Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Boost your brain, while having a break! The effects of long-term cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children’s executive functions and academic achievement

  • Fabienne Egger ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    fabienne.egger@ispw.unibe.ch

    Affiliation Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland

  • Valentin Benzing,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland

  • Achim Conzelmann,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision

    Affiliation Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland

  • Mirko Schmidt

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland

Abstract

Classroom-based physical activity (PA) is gaining attention in terms of its potential to enhance children’s cognitive functions, but it remains unclear as to which specific modality of PA affects cognitive functions most. The aim of the study was to examine the effects of qualitatively different PA breaks on children’s cognitive outcomes. Children (N = 142) aged between 7 and 9 years were allocated to a 20-week classroom-based PA program, with either high physical exertion and high cognitive engagement (combo group), high physical exertion and low cognitive engagement (aerobic group), or low physical exertion and high cognitive engagement (cognition group). Executive functions (updating, inhibition, shifting) and academic achievement (mathematics, spelling, reading) were measured pre- and post-intervention. Results showed that the combo group profited the most displaying enhanced shifting and mathematic performance. The cognition group profited only in terms of enhanced mathematic performance, whereas the aerobic group remained unaffected. These results suggest that the inclusion of cognitively engaging PA breaks seem to be a promising way to enhance school children’s cognitive functions.

Introduction

There is a growing body of research supporting a positive relationship between physical activity (PA), cognitive functions and academic achievement [14]. Cognitive functions, particularly executive functions (EFs), are acknowledged as a predictor for academic achievement [5]. The term “EFs” refers to a set of top-down mental processes that allows for controlled and goal-directed behaviour [6]. EFs can be subdivided into three core dimensions: The first dimension is updating, the ability to keep relevant information in working memory. The second dimension, inhibition, refers to the avoidance of dominant, automatic or prepotent responses. The third dimension, shifting, is based on updating and inhibition, and represents the ability to change among multiple tasks, operations, rules or perspectives. From a developmental perspective, inhibition is the first EF to be fully developed in children, whereas shifting is the last [5]. In general, high levels in EFs predict school readiness in young children [7] and explain a substantial amount of variance in elementary school children’s academic achievement [8]. Although not deriving from interventional studies, longitudinal data supports the mediating role of EFs between PA and academic achievement [9, 10].

Research has shown that long-term PA programs have a positive effect on the EFs and on academic achievement [3, 4]. Several physiological mechanisms have been proposed to explain these positive effects, including exercise-induced neurogenesis, angiogenesis, enhanced nervous system metabolism and increased catecholamine neurotransmission [11, 12]. EFs in turn seem to play a mediating role in the relationship between PA and academic achievement [10, 13, 14], indicating that increased PA promotes motor abilities and better EF performance, which consequently impacts academic achievement. Thus, specifically designing physical activities to infuse them into the school setting might be a cost-efficient way to enhance both children’s EFs and at the very end their academic achievement.

Recently, intervention studies are revealing that not all forms of PA benefit cognition equally. Besides quantitative aspects (such as exercise duration and intensity), qualitative aspects (such as exercise type) have also been shown to affect children’s EFs [15]. To date, the cognitive engagement (CE) inherent to many forms of PA is one of the qualitative aspects most widely discussed [16]. Increased cognitive demand is thought to induce CE, which is defined as the degree to which cognitive effort is needed to master difficult skills [17]. The “cognitive stimulation hypothesis” [18] provides a possible explanation for the cognitive improvement achieved through the cognitive demands inherent to PA exercises. The assumption is that cognitively demanding exercises activate similar brain regions used to control higher-order cognitive processes [16, 19]. The activation of these specific regions, through the participation in cognitively demanding exercises, leads to cognitive benefits in the circumscribed domains of executive functioning [15, 20].

Considering the existing literature, the current results seem to support this theoretical assumption. Vazou, Pesce, Lakes and Smiley-Oyen [21] found a medium effect of cognitively engaging PA on cognitive outcomes in their meta-analyses. Interventions comparing cognitively challenging vs. cognitively non-challenging PA, found the enhancement to be significantly more pronounced in response to cognitively engaging activities [22, 23]. It is evident that the interest in cognitive engagement during PA is gaining attention, however, research into PA modalities and their impact on cognitive outcomes is still in its infancy. Furthermore, several limitations make it difficult to draw comparisons between existing studies [21]. The high heterogeneity within the same categories of PA programs aggravated a clear understanding of effectiveness. Firstly, it is possible that programs defined as one specific modality of PA include substantially different contents and PA characteristics. Secondly, within a specific PA program several PA characteristics can be responsible for the cognitive stimulation induced. Thirdly, the reliability by which a PA program is implemented in practice can influence its efficacy and effectiveness. Consequently, there is a need to systematically vary the amount of CE inherent in PA exercises. For example, one study which varied the amount of CE systematically found positive effects, only through the implementation of cognitively enriched PA. Cognitively enriched PA games, such as basketball and floorball, elicited cognitive improvements in 10 to 12-year-old children, whereas both the mostly aerobic intervention (running exercises) and a traditional physical education curriculum, including only few cognitive demands, showed no beneficial effects on children’s cognitive outcomes [23]. Thus, it seems plausible that cognitively engaging long-term PA interventions provide an opportunity to modulate children’s cognitive functions effectively.

Classroom-based PA seems to be a promising tool to enhance not only daily PA, but also EFs and academic achievement [24]. Recently, studies have applied chronic classroom-based interventions and found encouraging results. These benefits range from enhanced classroom behavior [2527] to improved academic achievement [2830]. In general, two different types of (long-term) classroom-based PA can be distinguished: a) integrated PA, which incorporates PA during academic lessons (e.g. hopping the result of an arithmetic problem) and b) PA breaks, which consist of short bouts of PA between lessons (e.g. performing coordinative exercises) [31]. In a large study, including 1322 participants, improved classroom behavior was observed after 8 months of daily implemented PA breaks [25]. In another large long-term intervention, no improvements in academic achievement through daily PA breaks over 3 years were found, but academic achievement outcomes did not diminish compared to the control group [32]. This suggests that additional PA time in classroom, at least, has no negative influence on academic achievement.

A closer examination of studies finding positive effects on children’s cognitive outcomes through classroom-based PA uncovers differences concerning the specific PA modality implemented. Most classroom-based interventions featured basic aerobic movements (jogging, hopping, skipping) [27, 33, 34], whereas in others specific coordinative movements, like rope skipping or dancing had to be performed [35]. However, if experimental conditions were mostly compared to an passive control condition, e.g. sedentary academic or regular lessons, it is not surprising that children’s cognitive functions benefit from all varieties of PA interventions when they were compared to either no treatment or purely academic content. With the aim of systematically varying the amount of CE inherent in PA exercises, more than two groups including an active control group seem to have several advantages: improvements in academic related outcomes can be traced back as a result of the PA breaks intervention and shed light on a specific underlying mechanism. Further, active control groups deal better with confounds such as motivation, treatment credibility and internal validity [36].

