Correction
29 Jan 2019: Mannino G, Abdi G, Maffei ME, Barbero F (2019) Correction: Origanum vulgare terpenoids modulate Myrmica scabrinodis brain biogenic amines and ant behaviour. PLOS ONE 14(1): e0211749. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211749 View correction
Figures
Abstract
Coordinated social behaviour is fundamental for ant ecological success. However, even distantly-related organisms, such as plants, have evolved the ability to manipulate ant collective performances to their own advantage. In the parasitic system encompassing Maculinea butterflies, Myrmica ants, and Origanum vulgare plants, the ant-plant interaction elicits the release of a volatile terpenoid compound (carvacrol) which is used by the gravid butterfly to locate the ideal oviposition site. Here we show that this ant-plant association is maintained by the effect of O. vulgare terpenoids on ant behaviour and that food plants might gain protection by Myrmica ants by chemically manipulating workers to forage in their surroundings. The variation in the locomotor ability of three ant species (Formica cinerea, Tetramorium caespitum, and Myrmica scabrinodis) was studied after treatment with the two major O. vulgare terpenoid volatile compounds (i.e., carvacrol and thymol). The brain levels of three biogenic amines (dopamine, tyramine and serotonin) were analysed in ants exposed to the O. vulgare terpenoids by HPLC-ESI-MS/MS. Carvacrol and thymol increased the locomotor activity of all ant species tested, but if blended reduced the movement propensity of Myrmica scabrinodis. Dopamine and tyramine production was positively correlated with the worker locomotor activity. In Myrmica ants, both brain biogenic ammines were negatively correlated with the aggressive behaviour. Blends of O. vulgare volatiles affected the locomotor ability while increased the aggressiveness of Myrmica workers by altering the aminergic regulation in the ant brains. This behavioural manipulation, might enhance partner fidelity and plant protection. Our findings provide new insights supporting a direct role of plant volatiles in driving behavioural changes in social insects through biogenic amine modulation.
Citation: Mannino G, Abdi G, Maffei ME, Barbero F (2018) Origanum vulgare terpenoids modulate Myrmica scabrinodis brain biogenic amines and ant behaviour. PLoS ONE 13(12): e0209047. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209047
Editor: Wolfgang Blenau, Philipps-Universitat Marburg Fachbereich Biologie, GERMANY
Received: August 9, 2018; Accepted: November 27, 2018; Published: December 26, 2018
Copyright: © 2018 Mannino et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information.
Funding: This work was supported by the PhD Program in Pharmaceutical and Biomolecular Sciences of the University of Turin.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
The social organization of ants is based on communication signals which coordinate large numbers of individuals in collective-decision processes, without a centralised control [1]. Although some decisions are individually made, the majority of choices influencing the colony success are performed cooperatively. The decision-making occurs in the selection of the most appropriate food in terms of amount and quality [2], best migration or foraging route [3,4], most suitable place where to build a new nest [5,6] as well as decisions to attack either enemies or competitors [7,8]. This peculiar trait of eusocial organisms, together with the ability to regulate the number of individuals performing specific tasks (division of labour), enhances the colony plasticity to react promptly against biotic and abiotic variations [9,10].
Plant chemical signalling can manipulate and influence the decision-making of ants [11–13]. However, how the manipulation of social activities is achieved is not fully disentangled and the mechanism is likely to vary in distinct systems. A role of biogenic amines in driving behavioural changes of both vertebrates [14,15] and invertebrates [16–19], including ants [20–22], has been demonstrated. Research on ants is still in its infancy, yet some supporting evidence was provided for colonies of the Red Imported Fire Ant, Solenopsis invicta, with a change in the nest-mate recognition determined by a decrease in the brain level of octopamine [23], and in Pheidole ants, where the depletion in the serotonin content modulates the trail-following behaviour of workers [24].
In spite of their ecological dominance, ants suffer from social parasitism by several invertebrates [1]. Maculinea butterflies are outstanding examples of cheaters employing both chemical [25] and acoustical [26] strategies to exploit their Myrmica host ants. In this multitrophic system, the ant-butterfly association is obligately parasitic and involves also the species-specific plant, used as the egg-laying site by butterfly females. In the case of Maculinea arion, early-instar butterfly larvae feed on Origanum vulgare, until they drop on the ground where thanks to chemical mimicry are retrieved by Myrmica foragers and taken into the ant nest. Here M. arion larvae spend the next ten months consuming the resources of the colony and feeding on the ant brood until the adult will eventually emerge from the nest [27].
