Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effect of behavioral inhibition system and childhood emotional neglect on serotonergic activity, negative affect, and rejection sensitivity in non-clinical adults

  • Min Jin Jin,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations Clinical Emotion and Cognition Research Laboratory, Inje University, Goyang, Republic of Korea, Department of Psychology, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

  • Wookyoung Jung,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Keimyung University, Daegu, Republic of Korea

  • Myoung Ho Hyun,

    Roles Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

  • Seung-Hwan Lee

    Roles Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    lshpss@paik.ac.kr, lshpss@hanmail.net

    Affiliations Clinical Emotion and Cognition Research Laboratory, Inje University, Goyang, Republic of Korea, Department of Psychiatry, Inje University, Ilsan-Paik Hospital, Goyang, Republic of Korea

Abstract

Introduction

Behavioral inhibition system (BIS) has a strong genetic basis, and emotional neglect (EN) in childhood is one of many environmental experiences that can affect individuals. This study aimed to examine the effects and interaction between BIS and EN on central serotonergic activity and other negative affect and cognition.

Methods

A total of 153 non-clinical volunteers (54 men and 99 women; average age, 27.72 years, standard deviation = 6.40) were included in the analyses. The Behavioral Inhibition System scale, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, and negative affect and cognition (Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire) were measured. As a biomarker of central serotonergic activity, the loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials was measured.

Results

High EN was associated with higher loudness dependence of auditory evoked potential (LDAEP) levels and low EN was associated with lower LDAEP levels in high BIS people only. People with high EN people showed significantly higher levels of depression and state anxiety than did those with low EN. Moreover, of people with low BIS, those who had more EN experience had higher levels of rejection sensitivity than did those with less EN experience, while people with high BIS did not show different patterns of rejection sensitivity regardless of the difference of EN.

Conclusions

This study revealed different effects on physiological (loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials), intrapersonal (depression and state anxiety), and interpersonal aspects (rejection sensitivity) based on the interaction of BIS and EN. Our results suggest that the physiological and interpersonal aspects, but not the intrapersonal aspect, are significantly influenced by the interactive effect of BIS and EN.

Introduction

Over the past decades, numerous studies have found that the interactive effect of genetic and environmental factors contributes to individual differences [1] in serotonin system markers [2], depression [2], anxiety [3], and sensitivity to rejection [4]. Although the DNA-sequence has been considered as an up-to-date method in recent studies of gene-environment interactions, there could be more practical ways to identify such interactions using non-molecular approaches [5]. This study is aimed to examine possible gene-environment interactions using sources that are more economical and easily accessible by many social scientists, including psychologists.

Gray proposed behavioral inhibition and activation systems [6] as innate dispositions which mediate genetic influence on individual differences, such as temperament, behavior, and emotions [7]. The behavioral activation system (BAS) and behavioral inhibition system (BIS), defined by Gray [8], are particularly essential functions of communication and human emotion [6]. There is substantial evidence of neurobiological mechanisms for each of these systems [9]. Notably, BIS inhibits behavior in response to signals of punishment, non-reward, and novelty [9, 10]. BIS is known to be heritable [11, 12] and to have basis of neurobiological and biological mechanisms [13, 14]. Therefore, BIS could be utilized as an expedient psychological assessment that strongly reflects genetic differences among individuals.

A factor that could be conceived as an environmental component that influence individuals is the experience of emotional neglect (EN) in childhood. EN is defined as emotional deprivation or the absence of a nurturing emotional environment in childhood [15]. EN is a particularly important environmental factor compared with other childhood adversities since it causes various severe emotional problems [16], brain dysfunction [17], and psychological and physical distress even after adjustment for childhood physical and sexual abuse [15]. Therefore, many studies have examined EN independently [18, 19]. Individuals with a history of EN during childhood have an attentional bias toward threatening stimuli [20] and experience emotional and physical distress, in both youth [21] and adulthood [15]. Although assessing EN of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) relies on retrospective and subjective reports, the CTQ is well-validated and known to be stable overtime [22], and various studies used EN of CTQ as a measurement of environmental stressor [2325]. Thus, EN could be employed as an element that is influenced by environmental experiences.