Most previous studies either targeted children’s EFs [3] or academic achievement [30, 33, 35, 37, 38] as a separate outcome. Bearing in mind the aforementioned connection between PA and academic achievement with EFs playing a mediating role, only few studies [10, 39] included the core EFs and academic achievement as linked outcomes in the same design. Egger, Conzelmann and Schmidt [40] disentangled the separate and- /or combined short-term effect of physical exertion and CE induced by a single bout of classroom-based PA on children’s EFs. Consequently, there is another need to systematically differing between the long-term effects of cognitively engaging PA breaks, comparing them to active controls with less CE or less physical exertion.

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of three qualitatively different long-term PA break interventions, each with diverging quantities of cognitive engagement and physical exertion on both primary school children’s three core EFs and their academic achievement. It was hypothesized that PA breaks that combine physical effort with high cognitive demands are more effective than PA breaks with either low cognitive demands or low physical effort. Therefore, the effectiveness of three PA break interventions were compared: (1) The combo group with high amounts of both cognitive engagement and physical exertion, (2) the aerobic group with low cognitive engagement and high physical exertion, and (3) the cognition group with high cognitive engagement and low physical exertion.

Materials and methods

Design

The parents of the children provided written informed consent to participate in this study. Discontinuation was possible at any point during the study, and all data were treated with strict confidentially. The Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Human Sciences at the University of Bern has approved the study protocol prior the enrolment of the first participant.

Three 20-week interventions with different amounts of cognitive engagement and physical exertion were compared with respect to their effects on children’s EFs and academic achievement. The intervention duration of 20 weeks was chosen in order to follow recommendations of a recent meta-analysis indicating that the longer the intervention, the greater the effects on cognitive performance [41]. Furthermore, interventions were limited to 20 weeks because they had to fit into a school phase, which was not interrupted by holiday, in order to control confounding effects of the holiday activities.

Altogether, twelve classes were randomly assigned to one of the three experimental conditions: (1) The combo group with high levels of both cognitive engagement and physical exertion, (2) the aerobic group with low cognitive engagement and high physical exertion, and (3) the cognition group with high cognitive engagement and low physical exertion. The teachers were informed about the basic aims of the study but were blinded with respect to the specific hypotheses. Nevertheless, in each condition, the teachers were told that a positive effect on cognitive performance was expected. As usual in classical comparison pretest-to-posttest designs, each intervention was both preceded and followed by a measurement point for data collection (of the dependent variables: EFs and academic achievement). Prior to the intervention, information about age, gender, PA level, and socioeconomic status was collected using questionnaires. Height and weight (for calculating the body mass index, BMI), aerobic fitness and gross motor coordination were determined using standardized measures and tests. For the manipulation check, children’s step counts were measured using accelerometers throughout one specific hour (10:30 a.m.– 11:30 a.m.) over five consecutive school days. It was ensured that the PA break was implemented during this hour.

Participants

Based on a recent meta-analysis in children targeting EFs [41], a small to moderate effect (f = .12) of classroom based physical activity breaks on the core EF was expected. A priori power analysis using G*Power [42] with power (1- β) set at 0.80 and α = .05 showed that a sample size of N = 138 was requested to reach statistical significance at the .05 level. To recruit participants for the current study, teachers and headmasters were directly contacted. Altogether, 142 participants ranging from 7 to 9 years (M = 7.91 years, SD = 0.40; 54.9% girls) from the region of Bern, Switzerland, were included in this study. The exclusion criteria required for participants to have no formal diagnosis of ADHD, dyslexia, dyscalculia and colorblindness. Classes were randomly assigned to conditions using randomizer.org. There was some data loss due to sickness, technical problems with the tablets, or non-participation in the fitness tests because of injury. The percentage of pupils with incomplete values was 3.9% at pre-test and 5.9% at post-test. Since the missing completely at random (MCAR) test according to Little [43] was not significant (χ2 (20) = 13.93, p = .834), the missing values were estimated with the help of the expectation–maximization (EM) algorithm. Hence, a complete set of data was used for the statistical analyses.

There were no significant differences between the three experimental conditions with respect to age (F(2, 139) = 1.76, p = .176, ηp2 = .025), gender distribution (χ2(2) = 0.24, p = .887, Cramer’s V = .041), PA level (F(2, 139) = 2.31, p = .103, ηp2 = .032), socioeconomic status (F(2, 139) = 0.90, p = .408, ηp2 = .013), BMI (F(2, 139) = 0.23, p = .795, ηp2 = .003), aerobic fitness (F(2, 139) = 1.90, p = .153, ηp2 = .027) and gross motor coordination (F(2, 139) = 0.51, p = .599, ηp2 = .007).

General procedure

The interventions were carried out in the classroom by the regular class teachers, which were blinded with regard to the specific hypotheses. Two times 10 minutes sessions per day over a period of 20 weeks should be carried out, whereby the entire intervention was intended to cover 200 PA breaks. In each group, prior to the study, teachers completed a half-day training program instructing them in the basic principles, aims and purposes of the intervention program, demonstrating the specific contents with the special teaching materials. To test implementation compliance [44] teachers had to report the number of PA breaks effectively carried out, including the implementation accuracy. The teachers reported that they had implemented M = 145.4 of the prescribed 200 PA breaks (range between 107–172). The realized number of PA breaks during the 20 weeks did not differ between the three conditions (Mcognition = 151.8, SD = 29.7 vs. Maerobic = 125.0, SD = 21.6 vs. Mcombo = 157.3, SD = 14.6; F(2,7) = 1.61, p = .267, ηp2 = .315). Concerning the implementation accuracy, teachers reported that they could implement the PA breaks at a level of M = 3.4 using a 4-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = “not at all” to 4 = “exactly as the description”). The three conditions did not differ in terms of implementation accuracy (Mcognition = 3.6, SD = 0.09 vs. Maerobic = 2.9, SD = 0.60 vs. Mcombo = 3.5, SD = 0.40; F(2,7) = 2.36, p = .165, ηp2 = .403).