Recently, it has been demonstrated that the interaction between Myrmica ants and O. vulgare plants leads to an increased production of the plant volatile monoterpene carvacrol [28]. The release of carvacrol is the indirect signal interpreted by M. arion gravid females to locate the food plant growing in the vicinity of its Myrmica host colonies, thus providing its brood with both sequential hosts (i.e., the source of food, shelter and care in the nest) [28,29]. Myrmica ants survive longer than other ant species when exposed to carvacrol, by upregulating specific genes coding for detoxifying enzymes [28]. Therefore, it seems that Myrmica ants benefit from being resistant to carvacrol by occupying a competitor-free spaces surrounding oregano plants, while parasite larvae increase their chance of encountering ants [28,29].
The mechanism that induces Myrmica ants to forage and found their nests in the proximity of oregano plants is still unknown. Here we show that thymol and carvacrol produced by O. vulgare act on Myrmica behaviour by modifying the biogenic amine level in their brain, thus affecting their locomotor activity and the aggressive behaviour. We also tested and compared the effect of these two monoterpenes on other ant species (Tetramorium caespitum and Formica cinerea) and confirmed that behavioural changes are correlated to variation on terpenoid-modulated biogenic amines levels.
Materials and methods
Animal material
Colonies of Myrmica scabrinodis, Formica cinerea and Tetramorium caespitum were collected at the Parco Fluviale Gesso e Stura, North Italy (44°25’N, 7°35E, 440 m), where a previous study on acute exposure of ants to specific O. vulgare volatiles was made [28]. Ant colonies were collected and reared in plastic boxes (24×24×9 cm). Ants were fed twice a week on a honey and protein diet [30].
Although our field study did not involve endangered or protected species, the Parco Fluviale Gesso e Stura issued the permission to collect ant specimens for each location.
Chemicals
GC-grade carvacrol and thymol were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO. USA). Pure standards (dopamine, tyramine, 3,4-dihydroxybenzylamine hydrobromide and serotonin) and formic acid were purchased from Merck (Darmstadt, Germany). Isopropanol, chloroform and acetonitrile were purchased from VWR International (Radnor, PA, USA).
Locomotor activity
Variations in the locomotor activity were evaluated after treatment of the three ant species with either carvacrol (C) or thymol (T), or either a 3:1 (v/v) (Ct) or a 1:3 (v/v) (Tc) mixture of carvacrol/thymol. A 477 μg ml-1 solution of the above chemical and mixture was used in order to provide a final concentration of 0.1 ppm inside the Petri dish (9 cm internal diameter × 1.5 cm height). 20 μL of each solution were poured on a small paper disk (5 mm diameter) which was left in the centre of a Petri dish. Two controls were used in the bioassays: the first to assess the effect of the artificial condition (blank) and the second to evaluate the effect of the solvent (DMSO) on the ant movements.
Afterwards, 18 F. cinerea, 30 M. scabrinodis and 30 T. caespitum ants were randomly chosen among foraging workers. Due to their different size, three F. cinerea ants (6–7 mm each) and five workers in the case of M. scabrinodis and T. caespitum (around 4 mm each) were placed per each Petri dish and allowed to settle for 5 minutes. Ants of each species were tested simultaneously in six Petri dishes (4 treatments and 2 controls). Three colonies per ant species were assayed (N = 9) and a total of 234 workers were observed.
Protocols described by Hojo and colleagues [31] were followed to assess the ant locomotion. A bisecting line (i.e., one diameter) was drawn on the lid of each Petri plate. The number of times each ant crossed the solid line was recorded for 1 h by a video camera (Panasonic HC-4k) and measured by operators who were blind to experimental condition. Ants were not marked, thus crossings were counted irrespectively of the individual. No deviant behaviour (e.g., stay still) of any individual was observed during the bioassay.
Aggression bioassays
Aggression bioassays were performed on Myrmica foraging workers of 3 colonies. All ants were marked on the thorax and allowed to recover before the aggression bioassay was carried out. Bioassays were performed as described by Csata et al. [32]. Briefly, two transparent plastic tubes (3 cm long) were joined and separated by a small piece of red plastic foil. One worker was placed per tube and after one minute the plastic foil was removed.
Individuals of the same colony were treated with either C, T, Ct or Tc. Ants were separated in a Petri dish each containing a single worker which was treated for 30 min with 0.1 ppm of the aforementioned compounds. Then bioassays were carried out testing non-nestmate (heterocolonial) ants with the following combinations, carvacrol vs carvacrol (CC), thymol vs thymol (TT), carvacrol/thymol vs carvacrol/thymol (Ct-Ct) and thymol/carvacrol vs thymol/carvacrol (Tc-Tc). Control tests were carried out using ants left in a Petri dish for 30 min without addition of compounds (CTRL). For each treatment, three tests using all possible combinations of the three colonies were replicated three times. Observations started with the first contact of the workers and lasted for three minutes. All behaviours were recorded, including biting, pulling, stinging, allogrooming and antennation. The latter two were considered as positive or neutral interactions, respectively, whilst the other behaviours were categorized as aggressive. The aggression index (AI) was calculated as the number of aggressive behaviours divided by the total number of interactions.