Both BIS and EN are related to serotonin activity. Behavioral inhibition is regulated by central serotonin activity [26]. BIS, which is sensitive to cues of threat, functions through serotonergic activity in the septohippocampal system [27]. The main role of serotonin in motivation is the inhibition of behavior [28]. A childhood history of EN can also influence the development of various neurobiological systems and can cause reduced serotonin activity in adulthood [2931]. EN in childhood was reported to be associated with lower cerebrospinal fluid levels of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in adulthood [32].

The loudness dependence of auditory evoked potential (LDAEP), also known as the intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials (IAEP), is an electroencephalography (EEG) measure and known to be inversely related to central serotonergic neurotransmission [3336]. As a biomarker of serotonin activity, LDAEP may be associated with not only BIS but also EN.

In association with serotonin activity, BIS and EN could also be related to factors of intrapersonal distress such as depression and anxiety. Individuals with high BIS reactivity are expected to have higher levels of anxiety [27, 37, 38] and depression [27, 39, 40]. EN is also a predictor of emotional distress, including anxiety and depression in youth [21] and adults [15].

Both BIS and EN may be related to interpersonal factors such as rejection sensitivity (RS). RS is a cognitive-affective processing disposition in which the individual anxiously expects rejection [41]. From a social perspective, rejection includes aspects of punishment and increases the avoidance motivation that is salient for high BIS individuals [42]. In addition to BIS, a history of abuse may markedly alter an individual’s sensitivity to social rejection [43, 44].

While both BIS and EN could be related with various characteristics, it remains unclear how they are related or whether there is an effect of their interaction. Therefore, this study was designed to discover the main and the interactive effects of BIS and EN–constructs that can be assessed conveniently yet proposed to reflect genetic and environment influences, respectively–on individual differences. Measures of individual differences included LDAEP (physiological aspect), anxiety and depression (intrapersonal aspect), and RS (interpersonal aspect).

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 157 healthy and non-smoking Korean volunteers recruited through advertisements in local newspapers were included in this study. Four subjects were eliminated because of missing data; thus, the final sample included 153 subjects. The population consisted of 54 (35.3%) men and 99 (64.7%) women with a mean age of 27.72 years (standard deviation [SD] = 6.40). The subjects had a mean of 14.44 years of education (SD = 1.78). Each participant provided a written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Inje University Ilsan Paik Hospital before initiation (IRB no. 2015-07-026-001).

Psychological measures

The Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) scale from the Behavioral Inhibition System and Behavioral Activation System Scales, designed in accordance with Gray’s theory [45] and well-validated in Korea [46], was used. The BIS scale is a self-reported questionnaire consist of 7 items and is assessed using 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“I strongly agree”) to 4 (“I strongly disagree”).

The EN scale was used from the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) which was validated in Korea [47]. The CTQ is a self-reported questionnaire that is reported to be well-validated and stable over the time [22]. The CTQ is used to assess childhood trauma, including emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, EN, and physical neglect [48]. The EN subscale consists of 8 items and is assessed using a 5-point Likert scale that ranges from 1 (“never true”) to 5 (“very often true”). The score range of 5–9 is considered as none or minimal, 10–14 as low, 15–17 as moderate, and 18–25 as severe emotional neglect [4951].

For the intrapersonal aspects, depression and state anxiety were measured. Depression was evaluated using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), developed by Beck [52] and validated in Korea [53]. It is composed of 21-items and assessed using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (‘‘almost never”) to 3 (‘‘almost always”). State anxiety was measured using the Korean-validated version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory [54], which was used to evaluate both state and trait anxiety [55]. It is composed of 40 items and assessed using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (‘‘almost never”) to 4 (‘‘almost always”). The subscale of state anxiety with only 20 items was used in this study.

RS was measured as an interpersonal aspect. Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire (RSQ) was used to measure RS [56]. The RSQ consists of 18 hypothetical interpersonal situations with possible rejection by a significant other. Participants rated (1) the level of anxiety and concern about the outcome and (2) the perceived likelihood of acceptance or rejection by their significant other in each situation with 6-point Likert scale. The scores were calculated by first weighting the expected likelihood of rejection for each situation by the degree of anxiety, and then averaging these weighted scores across the 18 situations.

Loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials

Electroencephalogram data for all participants were acquired in a sound-attenuated EEG room using a NeuroScan SynAmps amplifier (Compumedics USA, Charlotte, NC, USA) with 64 Ag–AgCl electrodes mounted on a Neuroscan Quik-Cap (Compumedics, Charlotte, NC) using an extended 10–20 placement scheme. The ground electrode was located on the forehead, and the physically linked reference electrode was attached to both mastoids. The vertical electrooculogram (EOG) was positioned above and below the left eye, and the horizontal EOG was obtained at the outer canthus of each eye. The impedance was maintained below 5 kΩ. All data were processed with a 0.1–100 Hz band pass filter and sampled at 1000 Hz.

The recorded electroencephalogram data were preprocessed using CURRY 7 (Compumedics USA, Charlotte, NC, USA). Gross artifacts, such as those caused by movements, were rejected through visual inspection by a trained person who was blinded to the origin of the data. Artifacts related to eye movement or blinks were eliminated using the mathematical procedure implemented in the preprocessing software [57]. The data were filtered using a 0.1–30Hz bandpass filter and epoched from 100ms pre-stimulus to 900ms post-stimulus. The epochs were subtracted from the average value of the pre-stimulus interval for baseline correction. If any remaining epochs contained significant physiological artifacts (i.e., amplitudes exceeding ±75 μV) in any of the 62 electrode sites, they were excluded from further analysis.

Auditory stimulation included 1000 stimuli with an inter-stimulus interval randomized between 500 and 900 ms. Tones of 1000 Hz that were 80 ms in duration (10 ms rise and 10 ms fall) were presented through MDR-D777 headphones (Sony, Tokyo, Japan) at five intensities: 60, 70, 80, 90, and 100 dB SPL. These stimuli were generated by E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). For each subject, the N1 peak (the most negative peak between 50 and 200 ms from the stimulus) and P2 peak (the most positive peak between 150 and 300 ms from the stimulus) were subsequently determined at the Cz electrode [58, 59] for the five intensities. Cz was chosen as it is the best single electrode position for assessing activity of the auditory cortices [60] and the reliabilities at Cz can reach the same level as previously reported by dipole-source-localization methods [61]. The peak-to-peak N1/P2 amplitudes were calculated for each of the 5 stimulus intensities, and the LDAEP was calculated as the slope of the linear regression.

Statistical analysis

Normality was tested for each variable before statistical analysis. Skewness > 2.0 and kurtosis > 7.0 were considered to be substantial departure from normality [62]. All variables in our results were within the range of normal distribution. After analyzing averages and SDs of variables, EN and BIS scores were dummy coded into two groups (low and high). Regression analyses were conducted with sex as a covariate to examine the main and interaction effects of the low/high EN and low/high BIS. Regression analyses were performed instead of analysis of covariance in order to control for the possible effect of sex, which was found to be different between the groups [63]. The significant level was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed). Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 21 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and regression analyses to examine interaction effects were performed using Process designed for SPSS [64].

Results

Descriptive statistics

The subjects were divided into four groups: low EN and low BIS score, low EN and high BIS score, high EN and low BIS score, and high EN and high BIS score groups. The high and low EN scores were divided based on a score of 10, which is the cut-off score between none and low severity of EN [4851]. The high and low BIS scores were divided based on a score of 21, which was the median score of the study population. Table 1 presents the comparisons of demographic and psychological characteristics between the four groups.

thumbnail
Table 1. Comparison of demographic, psychological, and behavioral characteristics in participants with low EN and low BIS scores, low EN and high BIS scores, high EN and low BIS scores, and high EN and high BIS scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207746.t001