Prior to the study, background variables were measured at the respective schools. In addition, each child completed the same EF testing twice: before (pre-test) and after (post-test) the intervention, each completed in one session. The cognitive testing took place in a quiet room in groups of four children. Two investigators, who were blinded with respect to the conditions, performed the testing. They gave general instructions whilst the children were encouraged to work quietly, but encouraged to ask questions about the test whenever something was unclear. The cognitive tasks were completed on tablets, and children received the instructions over headphones with supportive images on the screen. To complete the tasks at their own pace and without distraction, the children were seated far apart from each other. Academic achievement (mathematics, spelling, reading) was also assessed before and after the intervention, using three standardized tests. However, the tests for academic achievement were not completed on the same day as the EF testing, to reduce the impact of increased cognitive load as a confounding variable. The academic achievement testing took place as a group testing in the classroom. Two separate blinded investigators with teaching experience tested the children during one regular school lesson. The overall testing was around 45 minutes and started in every class at 10.15 a.m. After the general instructions were advocated to the class, the mathematic test started (10.15 a.m.– 10.40 a.m.). After a short break of 5 minutes, the children were tested in their spelling performance (10.45 a.m.– 10.55 a.m.), and finally in their reading performance (10.55 a.m.– 11.05 a.m.).

Experimental conditions

Combo group (high cognitive engagement, high physical exertion; n = 47): This intervention consisted of specifically designed PA breaks tailored to challenge EFs (more information regarding the intervention can be obtained from the first author). For example, children were standing in a circle and playing the game “Horserace”. They had to run on the spot until the teacher said a key word. Whenever one of the keywords (starting with three different key words) was mentioned, the children had to react as quickly as possible with a predefined movement. For example, when they heard the word “hurdle” they had to jump up and then keep on running. The game was played for around 3 minutes, following incremental levels of difficulty (e.g. an additional keyword was defined, or the predefined movement was changed). Such rule changes intended to keep the game cognitively demanding, where the children were required to adapt their movements to the key words. Thus, the repetition with additional keywords and altered corresponding movements was crucial in this exercise. The children had to update the new information, inhibit the movements from the previous run which were no longer correct, and shift between the different words and their corresponding new movements.

Aerobic group (low cognitive engagement, high physical exertion; n = 49): This condition was designed to promote children’s aerobic fitness. Although it is not possible to exclude cognitive engagement entirely from long-term PA interventions, the attempt was made to choose exercises that had as little cognitive demand as possible. For example, the same game as mentioned in the combo group (“horserace”) was also played in the aerobic group but without the cognitive demands. The children were standing in the circle and had to run on the spot. The teacher acted out a movement (e.g. jumping) which the children had to imitate. Therefore, the children were only required to imitate the movements without remembering the correct movements relevant to the keywords. The same physical components were repeated to guarantee that PA in this group did not differ from the PA breaks in the combo intervention, regarding the physical intensity and/or the amount of social interaction.

Cognitive group (high cognitive engagement, low physical exertion; n = 46):

This intervention consisted of specifically designed breaks tailored to stimulate EFs by targeting fine motor skills. Instead of standing in a circle, the children sat in a circle and played the “horserace” game without any physical exertion. Using the same three keywords, the children were instructed to react as quickly as possible with their arms and fingers whenever they heard a keyword. For example, when they heard “hurdle”, they had to imitate a jump from a horse with their arms. After about 3 minutes, the level of difficulty was increased with either an additional keyword or by changing the matching movement, equivalent to the rules in the combo condition.

Manipulation check variables

To test the experimental manipulation, objective as well as subjective measures were used. To assess the physical exertion in the three experimental conditions, children’s step counts were objectively measured using ActiGraph GT3X (ActiGraph LLC, Pensacola, FL, USA). The average number of children’s step counts and the percentage of time spent in moderate-to-vigorous intensity was calculated using the PA of one specific hour (10:30 a.m.– 11:30 a.m.) over five consecutive school days. It was ensured that the PA break was implemented during this hour in the three experimental condition.

Perceived cognitive engagement was measured with an adapted version of the Self-Assessment Manikin [45]. The average of 10 rated PA breaks (two PA breaks a day during five school days) was calculated. The Self-Assessment-Manikin is a widely used non-verbal pictorial assessment technique to measure a person’s affective reaction to a variety of stimuli. It consists of one item for each construct. Acceptable psychometric properties have been demonstrated by Bradley and Lang [45]. As in the original Self-Assessment Manikin, for example when judging arousal, the children had to rate their perceived cognitive engagement ranging from 1 (“not cognitively engaging at all”) to 9 (“very, very cognitively engaging”). The question they had to answer was: “how cognitively engaging was the previous activity for your brain?”. Even though the instrument has not been validated, it has been shown to be feasible in children [40] and adolescents [46].

To test whether the three conditions were comparable with respect to induced pleasure, children’s pleasure was measured using the original Self-Assessment-Manikin [45]. The mean of 10 rated PA breaks (five running school days) was calculated. Acceptable reliability and validity has been shown, with 7 to 11 year old children being able to make dimensional ratings of pleasure and arousal in ways similar to adults [47].

Cognitive assessment

The core EFs were measured by two customized tablet-based tasks using E-Prime Software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA). Each task took about 12 minutes to complete and the order of the two tasks was counterbalanced between participants.

Updating was assessed with the Backwards Colour Recall task [7, 48]. The task is embedded in a cover story about a dwarf who loses sequences of coloured discs, starting with a two-disc sequence. The discs were presented for 1 s, separated by interstimulus-intervals of 500 ms. The children were asked to recall the sequences in the reverse order (by pressing the correct colour disc on the tablet). The two practice blocks (two- and three-disc sequence) included 3 trials, with a feedback loop whenever 66% of the trials were incorrect. Sequence length was increased by one disc when 50% of the six trials were correct, otherwise the task was interrupted. The total score of trials recalled correctly was used as the dependent measure. Acceptable retest reliability of this measure has been demonstrated in young (4- to 5-year old) children [48].

Inhibition was assessed with a child-adapted version of the Eriksen flanker task [49]. The fish flanker task is considered as the child version of the Attention Network Test [50], and has widely been applied in developmental research [5153] including exercise and cognition studies [10, 23, 54, 55]. The task consisted of two different blocks including five practice trials per block, and a feedback loop whenever the performance was below 60%. The “pure” block consisted of 16 congruent trials, and the “standard” block consisted of both 16 congruent and 16 incongruent trials, presented randomly. Inter-stimuli-intervals varied randomly from 800 to 1400 ms [51, 54]. As a dependent measure for inhibition, the conflict score between incongruent (highest rate of distraction in the standard block) and congruent trials (lowest rate of distraction in the pure block) was calculated [53, 56].