Ant brain dissection and sample preparation
Sample preparation and the dissection of ant brains were performed following the protocols by Hojo et al. [31]. Briefly, two weeks after freezing at -25°C, ants were rapidly beheaded under a dissection microscope using micro-scissors. A small medial-lateral incision was made directly behind the mandibles and, in order to prevent contamination by retinal pigments, optic lobes were removed from the rest of the brain. Each sample contained two brains with each dissection time averaging less than 1 min. Brains were homogenized in 20 μl water solution containing 0.05% (v/v) formic acid and 1 ng 3,4-di-hydroxybenzylamine (DHBA) used as an internal standard. After vortex mixing, samples were centrifuged at 10,000 g for 20 min at 4°C. In order to remove undesirable compounds, a solution of isopropanol/chloroform (1:4, v/v) was directly added to the supernatant in a 1:1 (v/v) ratio. Samples were centrifuged at the same conditions described above. The aqueous layer obtained after centrifugation was immediately frozen on dry ice and stored at -80°C until injection into an HPLC-ESI-MS/MS (1200 HPLC, Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) for amine quantification.
Isolation and quantification of biogenic amines by HPLC-ESI-MS/MS
Biogenic amines (serotonin, dopamine and tyramine) and the internal standard DHBA were identified and quantified by liquid chromatography (1200 HPLC, Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) equipped with a reverse phase Kinetex F5 Core-Shell LC Column (2,6 μm, 150 mm × 3.0 mm, 100 Å, Phenomenex, Torrance, CA, USA) which was kept at 40°C during chromatography. The binary solvent system was: (A) MilliQ H2O containing 0.1% (v/v) formic acid and (B) MeCN containing 0.1% (v/v) formic acid. The chromatographic separation was carried out at a constant flow rate (200 μl·min-1) with the following conditions: isocratic gradient at 3% B for the first 15 min, then 40% B in 5 min, then after 24 min B concentration was raised to 98%. The concentration of B was then maintained at 98% for 5 min. The initial mobile phase was re-established for 10 min before the next injection. The ionization of each amine was performed by electrospray ionization (ESI) operating in positive mode and tandem mass spectrometry analyses were performed with a 6330 Series Ion Trap LC-MS System (Agilent Technologies, USA). The flow rate of nitrogen was set at 325°C and 5.0 l·min-1, and the capillary voltage was set at 1.5 kV. Helium was used as a collision gas. Quantitative analyses were performed by Multiple Reaction Monitoring (MRM) by monitoring the fragmentation of quasi-molecular ions for serotonin (m/z: 177.1; 159.9), dopamine (m/z: 154.1; 137.0), tyramine (m/z: 138.0; 121.0) and DHBA (m/z: 140.0; 123.0). Quantification was performed by internal standard and external calibration curves with pure standards. Limit of Detections (LOD) and Limit of Quantification (LOQ) for each compound were determined as described in the complementary guideline of validation of analytical procedures [33] (see S1 Table).
Statistical analysis
Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were used to assess the data distribution. Aggression indices were compared using a linear mixed model (LMM) with the colony ID as a random factor and CC, TT, Ct-Ct, Tc-Tc, CTRL as fixed factors. For locomotor activity (LA) assays, data were analysed using a linear mixed model with cumulated number of crossings as a response variable, ant colony as a random intercept, and treatment (CTRL, DMSO, C, T, Ct, Tc) as fixed factors. Differences in the amount of each biogenic amine was tested by LMM using ant colony as a random factor and treatments (CTRL, DMSO, C, T, Ct, Tc) as fixed effect. Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD) post hoc was used to test pairwise comparisons. Pearson’s coefficients were used to test the correlation between chemical and behavioural data. All statistics were performed using SPSS package ver. 25.
Results
Oregano terpenoids modulate ant locomotor activity
Sublethal concentrations of carvacrol and thymol triggered a significant variation in the locomotor activity (LA) of the three ant species under study (Fig 1).
Data are expressed as the frequency of crossing the bisector traced on the Petri lid, taken as a proxy for worker locomotor activity affected by treatments (C: carvacrol; T: thymol; Ct: 3:1 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol; Tc: 1:3 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol) and controls (CTRL and DMSO) on Formica cinerea (A), Tetramorium caespitum (B) and Myrmica scabrinodis (C). Boxplots show median, quartile, maximum and minimum values; different letters indicate significant differences (Tukey’s HSD post hoc, P < 0.05; S2 Table).
Single or mixed compounds increased LA in F. cinerea with respect to controls (Fig 1A; F18,5 = 28.641; P < 0.001). Movements were enhanced especially when ants were treated with Tc and, to a lesser extent, with C and T. Although increased, LA of workers exposed to a blend with a larger amount of Ct did not vary significantly from controls (S2 Table). Differences in the LA were also found in treated and untreated T. caespitum workers (Fig 1B; F18,5 = 13.658; P < 0.001). Overall, treatments elicited an increase in ant movements, but this variation was significant only in response to Tc. A different response was found when LA was assayed in M. scabrinodis. Treatments affected the movements of M. scabrinodis (Fig 1C; F18,5 = 19.268; P < 0.001) and exposure to C and T induced an LA increase in workers. In contrast, both Ct and Tc caused a significant decrease in ant movements.