Regression analysis

In order to examine the interactive effect of the low/high EN and low/high BIS, regression analyses were performed with sex as a covariate. For the LDAEP slope level, the coefficients of EN (B = -0.51, p = .098) and BIS (B = -0.26, p = .109) were not significant. However, the coefficient of the interaction of EN and BIS (B = 0.48, p = .022) was significant (Table 2). The R2 was increased due to the interaction (Δ R2 = 0.03, Δ F = 5.37, p = .022). For subjects with low BIS score, the LDAEP level was not significantly different whether EN was low or high [t(87) = .10, p = .921]. However, for the subject with high BIS score, LDAEP level was significantly higher in those with high EN than in those with low EN [t(62) = −2.43, p = .018] (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Interactive effect of the behavioral inhibition system and emotional neglect on the loudness dependence of auditory-evoked potentials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207746.g001

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of regression analyses of EN, BIS, and the interaction of EN and BIS on LDAEP and RS.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207746.t002

For depression, all coefficients, including EN (B = 3.46, p = .218), BIS (B = 1.11, p = .441), and the interaction between EN and BIS (B = -0.22, p = .906) were not significant. Separate simple linear regressions were conducted to examine the effect of EN and BIS independently. There was significant effect of EN (B = 3.19, p = .001, R2corr = .081), while the effect of BIS alone was insignificant (B = 1.12, p = .242, R2corr = .016). In addition, for state anxiety, the coefficient of EN (B = 9.73, p = .013) was significant, yet coefficients of BIS (B = 2.38, p = .233) and the interaction of EN and BIS (B = -3.51, p = .175) were not significant. Results of simple linear regressions also revealed that the effect of EN alone was significant (B = 4.79, p < .001, R2corr = .083), while the effect of BIS alone was insignificant (B = 0.50, p = .704, R2corr = -.003).

For RS, all coefficients of EN (B = 4.10, p = .002), BIS (B = 1.85, p = .008), and the interaction of EN and BIS (B = -2.14, p = .018) were significant (Table 2). The R2 was increased due to the interaction (Δ R2 = 0.04, Δ F = 5.74, p = .018). The RS score was significantly higher in subjects with low BIS score and high EN than in those with low BIS score and low EN [t(62) = −3.69, p < .001]. However, there was no significant difference of the RS score depending on the level of EN in subjects with high BIS score [t(87) = .24, p = .808] (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Interactive effect of the behavioral inhibition system and emotional neglect on results of the rejection sensitivity questionnaire.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207746.g002

Discussion

This study aimed to demonstrate the effects of BIS and EN on physiological, interpersonal, and intrapersonal factors: the LDAEP, RS, and depression and state anxiety, respectively. While BIS was considered to reflect genetic influence substantially, EN was considered as one of the environmental experiences that can affect individuals. The results revealed that the interaction between BIS and EN influenced both LDAEP and RS. However, there was no significant interaction found for depression and state anxiety.

This study determined whether the LDAEP level was influenced by the interaction of BIS and EN. Because LDAEP is known to reflect central serotonergic neurotransmission inversely [34, 36], it is plausible that LDAEP was influenced by both BIS and EN. Central serotonin function is a key element of behavioral inhibition [26, 65], and the development of a functioning serotonergic system is negatively affected by exposure to early EN [17, 66]. Previous studies that examined the relationship between BIS/BAS and LDAEP discovered that LDAEP was positively correlated with BIS [67] and negatively correlated with BAS [68]. The present study tried to examine this relationship with regard to the history of EN, which reflects bad environment. The LDAEP level was low when the level of EN was low and it was high when the level of EN was high as well in people with high BIS (high susceptibility), whereas it was not different depending on the level of EN in people with low BIS (low susceptibility).

This diametric effect is also discovered with serotonin-transporter genes. A study of the serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) revealed that although individuals with two risk alleles proved most adversely affected, they showed the most positive outcomes when stressful life events were absent [69]. This diametric effect including the current result could be explained as the differential-susceptibility hypothesis, which is that the most vulnerable individuals who are affected by many kinds of stressors could be the very same ones those who reap the most benefit from environmental support, including the absence of adversity [70]. BIS, which is considered to reflect more innate temperamental features, activates responses of inhibition and avoidance [71] and controls sensitivity and responsiveness to threats via serotonergic activity [40]. Because people with a low BIS score (low susceptibility) show decreased behavioral inhibition and sensitivity to threats related to low reactivity of serotonin level regardless of EN, they could be less reactive to emotional adversities than people with high BIS scores and show comparable levels of LDAEP regardless of a childhood EN experience. In contrast, the subjects with high BIS scores (high susceptibility) showed increased behavioral inhibition and sensitivity to threats, related to high reactivity of serotonin, so that they could react with higher sensitivity to EN and even show less level of LDAEP when EN was not present.