Shifting was assessed with an additional “mixed” block within the flanker task. A new rule cued by different coloured trials was introduced. Children had to adapt their response relating to the colour. Whenever the colour of the trials changed, a switch between the two rules was required. As before, a total of 16 congruent and 16 incongruent trials were randomly presented. Inter-stimuli-intervals varied randomly from 800 to 1400 ms [51, 54]. As a dependent variable for shifting, the global switch costs was calculated [57], determined as the difference between the mixed block and the standard block. Hence, the inhibition components within the “mixed” block were controlled (trials in the mixed block not only required shifting between different tasks, but also involve inhibitory demands).

Academic achievement assessment

Children’s academic achievement (mathematics, spelling, reading) was assessed using three standardized academic achievement tests for second graders.

Mathematics performance was measured using the two subscales “arithmetic operations” and “visual-spatial functions” containing six subtests from the Heidelberger Rechentest (HRT 1–4) [58]. For each subtest a time limit between 30 s and three minutes is given. The t-score was calculated, which reflects the deviation of the age-related mean score. Evidence for the reliability and validity of the Heidelberger Rechentest has been provided by Haffner et al. [58].

Spelling was measured using the Hamburger Schreib-Probe (HSP 1–10) [59], where children are required to spell eight words and a sentence that were read aloud to them, without any time pressure. As a measure of writing, the number of correctly spelled graphemes was calculated. The t-scores were calculated, which reflects the deviation of the age-related mean score. Test-retest reliability for graphemes (rtt = .97) has been shown by May [59].

Reading was measured using the Salzburger Lesescreening [60]. Children needed to read as many sentences as possible within 3 minutes and check if the statement is correct or not. The reading quotient, reflecting the z-standardized mean, was calculated. The test has been demonstrated to have acceptable reliability and validity [60].

Background variables

The Physical Activity Questionnaire for Children (PAQ-C) [61] was used to measure the general PA level. The PAQ-C is a 7-day self-administered recall measure that provides a PA summary score derived from nine items. The response format varies by item, but each is scored on a 5-point scale, a sample item being: “In the last 7 days, how many times did you do sports or PA after school?”. Response options range from: “None” (1 point) to “5 times last week” (5 points). Evidence for the reliability and validity of the questionnaire in 8- to 16-year-olds has been provided by Crocker et al. [61]. The Family Affluence Scale II (FAS II) [62] was used to assess the socioeconomic status. The scale consists of 4 questions asking children things they are likely to know about their family (e.g. number of family-owned cars, computers, number of family holidays in the past year, and having an own bedroom at home). A sample item is: “Does your family own a car, van or truck?” Response options are: no (0 points); yes, one (1 point); yes, two or more (2 points). The response format varies by item. The prosperity index (ranging from 0 to 9) was calculated from the sum of the three items. Evidence for the reliability and validity has been provided by [63]. The BMI was calculated as the body weight (in kg) divided by the square of the height (in m).

Aerobic fitness was assessed using the Multistage 20 metre Shuttle Run test [64]. Participants have to run back and forth on a 20 m course as instructed by a sound signal emitted from a pre-recorded tape, ensuring they touch the 20 m line with their foot. The frequency of the sound signal increases by 0.5 km/h every minute, indicating the next stage (level), starting with a speed of 8.5 km/h. The test ends when participants fail to reach the line before the signal. Evidence for the reliability and validity of the 20 metre Shuttle Run test has been provided by Liu, Plowman, and Looney and McVeigh, Payne, and Scott [65, 66].

Gross motor coordination. Children’s gross motor coordination was measured using the “Körperkoordinationstest für Kinder” [67]. The children performed the four subtests: a) walking backwards b) moving sideways c) hopping for height and d) jumping sideways. Points were given for each test item to make up the overall motor quotient (MQ) under consideration of gender and age factor. The calculated internal consistency of the four tests was acceptable (α = .79). A test-retest reliability of r = .97 of the KTK was reported by Kiphard and Schilling [67].

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 24.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). In the outlier analysis, trials with a reaction time under 150 ms were excluded. In a next step, trials with reaction times deviating by more than 3 SD from the child’s mean were also excluded. Only correct trials were included in the calculation of reaction times. Subsequently, blocks with an accuracy of less or equal to 50% were excluded assuming that those children seemed to have either not understood the task, or to have completed it incorrectly due to a lack of motivation. In preliminary analyses, separate ANOVAs were completed to test: (a) the potential between-group differences in background variables (age, general PA level, socioeconomic status, BMI, aerobic fitness, gross motor coordination), (b) the between group differences in pre-test values of the dependent variables (updating, inhibition, shifting, mathematics, spelling, reading) and finally (c) the differences in manipulation check variables (mean step counts during one hour, perceived physical exertion, perceived cognitive engagement, pleasure during PA breaks) (see Table 1). Partial eta square (ηp2) was reported as an estimate of effect size. When the overall ANOVA proved significance, Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons were used to determine the specific differences between the three groups. In the main analyses, ANCOVAs were conducted using the pre-test values of each dependent variable as covariate. The level of significance was set at p < .05 for all analyses. Table 2 shows means and standard deviations for accuracy and reaction times in the three core EFs at pre- and post-test for the three groups. Table 3 shows means and standard deviations for academic achievement (mathematics, spelling, reading) at pre- and post-test for the three groups.

thumbnail
Table 1. Means (and standard deviations) for the background, the manipulation check and the dependent variables in academic achievement in the three experimental conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Means and standard deviations in the Flanker task and the Backwards Colour Recall task at pre- and post-test for the three different groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Means and standard deviations in mathematics, spelling and reading at pre- and post-test for the three different groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482.t003

Results

Preliminary analyses

At pre-test, ANOVAs revealed no significant group differences in background variables (age, general PA level, socioeconomic status, BMI, aerobic fitness, gross motor coordination,), Fs(2, 139) ≤ 1.90, n.s., in academic achievement (mathematics, spelling, reading) Fs(2, 139) ≤ 2.96, n.s., nor in inhibition and shifting Fs (2, 139) ≤ 2.65, n.s. Only updating revealed a significant group difference at pre-test (F = 7.98, p = .001, ηp2 = .103). To test whether physical exertion in the three experimental conditions differed, the mean step counts were compared between the three groups. The manipulation check analyses revealed significant differences of children’s mean step counts (F (2, 139) = 40.75, p < .0005, ηp2 = .370) and time spent in moderate-to-vigorous PA (F (2, 139) = 31.33, p < .0005, ηp2 = .311) during the measured timeframe of 60 minutes. The post-hoc tests revealed that the cognition group had significantly lower physical exertion (mean step counts) compared to both, the combo (ps < .0005) and the aerobic group (ps < .0005), whereas the combo group and the aerobic group did not differ from each other (ps > .386). To check whether the perceived cognitive engagement differed between the three experimental conditions, the perceived cognitive engagement was compared between the three groups. As expected, the three groups differed significantly (F(2, 139) = 9.35, p < .0005, ηp2 = .119). Post-hoc tests showed that the aerobic group had a significantly lower perceived cognitive engagement compared with the combo group (p < .0005) and the cognition group (p = .048), whereas the combo group and the cognition group did not differ between each other (p = .209). Furthermore, children’s pleasure during the five PA breaks did not differ between the three groups (F(2, 139) = 1.67, p = .191, ηp2 = .024).