Oregano terpenoids modulate brain biogenic amine content
Treatments with C and T significantly affected the brain content of dopamine in F. cinerea (Fig 2A: F18,5 = 63.744, P < 0.001), T. caespitum (Fig 2B: F18,5 = 58.104, P<0.001) and M. scabrinodis (Fig 2C: F18,5 = 6.500, P = 0.006).
Effects of treatments (C: carvacrol; T: thymol; Ct: 3:1 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol; Tc: 1:3 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol) and controls (CTRL and DMSO) on dopamine contents in ant brains of Formica cinerea (A), Tetramorium caespitum (B) and Myrmica scabrinodis (C). Values are expressed as pmol dopamine per ant brain. Boxplots show median, quartile, maximum and minimum values; different letters indicate significant differences (Tukey’s HSD post hoc, p < 0.05; S3 Table).
Tyramine brain content was also significantly affected by Oregano terpenoids in F. cinerea (Fig 3A: F18,5 = 15.799, P < 0.001), T. caespitum (Fig 3B: F18,5 = 27.315, P < 0.001) and M. scabrinodis (Fig 3C: F18,5 = 5.963 P = 0.008).
Effects of treatments (C: carvacrol; T: thymol; Ct: 3:1 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol; Tc: 1:3 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol) and controls (CTRL and DMSO) on tyramine contents in ant brains of Formica cinerea (A), Tetramorium caespitum (B) and Myrmica scabrinodis (C). Values are expressed as pmol tyramine per ant brain. Boxplots show median, quartile, maximum and minimum values; different letters indicate significant differences (Tukey’s HSD post hoc, p < 0.05; S3 Table).
Irrespectively of the species, brain dopamine and tyramine contents were similarly affected by treatments, as they both increase or decrease in response to the same compound (all ant species Pearson correlation; ρ = 0.97, P < 0.001).
In F. cinerea, the highest dopamine content was found after Tc treatment. C and, to a lesser extent T and Ct, triggered increased dopamine levels (Fig 2A; S3 Table). A similar pattern was found for tyramine level, which was correlated to dopamine content (Pearson correlation; ρ = 0.96, P < 0.001). Significant changes in tyramine content were only reported between treatments and controls (Fig 3A; S3 Table).
In T. caespitum, C and Tc caused the largest increase in the dopamine and tyramine levels (Figs 2B and 3B), which showed a significant correlation (Pearson correlation ρ = 0.98, P < 0.001). T and Ct triggered a lower but significant effect, with respect to controls (Fig 2B; S3 Table).
In M. scabrinodis, the levels of brain dopamine and tyramine varied consistently (Pearson correlation. ρ = 0.99, P < 0.001). With respect to controls, C and T triggered an increase of both dopamine and tyramine (Figs 2C and 3C; S3 Table). Although not significant, an average decrease in tyramine is elicited by exposure to the two blends (Fig 3C; S3 Table).
The locomotor activity of all ant species was strongly correlated with changes in the dopamine (Pearson correlation; ρ = 0.94, P < 0.001) and tyramine (Pearson correlation; ρ = 0.91, P < 0.001) levels.
In contrast to dopamine and tyramine, no significant variation in the serotonin brain content was reported in all ant species (S1 Fig).
Oregano terpenoids induce M. scabrinodis aggression
Aggression behaviour was assessed in M. scabrinodis workers only, because this species is directly involved in the associations with O. vulgare plants and in the interaction with the Maculinea butterfly parasite. The level of aggression varied with exposure to different treatments (Fig 4: F45,4 = 8.031, P = 0.007).
Aggression index between heterocolonial M. scabrinodis workers treated with different compounds or pure air (CTRL: non-treated; CC: carvacrol vs. carvacrol; TT: thymol vs. thymol; Ct-Ct: 3:1 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol vs. 3:1 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol, and Tc-Tc 1:3 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol vs. 1:3 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol). Data are expressed as number of aggressive behaviours on the total number of interaction (see methods for details). Boxplots show median, quartile, maximum and minimum values; different letters indicate significant differences (Tukey’s HSD post hoc, p < 0.05; S4 Table).
Aggression was significantly lower when heterocolonial ants were treated with CC, whilst TT caused a reduction in the antagonist behaviour, which was not significantly different from the control tests (Fig 4; S4 Table). Exposure to Ct-Ct and Tc-Tc significantly increased the aggression of M. scabrinodis, when compared to C and T treatments (Fig 4; S4 Table). Changes in the worker aggression were negatively correlated with dopamine (Pearson correlation ρ = -0.93, P < 0.001) and tyramine (Pearson correlation ρ = -0.90, P < 0.001).