Neither depression nor state anxiety was influenced by the interaction of EN and BIS. Simple linear regression analyses revealed that the levels of depression and state anxiety were influenced by EN alone. Subjects who experienced EN showed significantly higher levels of depression and state anxiety than did those who did not. Although the serotonin level is related to these emotions [72], emotional distress could be more affected by an individual’s personal history of emotional experiences [15]. Particularly, emotional adversities in childhood could result in insecure attachment, which can later influence depression and anxiety in adulthood [73, 74].

RS was observed to be predicted by EN, BIS, and the interactive effect of EN and the BIS. In the subjects with low BIS scores (low susceptibility) who had more EN experiences (bad environment) had higher levels of RS than did those with less EN experience. However, in the subjects with high BIS scores (high susceptibility), the experience of EN did not result in significant differences in RS. These results indicate that people with high BIS scores are inherently highly sensitive to rejection; therefore, the experience of EN does not result in any significant difference. In contrast, people with low BIS scores are highly sensitive to rejection, but only when they experience EN. This is dissimilar to intrapersonal emotional problems because RS is an index of interpersonal, rather than intrapersonal, issues. Indeed, distinct differences between the interpersonal and intrapersonal effects of emotion and motivation have been demonstrated [75, 76]. People with high BIS shows worry proneness [77], sensitivity to signals of punishments and threat [13, 40, 78], and avoidance behavior [10]. Because rejection is known to encompass the aspects of punishment and increase avoidance motivation [42], people with high BIS scores might react sensitively to rejection, irrespective of an EN experience. In contrast, people with low BIS scores generally are not sensitive to rejection unless they experience EN. Therefore, both BIS and EN play a critical role in RS.

This study has several limitations. First, although LDAEP is known to be a reliable indicator of serotonin, further studies comparing LDAEP with more direct measurement of central serotonin levels are necessary before drawing firm conclusions. Moreover, this study only used the old version of the BIS scale, since the newest version of the BIS/BAS questionnaire is not yet validated in Korea. In addition, the use of more genetic indices such as single-nucleotide polymorphisms and assessing objective environmental effect would be needed in further studies to ensure results of this study. Lastly, this study used RS as the only interpersonal aspect. There may be other interpersonal variables to examine the effects of the BIS and EN that should be considered in future studies.

In conclusion, this study is the first to investigate the interactive effect of the BIS and EN on LDAEP, anxiety and depression, and RS. LDAEP, which reflects the serotonin level, was influenced by the interaction of the BIS and EN such that high EN (bad environment) was associated with higher LDAEP, whereas low EN was associated with lower LDAEP in high BIS subjects (high susceptibility), but these relationships were not observed in low BIS subjects (low susceptibility). However, intrapersonal negative emotions, such as depression and anxiety, were more influenced in subjects with a history of EN. In contrast, RS was affected by the interaction of the BIS and EN, such that the experience of EN resulted in a higher level of RS, but only in subjects with low BIS scores. Our results reveal the different effects of BIS, EN, and their interaction on the various characteristics of individuals.

Supporting information

S1 Table. All data underlying the findings.