Main analyses

To test the main hypotheses of the study, the three groups were compared regarding the three core EFs between pre- and post-test. The three separate ANCOVAs revealed that updating (F(3, 138) = 0.16, p = .856, ηp2 = .002) and inhibition (F(3, 138) = 0.68, p = .507, ηp2 = .010) did not differ significantly between the three groups. However, shifting differed significantly between the groups (F(3, 138) = 4.68, p = .011, ηp2 = .064). Post-hoc analysis revealed a significantly greater improvement in shifting performance in the combo group than in the aerobic group (p = .003), but no significant difference between the combo group and the cognition group (p = .176). The aerobic and cognition group did not differ significantly from each other (p = .095). The results are depicted in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Means and error bars (representing the standard error of the mean) for the change (Δ) in the three core EFs (updating, inhibition, shifting) in the three groups between pre- and post-test.

RT = reaction time. *p < .05 .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482.g001

For academic achievement, only mathematics differed significantly between the three groups (F(2, 138) = 7.34, p = .001, ηp2 = .096), with post-hoc tests revealing both the combo (p = .001) and the cognition group (p = .002) to displaying a significantly better mathematic performance when compared against the aerobic group (see Fig 2). The combo group and the cognition group, both characterized as cognitively engaging interventions, did not differ significantly from each other (p = .901). No significant differences were found between the three groups in both spelling (F(2, 138) = 1.26, p = .287, ηp2 = .018) and reading scores (F(2, 138) = 1.46, p = .236, ηp2 = .021).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Means and error bars (representing the standard error of the mean) for the change (Δ) in the three academic achievement tests (mathematics, spelling,reading) in the three groups between pre- and post-test.

*p < .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482.g002

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of three qualitatively different PA interventions, with distinguishable levels of cognitive engagement and physical exertion, on primary school children’s EFs and academic achievement. It was hypothesized that a PA intervention consisting of both high cognitive engagement and high physical exertion would have a stronger impact on children’s core EFs and academic achievement than a “low” aerobic or a “low” cognitive intervention alone. In summary, the results showed (1) that only the combo intervention (high cognitive engagement and high physical exertion) fostered significant increases in children’s shifting performance, whereas updating and inhibition remained unaffected and (2) that the two cognitively challenging interventions enhanced children’s mathematic performance significantly more than the aerobic intervention. However, spelling and reading performance could not be improved through any of the three interventions. The current results are in line with previous research showing a higher improvement on EFs for those PA interventions with higher amounts of cognitive engagement [23, 68]. In terms of academic achievement, the current results confirm previous studies showing positive effects on children’s mathematic performance after acute PA breaks [69] and long-term classroom-based PA [38].

Considering the existing endurance-orientated literature [7072] supporting the positive effects of chronic aerobic fitness interventions, an improvement in cognitive function was expected in the aerobic group. Surprisingly, the current results showed that “pure” physical PA breaks with little cognitive effort are not able to enhance specific cognitive components, including the core EFs as well as more global cognitive components, such as academic achievement. This may be explained by the setting of classroom-based PA and its limitations. Due to time and room limitations, compared to physical education and after school programs, classroom-based PA is limited to smaller durations and reduced intensities. Analyses of this current study revealed that only whereas the rest of the time was spent in low to moderate PA. According to Chang et al. [73], heart rates ranging between 70% - 85%, indicating moderate-to-vigorous intensity, seem to benefit cognitive outcomes the most. The suggested range of heart rates of 70% - 85% was hard to reach in this study due to limited space in the classroom. Another plausible explanation is that “pure” aerobic PA does not automatically lead to improved cognitive functions and academic achievement due to the lack of CE in these PA breaks. This explanation is supported by the findings of Schmidt et al. [23], where systematically varying the amount of CE and physical exertion between groups demonstrated no significant improvement in children’s cognitive performance following a “pure” aerobic 6-week intervention, despite their equal increase in aerobic fitness.

According to the cognitive stimulation hypotheses suggested by Tomporowski et al. [18], the improvement in EFs and academic achievement in both of the cognitively engaging intervention groups (combo group and cognition group) was to be expected, along with greater positive effects in the combo group (high CE and physical exertion). The current results confirm the hypotheses showing, that the combo intervention elicited improvements in both children’s shifting and mathematic performance. Surprisingly, the cognition group (high CE, low physical exertion) only demonstrated a beneficial effect in mathematic performance, but no improvement with respect to shifting performance. Results of the manipulation check–showing equally high CE for the combo group as well as for the cognition group–suggest that CE is not the only crucial characteristic responsible for the cognitive gains in terms of EFs. Evidence from an RCT-study [70] including a highly controlled exercise intervention and standardized achievement tests, supports the assumption that different PA intervention types have selective effects on children’s cognitive functions. It seems that EFs are harder to modulate through long-term classroom-based PA interventions compared to academic achievement. This assumption is supported by the meta-analyses of Watson and colleagues [24], showing improvements in academic achievement, yet no effect on cognitive functions, such as EFs, through classroom-based PA. It is likely that only the correct dosage of CE, PA type, duration and intensity enhance children’s cognitive outcomes efficiently.

To bring up a suitable theoretical framework to help explain the current findings, the strength model of self-control, revised by Audiffren and André [74], can be addressed. Self-regulation and EFs share effort as a resource to perform stressful or attentional demanding tasks, such as cognitively challenging PA. If children engage in behaviors requiring a lot of cognitive effort, the cognitive resource is depleted in a subsequent task. However, the training hypotheses postulates an analogy to a trained muscle that the capacity for self-control will decrease after an acute intervention but result in an increased capacity after a long-term intervention. An overload of cognitive capacities performing cognitively engaging PA may lead to an immediate decrease in shifting performance. Referring to the theory of “supercompensation” from the field of applied training science, which describes a decrease in muscle performance immediately after a bout of acute physical exercise but increases in performance after chronic PA, one might speculate that cognitively challenging PA have a similar effect on cognitive functions. Two previous studies [23, 55] investigating acute as well as chronic effects of cognitive engaging PA on children’s EFs may underline these suggestions. Whereas no beneficial effects of acute PA without CE were found, the chronic intervention showed an improved shifting performance after 6 weeks of a physical team game program with a high CE.