Discussion
The results of this work show that C and T modulate the locomotor activity of some ant species. Treatments with either C, T, or mixed solutions differently affected ant movements. F. cinerea and T. caespitum workers generally increased their propensity to move in response to Tc, whereas the increase in M. scabrinodis movements only occurred when either C or T were administered alone.
In general, an increased locomotor activity is used as a proxy for ant escaping behaviour, whereas its reduction indicates an enhanced partner fidelity [31]. The dissimilar response of ant species to O. vulgare terpenoids might be linked to their different success in detoxifying these plant volatiles. Both T and C can be detrimental for insect [34] and C has been uses as insecticide [34–36], fungicidal [34,37] and acaricide [35], although the mechanism of its action is still unclear [37–39]. Myrmica ants are able to adapt and survive to environmental doses of C by upregulating genes (e.g., acetylcholinesterase, glutathione S transferase and CYP4509E2) whose products bind and detoxify this monoterpene, by making the colonies more resistant than other common ant species [28]. On the contrary, F. cinerea and T. caespitum did not show any significant upregulation of detoxifying genes when exposed to C and died soon after the treatment [28]. Hence, the increased locomotor activity here reported for F. cinerea and T. caespitum upon treatment with C and T can be considered as a countermeasure to enhance their chances to survive.
Interestingly, a reduction in the locomotor activity is observed only in M. scabrinodis as a response to terpenoid blends (i.e., Ct, Tc). In natural environments, T is always released by O. vulgare [40] whilst C primarily occurs as a response to the ant-plant interaction [28]. Therefore, blends of the two terpenoids are mainly emitted when oregano plants respond to the presence of ants. The release of C and T mixtures is more than a generic deterrent signal [41] causing the dispel of some ant species and act as chemical cue manipulating the behavior of Myrmica ant. C and T blends not only decrease the M. scabrinodis locomotor ability (Fig 1), but also modulate and enhance another crucial behavioural trait, such as the worker aggressiveness (Fig 4).
The decrease in the ant propensity to move is consistent with field data demonstrating that Myrmica workers forage closed to O. vulgare plants more frequently than other ant species [28]. The co-occurrence of Myrmica and oregano plants has been previously explained [28] as a benefit for Myrmica ants to colonise an enemy-free space [42]. However the exploitation of the ground underneath the plants rises the risk of Myrmica colonies to be parasitized by Maculinea larvae, overall reducing the evolutionary advantage of colonising this niche [28]. Our results provide hints on how these complex dynamics might persist, suggesting that Myrmica ants are indeed manipulated (i.e., no net evolutionary advantage is expected) and workers are induced by the plant volatiles to stay and patrol the oregano surroundings [13,43].
On the plant side, the ability to attract a species which is not phytophagous [44] and to increase its chemical aggressiveness at the cost of a slight variation in the volatile emission could represent an advantageous strategy to get further protection against herbivores. There are several associations where ant pugnacity is exploited by plants as a defence against enemies (e.g. herbivores, encroaching vegetation) [45,46]. Recently, it has been demonstrated that the plant-ant interaction Acacia-Pseudomyrmex is not an entirely mutualistic association, but definitely an example of “partner manipulation” [47]. Through the manipulation of the worker digestive activity the Acacia makes the ants strictly dependent, thus enhancing partner fidelity and protection.
A similar scenario has been described by Hojo and co-workers [31] studying a lycaenid larva and its “supposed” mutualistic associated ants. The butterfly larva secretes manipulative drugs which lower the attendance ant locomotor activity and enforce ant aggressive behaviour through dopaminergic regulation.
The role of dopamine in modulating insect movements and aggression has been shown in Drosophila melanogaster [17,48] and some monoamines are also known to be involved in the response to biotic and abiotic stress, including the variation of environment temperatures [49], population density [50] and food and water availability [51]. Biogenic amines, like dopamine, may function as neurotransmitters, neuromodulators, or neurohormones; however, studies showing a direct causal role of these molecules in mediating the behavioural plasticity are rare [19], mainly performed on invertebrates [20], and especially in social insects [52].
A wide variation of biogenic amine effects has been reported [20], showing how complex is the neurochemical mechanisms underlying social behaviours in ants and making any generalisation difficult. In contrast, the correlation between the biogenic amine levels and the ant locomotor behaviour we found is consistent throughout the three ant species. Our results show that dopamine levels, which are known to be directly correlated to the complexity of behavioral repertoire [53,54], and the tyramine content rise as the locomotor ability increases. This correlation suggests a widespread (across the ant species) and crucial role of dopamine and tyramine in modulating ant moving propensity, whereas serotonin appears not to be involved. Furthermore, our findings concur with the general knowledge [20,55] that, unlike other Hymenoptera, the ant aggression is not modulated by the serotonergic system.
