fields in sea water and the function of the ampullae of Lorenzini in elasmobranch fishes. SIO Reference, Scripps Institution of Oceanography, UC San Diego, 1972.
  27. 27. Kempster RM, Egeberg CA, Hart NS, Collin SP. Electrosensory-driven feeding behaviours of the Port Jackson shark (Heterodontus portusjacksoni) and western shovelnose ray (Aptychotrema vincentiana). Mar Freshwater Res. 2015;67(2):187–94.
  28. 28. Kajiura SM, Fitzgerald TP. Response of juvenile scalloped hammerhead sharks to electric stimuli. Zoology. 2009;112(4):241–50. pmid:19097876
  29. 29. Egeberg CA, Kempster RM, Theiss SM, Hart NS, Collin SP. The distribution and abundance of electrosensory pores in two benthic sharks: a comparison of the wobbegong shark, Orectolobus maculatus, and the angel shark, Squatina australis. Mar Freshwater Res. 2014;65(11):1003–8.
  30. 30. Kempster RM, Hart NS, Collin SP. Survival of the Stillest: Predator Avoidance in Shark Embryos. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(1):e52551. pmid:23326342
  31. 31. Sisneros JA, Tricas TC. Neuroethology and life history adaptations of the elasmobranch electric sense. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 2002;96(5–6):379–89. ISI:000185271200004.
  32. 32. Kempster RM, Garza-Gisholt E, Egeberg CA, Hart NS, O’Shea OR, Collin SP. Sexual dimorphism of the electrosensory system: a quantitative analysis of nerve axons in the dorsal anterior lateral line nerve of the blue spotted fantail stingray (Taeniura lymma). Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2013;81(4):1–10. pmid:23817033
  33. 33. Brill R, Bushnell P, Smith L, Speaks C, Sundaram R, Stroud E, et al. The repulsive and feeding-deterrent effects of electropositive metals on juvenile sandbar sharks (Carcharhinus plumbeus). Fish B-Noaa. 2009;107(3):298–307. ISI:000268440700004.
  34. 34. Kalmijn AJ. Electro-orientation in sharks and rays: theory and experimental evidence. Oceanography SIo, editor. United States. Office of Naval Research: National Technical Information Service, US Dept. of Commerce; 1973.
  35. 35. Ryklief R, Pistorius PA, Johnson R. Spatial and seasonal patterns in sighting rate and life-history composition of the white shark Carcharodon carcharias at Mossel Bay, South Africa. African Journal of Marine Science. 2014;36(4):449–53.
  36. 36. Letessier TB, Meeuwig JJ, Gollock M, Groves L, Bouchet PJ, Chapuis L, et al. Assessing pelagic fish populations: The application of demersal video techniques to the mid-water environment. Methods in Oceanography. 2013;8:41–55.
  37. 37. Letessier TB, Juhel J-B, Vigliola L, Meeuwig JJ. Low-cost small action cameras in stereo generates accurate underwater measurements of fish. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 2015;466:120–6.
  38. 38. Harvey E, Shortis M. A system for stereo-video measurement of sub-tidal organisms. Mar Technol Soc J. 1995;29(4):10–22.
  39. 39. Vinano W, inventorWearable electronic shark deterrent unit. United States of America2013.
  40. 40. Vinano W, Lau C, inventorsHigh efficacy signal format and thin-profile ankle-mounting for electronic shark deterrent. United States of America2015.
  41. 41. Kock A, Johnson R. White shark abundance: not a causative factor in numbers of shark bite incidents. Finding a balance: White shark conservation and recreational safety in the inshore waters of Cape Town, South Africa. 2006:1–19.
  42. 42. Marcotte MM, Lowe CG. Behavioral Responses of Two Species of Sharks to Pulsed, Direct Current Electrical Fields: Testing a Potential Shark Deterrent. Mar Technol Soc J. 2008;42:53–61.
  43. 43. Smith E. Electro-physiology of the electrical shark-repellant. The Transactions of the Institute of Electrical Engineers. 1974;65(8):1–20.
  44. 44. Sisneros JA, Tricas TC. Ontogenetic changes in the response properties of the peripheral electrosensory system in the Atlantic stingray (Dasyatis sabina). Brain Behav Evolut. 2002;59(3):130–40. ISI:000176983700003.