The fact that only shifting was positively affected by the combo intervention, while updating and inhibition were not, needs to be discussed in detail regarding the specific content of the intervention. Shifting as a domain of EFs is stimulated when a quick shift between several tasks is required [5]. After counting the total number of required shifting performances inherent in the implemented PA breaks, more shifting performances were required, compared with inhibition and updating actions. It seems that updating and inhibition are harder to implement through a classroom-based PA setting, whereas shifting is likely to be a more immanent dimension of PA.

Both of the cognitively challenging PA interventions (combo group and cognition group) showed an increased mathematic performance after the 20-week intervention. The results, showing only a positive effect for mathematic performance and not for reading and spelling is supported by the review of Donnelly and colleagues [2] concluding that out of four classroom-based PA interventions, three could show a positive effect on children’s mathematic performance. In addition, a recent systematic review and expert panel comes to the conclusion that there is strong evidence for the beneficial effects of PA on mathematics [4]. The obtained result, therefore, is not surprising with respect to the successful enhancement of shifting performance for the combo group. EFs in general are predictors for academic achievement in children and adolescents [75]. Shifting, one of the core EFs, seems to predict academic performance most [8], particularly mathematic performance as shown by Latzman, Elkovitch, Young, and Clark [76] and Yeniad et al. [77]. Shifting is built on updating and inhibition and is strongly related to problem solving, which is a higher-ordered EF [5]. The relationship between shifting and mathematic performance can be explained by improvements in shifting abilities, which are in turn needed to perform different problem-solving strategies, such as flexibly shifting attention to relevant tasks, and moving back and forth between different types of task. Hence, these abilities are required when trying to solve a complex math problem. Although this explanation is quite conceivable and there is some empirical evidence deriving from developmental psychological literature [78, 79] it is hard to adopt it in the present study. The results are showing a better mathematic performance for both cognitively challenging groups (cognition and combo group), but only the combo group improved in terms of shifting performance. One possible explanation could be found in a potential improvement of children’s fine-motor skills (which unfortunately was not measured) through the cognitive intervention. As suggested by Pitchford, Papini, Outhwaite, and Gulliford [80] the influence of fine motor training is correlated with mathematic performance. Finger counting could be the linking mechanism between fine-motor skills and mathematical skills [81, 82].

Like any study, the present study has certain limitations, which need to be addressed. As usual for designs in school-based settings, the randomization of participants was done on a class-level. Further, since time was limited during testing, and the participating children already had large time commitments, the assessment of potential confounders had to be brief. Therefore, we were unable to measure important variables such as disability or language status. However, the teachers were asked in advance whether the participants in their class had any disabilities or language deficits. As a further limitation, it has to be mentioned that each of the three core EF was assessed by only one task. Using multiple tasks per EF component would have been advantageous [83]. However, with respect to ecologically valid implementations and large time commitments for the children involved, it served as a suitable method to test all three core EFs together. However, including all three core EFs is a benefit to give concrete practical advice. In terms of the core EFs, the results support the promotion of shifting performance through classroom-based PA. From a developmental point of view this is a feasible proposal, as shifting is the last EF to be fully developed, and therefore might be more sensitive to positive changes through PA interventions. One might speculate, therefore, that aspects of EFs which are not yet fully developed (as shifting) might be easier to change. This relationship between age and the responsiveness to chronic PA in terms of benefits on EFs needs to be explored in future studies. The implementation of daily PA breaks over a period of 20-weeks is challenging for teachers. The subjective treatment protocols show that the first 10 weeks of intervention, 77% of the breaks were implemented, following by a decrease of 7% in the second half of the intervention. In long-term studies using teachers to implement the interventions over a longer period, treatment fidelity, including direct observation, weekly supervision or periodic meetings [84] are needed and should be considered in the future.

Furthermore, the current study lacks an individually adjusted level of cognitive engagement and physical exertion during the PA breaks. A cognitive under- or overload might be prevented by examining relevant individual characteristics in advance, including sport-specific cognitive expertise, gross motor coordination and aerobic fitness [85]. Consequently, a personally fitted intervention for each child would be possible. However, due to room and time limitations, implementing an individualized cognitive and PA level intervention is a great challenge for future studies, especially for long-term classroom-based PA interventions. Future studies should acknowledge the separate effects of cognitively engaging and cognitively non-engaging interventions, where “cognitively engaging PA” should be defined as a specific type of PA in comparing different PA types in terms of most effective cognitive outputs.

In conclusion, this study adds to the literature on active breaks in schools. A feasible and innovative long-term intervention including cognitive and physical demands was developed and evaluated. Moreover, results revealed that only a combination of long-term PA breaks with high cognitive engagement leads to a stronger improvement in EFs, although not all core EFs seem to be affected equally. Shifting, as one of the core EFs, and its relationship to academic achievement was supported in this present study, where results displayed improvements in mathematic performance. Shifting, therefore, seems to be sensitive to cognitively engaging PA within the preadolescent period, and should be focussed in futures classroom-based PA interventions. Besides physical education, classroom-based PA breaks are a further opportunity, not only to enhance daily PA time, but also to improve children’s cognitive outcomes. High-quality PA, such as a combination of both PA and CE, seems to be the most effective if the adjustment of quantitative characteristics is considered as well.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Dataset underlying the findings of the current study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212482.s001

(SAV)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all schools, teachers and their students who were willing to participate in our study. We also gratefully acknowledge the help of our students, Claudia Böni, Phil Bonadimann, Caroline Christen, Rebecca Füglister, Mario Kamer, Eliane Koch, Aleksandra Lato, Lars Lenze and Pascal Stegmann with the data collection and Christof Imhof with the applied informatics in E-Prime.