Although further fieldwork experiments are needed and the mechanism through which plant volatiles can modulate biogenic amines content in ant brains is not yet clear, our study provides evidence of an interspecific behavioural manipulation through neurogenic dopamine and tyramine regulation.
Conclusions
Ants represent a good model system for neuroethological studies because of their relatively simple neural architecture. Our work provides new insights on the role of plant terpenoids in dopamine and tyramine modulation for the regulation of the locomotor activity and aggressiveness of ant workers, supporting a direct function of biogenic amines in the control of behaviour and colony organization. In the Maculinea-Myrmica-Origanum system the two plant monoterpenes, C and T, play crucial roles by modulating ant behaviour, through aminergic regulation, and fostering bi-level interspecific associations. Understanding how the flow of information among distinct biological levels works will be a fundamental challenge to better understand the interplay between different trophic levels [56,57].
It is increasingly clear that several interactions which have been considered to be mutualistic in the past, are nowadays revised as potentially parasitic because the ant partner is not receiving an actual reward but it is rather manipulated [31, 47]. In the future, the identification of specific neural networks on which biogenic amines act and the identification of genes involved in the development of these neurons, along with ethological studies, will be pivotal to understand how behavioural manipulation is widespread and actually achieved in plant-ant interactions.
Supporting information
S1 Table. Linear regression, coefficient of determination (R2), limit of detection (LOD) and of quantification (LOQ) of biogenic amines.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209047.s001
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Tukey’s HSD post hoc differences in ant locomotor activity.
*P<0.05; **P<0.01; ***P<0.001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209047.s002
(DOCX)
S3 Table. Tukey’s HSD post hoc differences in ant brain dopamine and tyramine contents.
*P<0.05; **P<0.01; ***P<0.001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209047.s003
(DOCX)
S4 Table. Tukey’s HSD post hoc differences in aggression index between heterocolonial Myrmica workers.
*P<0.05; **P<0.01.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209047.s004
(DOCX)
S1 Fig. Serotonin content.
Effects of treatments (C: carvacrol; T: thymol; Ct: 3:1 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol; Tc: 1:3 (v/v) carvacrol/thymol) and controls (CTRL and DMSO) on serotonin contents in ant brains of Formica cinerea (A), Tetramorium caespitum (B) and Myrmica scabrinodis (C). Values are expressed as pmol serotonin per ant brain. Boxplots show median, quartile, maximum and minimum values. No significant difference in the level of serotonin was found among the treatments.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209047.s005
(DOCX)
References
- 1.
Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. The Ants. Berlin: Harvard University Press; 1990
- 2. Beckers R, Deneubourg J-L, Goss S, Pasteels JM. Collective decision making through food recruitment. Insectes Soc. 1990;37: 258–267.
- 3. Franks NR, Fletcher CR. Spatial patterns in army ant foraging and migration: Eciton burchelli on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1983;12: 261–270.
- 4. Goss S, Aron S, Deneubourg J-L, Pasteels JM. Self-organized shortcuts in the Argentine ant. Naturwissenschaften. 1989;76: 579–581.
- 5. Franks NR, Wilby A, Silverman BW, Tofts C. Self-organizing nest construction in ants: sophisticated building by blind bulldozing. Anim Behav. 1992;44: 357–375.
- 6.
Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E, Deneubourg J-L. The mechanisms and rules of coordinated building in social insects. In: Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM editors. Information processing in social insects. Basel: Birkhäuser; 1999. pp. 309–330.
- 7. Hölldobler B. Foraging and spatiotemporal territories in the honey ant Myrmecocystus mimicus Wheeler (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1981;9: 301–314.
- 8. Adams ES. Boundary disputes in the territorial ant Azteca trigona: effects of asymmetries in colony size. Anim Behav. 1990;39: 321–328.
- 9. Robinson GE. Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annu Rev Entomol. 1992;37: 637–665. pmid:1539941
- 10. Gordon DM. The organization of work in social insect colonies. Nature. 1996;380:121–124.
- 11. Hölldobler B. Territorial behavior in the green tree ant (Oecophylla smaragdina). Biotropica. 1983; 241–250.
- 12.
Hölldobler B. Chemical manipulation, enemy specification and intercolony communication in ant communities. In: Huber F, Markl H editors. Neuroethology and behavioral physiology. Berlin: Springer; 1983. pp. 354–365.
- 13. Heil M, McKey D. Protective ant-plant interactions as model systems in ecological and evolutionary research. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2003;34: 425–553.
- 14. Wunderlich K, Smittenaar P, Dolan RJ. Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior. Neuron. 2012;75: 418–424. pmid:22884326
- 15. Yu Q, Teixeira CM, Mahadevia D, Huang Y, Balsam D, Mann JJ, et al. Dopamine and serotonin signaling during two sensitive developmental periods differentially impact adult aggressive and affective behaviors in mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19: 688. pmid:24589889
- 16. Huber R, Orzeszyna M, Pokorny N, Kravitz EA. Biogenic amines and aggression: experimental approaches in crustaceans. Brain Behav Evol. 1997;50: 60–68. pmid:9217993
- 17. Riemensperger T, Isabel G, Coulom H, Neuser K, Seugne L, Kume K, et al. Behavioural consequences of dopamine deficiency in the Drosophila brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108: 834–839. pmid:21187381
- 18. McQuillan HJ, Barron AB, Mercer AR. Age-and behaviour-related changes in the expression of biogenic amine receptor genes in the antennae of honey bees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Physiol A. 2012;198: 753–761.
- 19. Libersat F, Pflueger H-J. Monoamines and the orchestration of behavior. AIBS Bull. 2004;54: 17–25.
- 20. Kamhi JF, Traniello JFA. Biogenic amines and collective organization in a superorganism: neuromodulation of social behavior in ants. Brain Behav Evol. 2013;82: 220–236. pmid:24281765
- 21. Smith AR, Muscedere ML, Seid MA, Traniello JFA, Hughes WOH. Biogenic amines are associated with worker task but not patriline in the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex echinatior. J Comp Physiol A. 2013;199: 1117–1127.
- 22. Szczuka A, Korczyńska J, Wnuk A, Symonowicz B, Szwacka AG, Mazurkiewicz P, et al. The effects of serotonin, dopamine, octopamine and tyramine on behavior of workers of the ant Formica polyctena during dyadic aggression tests. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2013;73: 495–520.
- 23. Vander Meer RK, Preston CA, Hefetz A. Queen regulates biogenic amine level and nestmate recognition in workers of the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Naturwissenschaften. 2008;95: 1155–1158. pmid:18704354
- 24. Muscedere ML, Johnson N, Gillis BC, Kamhi JF, Traniello JFA. Serotonin modulates worker responsiveness to trail pheromone in the ant Pheidole dentata. J Comp Physiol A. 2012;198: 219–227.
- 25. Nash DR, Als TD, Maile R, Jones GR, Boomsma JJ. A mosaic of chemical coevolution in a large blue butterfly. Science. 2008;319: 88–90. pmid:18174441
- 26. Barbero F, Thomas JA, Bonelli S, Balletto E, Schönrogge K. Queen ants make distinctive sounds that are mimicked by a butterfly social parasite. Science. 2009;323: 782–785. pmid:19197065
- 27. Thomas JA. The behaviour and habitat requirements of Maculinea nausithous (the dusky large blue butterfly) and (the scarce large blue) in France. Biol Conserv. 1984;28: 325–347.
- 28. Patricelli D, Barbero F, Occhipinti A, Bertea CM, Bonelli S, Casacci LP, et al. Plant defences against ants provide a pathway to social parasitism in butterflies. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282: 20151111. pmid:26156773
- 29. Patricelli D, Barbero F, La Morgia V, Casacci LP, Witek M, Balletto E, et al. To lay or not to lay: oviposition of Maculinea arion in relation to Myrmica ant presence and host plant phenology. Anim Behav. 2011;82: 791–799.
- 30. Bhatkar A, Whitcomb W. Artificial diet for rearing various species of ants. The Florid. Entomol. 1970;53: 229–232.
- 31. Hojo MK, Pierce NE, Tsuji K. Lycaenid caterpillar secretions manipulate attendant ant behavior. Curr Biol. 2015;25: 2260–2264. pmid:26234210
- 32. Csata E, Timuş N, Witek M, Casacci L Pietro, Lucas C, Bagnères A-G, et al. Lock-picks: fungal infection facilitates the intrusion of strangers into ant colonies. Sci Rep. 2017;7: 46323. pmid:28402336
- 33.
Ich. ICH Topic Q2 (R1) Validation of Analytical Procedures: Text and Methodology. Int Conf Harmon. 2005;1994: 17. doi:https://doi.org/http://www.ich.org/fileadmin/Public_Web_Site/ICH_Products/Guidelines/Quality/Q2_R1/Step4/Q2_R1__Guideline.pdf
- 34. Kordali S, Cakir A, Ozer H, Cakmakci R, Kesdek M, Mete E. Antifungal, phytotoxic and insecticidal properties of essential oil isolated from Turkish Origanum acutidens and its three components, carvacrol, thymol and p-cymene. Bioresour Technol. 2008;99: 8788–8795. pmid:18513954
- 35. Ahn Y-J, Lee S-B, Lee H-S, Kim G-H. Insecticidal and acaricidal activity of carvacrol and β-thujaplicine derived from Thujopsis dolabrata var. hondai sawdust. J Chem Ecol. 1998;24: 81–90.