References

  1. 1. Diamond A, Ling DS. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016;18:34–48. pmid:26749076
  2. 2. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, et al. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: A systematic review. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2016;48:1223–4. pmid:27182987
  3. 3. de Greeff JW, Hartman E, Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S, Visscher C. Long-term effects of physically active academic lessons on physical fitness and executive functions in primary school children. Health Education Research. 2016;31:185–94. pmid:26826113
  4. 4. Singh AS, Saliasi E, van den Berg V, Uijtdewilligen L, de Groot RHM, Jolles J, et al. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents: A novel combination of a systematic review and recommendations from an expert panel. British Journal of Sports Medicine 2018. pmid:30061304
  5. 5. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2013;64:135–68. pmid:23020641
  6. 6. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41:49–100. pmid:10945922
  7. 7. Roebers CM, Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Cimeli P, Michel E, Jäger K. The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children’s transition to school: A latent variable approach. Human Movement Science. 2014;33:284–97. pmid:24289983
  8. 8. Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008;33:205–28. pmid:18473197
  9. 9. Geertsen SS, Thomas R, Larsen MN, Dahn IM, Andersen JN, Krause-Jensen M, et al. Motor skills and exercise capacity are associated with objective measures of cognitive functions and Academic Performance in Preadolescent Children. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0161960. pmid:27560512
  10. 10. Schmidt M, Egger F, Benzing V, Jäger K, Conzelmann A, Roebers CM, Pesce C. Disentangling the relationship between children’s motor ability, executive function and academic achievement. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0182845. pmid:28817625
  11. 11. van Praag H. Neurogenesis and exercise: Past and future directions. NeuroMolecular Medicine. 2008;10:128–40. pmid:18286389
  12. 12. Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie L-A. Exercise builds brain health: Key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30:464–72. pmid:17765329
  13. 13. Rigoli D, Piek JP, Kane R, Oosterlaan J. Motor coordination, working memory, and academic achievement in a normative adolescent sample: Testing a mediation model. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2012;27:766–80. pmid:22777140
  14. 14. van der Niet A. G., Hartman E, Smith J, Visscher C. Modeling relationships between physical fitness, executive functioning, and academic achievement in primary school children. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2014;15:319–25.
  15. 15. Pesce C. Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology. 2012;34:766–86.
  16. 16. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review. 2010;30:331–51. pmid:21818169
  17. 17. Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pendleton DM, Pesce C. Exercise and children's cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015;4:47–55.
  18. 18. Tomporowski PD, Davis CL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Exercise and children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educational Psychology Review. 2008;20:111–31. pmid:19777141
  19. 19. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–64. pmid:21852486
  20. 20. Pesce C, Ben-Soussan TD. “Cogito ergo sum” or “ambulo ergo sum”? New perspectives in developmental exercise and cognition research. In: McMorris T, editor. Exercise-Cognition Interaction: Neuroscience Perspectives. London: Elsevier; 2016. p.251–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800778-5.00012–8
  21. 21. Vazou S, Pesce C, Lakes K, Smiley-Oyen A. More than one road leads to Rome: A narrative review and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on cognition in youth. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2016:1–26.
  22. 22. Koutsandreou F, Wegner M, Niemann C, Budde H. Effects of motor versus cardiovascular exercise training on children's working memory. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2016;48:1144–52. pmid:26765631
  23. 23. Schmidt M, Jäger K, Egger F, Roebers CM, Conzelmann A. Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: A group-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2015;37:575–91. pmid:26866766
  24. 24. Watson A, Timperio A, Brown H, Best K, Hesketh KD. Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2017;14:114. pmid:28841890
  25. 25. Carlson JA, Engelberg JK, Cain KL, Conway TL, Mignano AM, Bonilla EA, et al. Implementing classroom physical activity breaks: Associations with student physical activity and classroom behavior. Preventive Medicine. 2015;81:67–72. pmid:26297105
  26. 26. Grieco LA, Jowers EM, Errisuriz VL, Bartholomew JB. Physically active vs. sedentary academic lessons: A dose response study for elementary student time on task. Preventive Medicine. 2016;89:98–103. pmid:27235602
  27. 27. Szabo-Reed AN, Willis EA, Lee J, Hillman CH, Washburn RA, Donnelly JE. Impact of 3 years of classroom physical activity bouts on time-on-task behavior. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2017;49:2343–50. pmid:28614194
  28. 28. Beck MM, Lind RR, Geertsen SS, Ritz C, Lundbye-Jensen J, Wienecke J. Motor-enriched learning activities can improve mathematical performance in preadolescent children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016;10:645. pmid:28066215
  29. 29. Kibbe DL, Hackett J, Hurley M, McFarland A, Schubert KG, Schultz A, Harris S. Ten years of TAKE 10!: Integrating physical activity with academic concepts in elementary school classrooms. Preventive Medicine. 2011;52:43–50. pmid:21281670
  30. 30. Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S, Visscher C. Improving academic performance of school-age children by physical activity in the classroom: 1-year program evaluation. Journal of School Health. 2015;85:365–71. pmid:25877433
  31. 31. Webster CA, Russ L, Vazou S, Goh TL, Erwin H. Integrating movement in academic classrooms: Understanding, applying and advancing the knowledge base. Obesity Reviews. 2015;16:691–701. pmid:25904462
  32. 32. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Greene JL, Hansen DM, Gibson CA, Sullivan DK, et al. Physical activity and academic achievement across the curriculum: Results from a 3-year cluster-randomized trial. Preventive Medicine. 2017;99:140–5. pmid:28193490
  33. 33. Katz DL, Cushman D, Reynolds J, Njike V, Treu JA, Walker J, et al. Putting physical activity where it fits in the school day: Preliminary results of the ABC (Activity Bursts in the Classroom) for fitness program. Preventing Chronic Disease. 2010;7:1–10.
  34. 34. Reed JA, Einstein G, Hahn E, Hooker SP, Gross VP, Kravitz J. Examining the impact of integrating physical activity on fluid intelligence and academic performance in an elementary school setting: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2010;7:343–51. pmid:20551490
  35. 35. Ahamed Y, Macdonald H, Reed K, Naylor P-J, Liu-Ambrose , McKay H. School-based physical activity does not compromise children's academic performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2007;39:371–6. pmid:17277603
  36. 36. Mohr DC, Spring B, Freedland KE, Beckner V, Arean P, Hollon SD, et al. The selection and design of control conditions for randomized controlled trials of psychological interventions. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2009;78:275–84. pmid:19602916
  37. 37. Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA, Smith BK, Washburn RA, Sullivan DK, et al. Physical activity across the curriculum (PAAC): A randomized controlled trial to promote physical activity and diminish overweight and obesity in elementary school children. Preventive Medicine. 2009;49:336–41. pmid:19665037
  38. 38. Erwin H, Fedewa A, Ahn S. Student academic performance outcomes of a classroom physical activity intervention: A pilot study. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education. 2012;4:473–87.
  39. 39. Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Sullivan DK, Hansen DM, Hillman CH, Poggio J, et al. Physical activity and academic achievement across the curriculum (A + PAAC): Rationale and design of a 3-year, cluster-randomized trial. Bmc Public Health. 2013;13:1–8.