- 36. Regnault-Roger C, Hamraoui A. Fumigant toxic activity and reproductive inhibition induced by monoterpenes on Acanthoscelides obtectus (Say)(Coleoptera), a bruchid of kidney bean (Phaseolus vulgaris L.). J Stored Prod Res. 1995;31: 291–299.
- 37. Rao A, Zhang Y, Muend S, Rao R. Mechanism of antifungal activity of terpenoid phenols resembles calcium stress and inhibition of the TOR pathway. Antimicrob Agents Chemother. 2010;54: 5062–5069. pmid:20921304
- 38. Monzote L, Stamberg W, Staniek K, Gille L. Toxic effects of carvacrol, caryophyllene oxide, and ascaridole from essential oil of Chenopodium ambrosioides on mitochondria. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2009;240: 337–347. pmid:19666043
- 39. Xu J, Zhou F, Ji B, Pei R, Xu N. The antibacterial mechanism of carvacrol and thymol against Escherichia coli. Lett Appl Microbiol. 2008;47: 174–179. pmid:19552781
- 40. Crocoll C, Asbach J, Novak J, Gershenzon J, Degenhardt J. Terpene synthases of oregano (Origanum vulgare L.) and their roles in the pathway and regulation of terpene biosynthesis. Plant Mol Biol. 2010;73: 587–603. pmid:20419468
- 41. Agliassa C, Maffei M.E. Origanum vulgare terpenoids induce oxidative stress and reduce the feeding activity of Spodoptera littoralis. Int J Mol Sci. 2018;19: 2805.
- 42. Jeffries MJ, Lawton JH. Enemy free space and the structure of ecological communities. Biol J Linn Soc. 1984;23: 269–286.
- 43. Grasso DA, Pandolfi C, Bazihizina N, Nocentini D, Nepi M, Mancuso S. Extrafloral-nectar-based partner manipulation in plant-ant relationships. AoB PLANTS. 2015;7-plv002.
- 44.
Radchenko A, Elmes GW. Myrmica ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) of the old world. Warsaw: Natura Optima Dux Foundation; 2010.
- 45. Dejean A, Champlain D, Orivel J. The plant ant Tetraponera aethiops (Pseudomyrmecinae) protects its host myrmecophyte Barteria fistulosa (Passifloraceae) through aggressiveness and predation. Biol J Linn Soc. 2007;93: 63–69.
- 46.
Koptur S. Extrafloral nectary-mediated interactions between insects and plants. In: Bernays E, editor. Insect-Plant Interactions. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2017. pp. 89–138.
- 47. Heil M, Barajas‐Barron A, Orona‐Tamayo D, Wielsch N, Svatos A. Partner manipulation stabilises a horizontally transmitted mutualism. Ecology letters. 2014;17: 185–192. pmid:24188323
- 48. Alekseyenko O V, Chan Y-B, Li R, Kravitz EA. Single dopaminergic neurons that modulate aggression in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci. National Acad Sciences; 2013; 201303446.
- 49. Traniello JFA, Fujita MS, Bowen R V. Ant foraging behavior: ambient temperature influences prey selection. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1984;15: 65–68.
- 50. Iba M, Nagao T, Urano A. Effects of population density on growth, behavior and levels of biogenic amines in the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. Zoolog Sci. 1995;12: 695–702.
- 51. Wada-Katsumata A, Yamaoka R, Aonuma H. Social interactions influence dopamine and octopamine homeostasis in the brain of the ant Formica japonica. J Exp Biol. 2011;214: 1707–1713. pmid:21525317
- 52. Kamhi JF, Arganda S, Moreau CS, Traniello JF. Origins of aminergic regulation of behavior in complex insect social systems. Front Syst Neurosci. 2017;11:74. pmid:29066958
- 53. Penick CA, Brent CS, Dolezal K, Liebig J. Neurohormonal changes associated with ritualized combat and the formation of a reproductive hierarchy in the ant Harpegnathos saltatory. J Exp Biol. 2014; jeb-098301.
- 54. Hoyer SC, Liebig J, Rössler W. BAs in the ponerine ant Harpegnathos saltator: serotonin and dopamine immunoreactivity in the brain. Arth Struct Develop. 2005;34: 429–440.
- 55. Kamhi JF., Nunn K, Robson SKA, Traniello JFA. Polymorphism and division of labour in a socially complex ant: neuromodulation of aggression in the Australian weaver ant, Oecophylla smaragdina. 2015. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282: 20150704.
- 56. Dicke M. Chemical ecology of host-plant selection by herbivorous arthropods: A multitrophic perspective. Biochem Syst Ecol. 2000;28, 601–617. pmid:10854737
- 57. Barbero F. Cuticular lipids as a cross-talk among ants, plants and butterflies. International journal of molecular sciences. Int J Mol Sci. 2016; 1966.