  40. 40. Egger F, Conzelmann A, Schmidt M. The effect of acute cognitively engaging physical activity breaks on children's executive functions: Too much of a good thing? Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2018;36:178–86.
  41. 41. de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Oosterlaan J, Visscher C, Hartman E. Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2018;21:501–7. pmid:29054748
  42. 42. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:175–91. pmid:17695343
  43. 43. Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical analysis with missing data: Hoboken, NJ:Wiley; 2002.
  44. 44. Dane AV, Schneider BH. Program integrity in primary and early secondary prevention:Are implementation effects out of control? Clinical Psychology Review. 1998;18:23–45. pmid:9455622
  45. 45. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 1994;25:49–59. pmid:7962581
  46. 46. Benzing V, Heinks T, Eggenberger N, Schmidt M. Acute cognitively engaging exergame-based physical activity enhances executive functions in adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0167501. pmid:28030542
  47. 47. McManis MH, Brady KT, Berg , Keith W., Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotional reactions in children: Verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses to affective pictures. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:222–231. pmid:11347868
  48. 48. Schmid C, Zoelch C, Roebers CM. Das Arbeitsgedächtnis von 4- bis 5-jährigen Kindern. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie. 2008;40:2–12.
  49. 49. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics. 1974;16:143–9.
  50. 50. Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, Posner MI. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1029–40. pmid:15093142
  51. 51. Roebers CM, Kauer M. Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science. 2009;12:175–81. pmid:19120425
  52. 52. Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Cimeli P, Michel E, Roebers CM. Improving executive functions in 5- and 6-year-olds: Evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Child Development. 2011;21:411–29.
  53. 53. Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:573–94. pmid:16144428
  54. 54. Jäger K, Schmidt M, Conzelmann A, Roebers CM. Cognitive and physiological effects of an acute physical activity intervention in elementary school children. Frontiersin Psychology. 2014;5:71. pmid:25566148
  55. 55. Jäger K, Schmidt M, Conzelmann A, Roebers CM. The effects of qualitatively different acute physical activity interventions in real-world settings on executive functions in preadolescent children. Mental Health and Physical Activity. 2015;9:1–9.
  56. 56. Fatzer ST, Roebers CM. Language and executive functions: The effect of articulatory suppression on executive functioning in children. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2012;13:454–72.
  57. 57. Chevalier N, Blaye A. Setting goals to switch between tasks: Effect of cue transparency on children’s cognitive flexibility. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:782–97. pmid:19413431
  58. 58. Haffner J, Baro K, Parzer P, Resch F. Heidelberger Rechentest (HRT 1–4): Erfassung mathematischer Basiskompetenzen im Grundschulalter. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2005.
  59. 59. May P. HSP–Hamburger Schreib-Probe 1–10 [Hamburg Writing Test 1–10]. Hamburg: vpm; 2012.
  60. 60. Mayringer H, Wimmer H. Salzburger Lesescreening (SLS 1–4). Bern: Huber; 2003.
  61. 61. Crocker PRE, Bailey DA, Faulkner RA, Kowalski KC, McGrath R. Measuring general levels of physical activity: Preliminary evidence for the physical activity questionnaire for older children. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 1997;29:1344–9.
  62. 62. Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W, Holstein B, Torsheim T, Richter M, et al. Researching health inequalities in adolescents: The development of the health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) family affluence scale. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;66:1429–36. pmid:18179852
  63. 63. Boudreau B, Poulin C. An examination of the validity of the family affluence scale II (FAS II) in a general adolescent population of Canada. Social Indicators Research. 2009;94:29–42.
  64. 64. Léger LA, Mercier D, Gadoury C, Lambert J. The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness. Journal of Sports Sciences. 1988;6:93–101. pmid:3184250
  65. 65. Liu NYS, Plowman SA, Looney MA. The reliability and validity of the 20-meter shuttle test in american students 12 to 15 years old. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1992;63:360–5. pmid:1439160
  66. 66. McVeigh SK, Payne AC, Scott S. The reliability and validity of the 20-meter shuttle test as a predictor of peak oxygen uptake in Edinburgh school children, age 13 to 14 years. Pediatric Exercise Science. 1995;7:69–79.
  67. 67. Kiphard EJ, Schilling F. Körperkoordinationstest für Kinder. 2nd ed. Göttingen: Beltz; 2007.
  68. 68. Crova C, Struzzolino I, Marchetti R, Masci I, Vannozzi G, Forte R, Pesce C. Cognitively challenging physical activity benefits executive function in overweight children. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2014;32:201–11. pmid:24015968
  69. 69. Howie EK, Schatz J, Pate RR. Acute effects of classroom exercise breaks on executive function and math performance: A dose-response study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2015;86:217–24. pmid:26009945
  70. 70. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, Boyle CA, Waller JL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA, Gregoski M. Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children's cognitive functioning: A randomized controlled trial. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2007;78:510–9. pmid:18274222
  71. 71. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH, Yanasak NE, et al. Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: A randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychology. 2011;30:91–8. pmid:21299297
  72. 72. Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, O’Leary KC, Scudder MR, Wu C-T, Castelli DM, Hillman CH. The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Developmental Science. 2011;14:1046–58. pmid:21884320
  73. 73. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. BRAIN RESEARCH. 2012;1453:87–101. pmid:22480735
  74. 74. Audiffren M, André N. The strength model of self-control revisited: Linking acute and chronic effects of exercise on executive functions. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015;4:30–46.
  75. 75. Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011;21:327–36. pmid:21845021
  76. 76. Latzman RD, Elkovitch N, Young J, Clark LA. The contribution of executive functioning to academic achievement among male adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2010;32:455–62. pmid:19813129
  77. 77. Yeniad N, Malda M, Mesman J, van Ijzendoorn MH, Pieper S. Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013;23:1–9.
  78. 78. Bull R, Lee K. Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives. 2014;8:36–41.
  79. 79. Friso-van den Bos I, van der Ven SHG, Kroesbergen EH, van Luit JEH. Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review. 2013;10:29–44.
  80. 80. Pitchford NJ, Papini C, Outhwaite LA, Gulliford A. Fine motor skills predict maths ability better than they predict reading ability in the early primary school years. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:783. pmid:27303342
  81. 81. Di Luca S, Pesenti M. Finger numeral representations: More than just another symbolic code. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011;2:272. pmid:22069392
  82. 82. Moeller K, Martignon L, Wessolowski S, Engel J, Nuerk H-C. Effects of finger counting on numerical development—the opposing views of neurocognition and mathematics education. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011;2:328. pmid:22144969
  83. 83. Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21:8–14. pmid:22773897
  84. 84. Smith SW, Daunic AP, Taylor GG. Treatment fidelity in applied educational research: Expanding the adoption and application of measures to ensure evidence-based practice. Education and Treatment of Children. 2007;30:121–134.
  85. 85. Pesce C. An integrated approach to the effect of acute and chronic exercise on cognition: the linked role of individual and task constraints. In: McMorris T, Tomporowski PD, Audiffren M, editors. Exercise and cognitive function. Chichester, UK, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Heinrich; 2009.