LDAEP, loudness dependence of auditory-evoked potentials; SAI, state anxiety inventory; BDI, Beck depression inventory; CTQ, childhood trauma questionnaire; BIS, behavioral inhibition system; RSQ, rejection sensitivity questionnaire; EN, emotional neglect.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207746.s001

(XLSX)

References

  1. 1. Plomin RE, DeFries JC, Craig IW, McGuffin PE. Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era: American Psychological Association; 2003.
  2. 2. Eley TC, Sugden K, Corsico A, Gregory AM, Sham P, McGuffin P, et al. Gene–environment interaction analysis of serotonin system markers with adolescent depression. Molecular psychiatry. 2004;9(10):908. pmid:15241435
  3. 3. Gunthert KC, Conner TS, Armeli S, Tennen H, Covault J, Kranzler HR. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety reactivity in daily life: A daily process approach to gene-environment interaction. Psychosomatic medicine. 2007;69(8):762–8. pmid:17942837
  4. 4. Troisi A, Frazzetto G, Carola V, Di Lorenzo G, Coviello M, Siracusano A, et al. Variation in the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) moderates the influence of early maternal care on fearful attachment. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2011;7(5):542–7. pmid:21742765
  5. 5. Hunter DJ. Gene–environment interactions in human diseases. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2005;6(4):287. pmid:15803198
  6. 6. McCroskey JC, Beatty MJ. The communibiological perspective: Implications for communication in instruction. Communication Education. 2000;49(1):1–6.
  7. 7. Heponiemi T, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Puttonen S, Ravaja N. BIS/BAS sensitivity and self-rated affects during experimentally induced stress. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;34(6):943–57.
  8. 8. Gray JA. The psychology of fear and stress: CUP Archive; 1987.
  9. 9. Gable SL, Reis HT, Elliot AJ. Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2000;78(6):1135. pmid:10870914
  10. 10. Gray JA. Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition & Emotion. 1990;4(3):269–88.
  11. 11. Robinson JL, Kagan J, Reznick JS, Corley R. The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: a twin study. Developmental Psychology. 1992;28(6):1030.
  12. 12. Takahashi Y, Yamagata S, Kijima N, Shigemasu K, Ono Y, Ando J. Gray’s temperament model: development of Japanese version of BIS/BAS scales and a behavior genetic investigation using the twin method. Japanese Journal of Personality. 2007;15:276–89.
  13. 13. Barros-Loscertales A, Meseguer V, Sanjuan A, Belloch V, Parcet M, Torrubia R, et al. Behavioral inhibition system activity is associated with increased amygdala and hippocampal gray matter volume: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroimage. 2006;33(3):1011–5. pmid:16979909
  14. 14. Heimberg RG. Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment: Guilford Press; 1995.
  15. 15. Spertus IL, Yehuda R, Wong CM, Halligan S, Seremetis SV. Childhood emotional abuse and neglect as predictors of psychological and physical symptoms in women presenting to a primary care practice. Child abuse & neglect. 2003;27(11):1247–58.
  16. 16. Heinz A, Jones DW, Mazzanti C, Goldman D, Ragan P, Hommer D, et al. A relationship between serotonin transporter genotype and in vivo protein expression and alcohol neurotoxicity. Biological psychiatry. 2000;47(7):643–9. pmid:10745057
  17. 17. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Clark DB, Casey B, Giedd JN, Boring AM, et al. Developmental traumatology part II: brain development. Biological psychiatry. 1999;45(10):1271–84. pmid:10349033
  18. 18. Bogdan R, Williamson DE, Hariri AR. Mineralocorticoid receptor Iso/Val (rs5522) genotype moderates the association between previous childhood emotional neglect and amygdala reactivity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169(5):515–22. pmid:22407082
  19. 19. Wark MJ, Kruczek T, Boley A. Emotional neglect and family structure: impact on student functioning. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2003;27(9):1033–43.
  20. 20. Jovanovic T, Blanding NQ, Norrholm SD, Duncan E, Bradley B, Ressler KJ. Childhood abuse is associated with increased startle reactivity in adulthood. Depression and anxiety. 2009;26(11):1018–26. pmid:19691032
  21. 21. Maheu FS, Dozier M, Guyer AE, Mandell D, Peloso E, Poeth K, et al. A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010;10(1):34–49.
  22. 22. Paivio SC. Stability of retrospective self-reports of child abuse and neglect before and after therapy for child abuse issues☆. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2001;25(8):1053–68.
  23. 23. Aguilera M, Arias B, Wichers M, Barrantes-Vidal N, Moya J, Villa H, et al. Early adversity and 5-HTT/BDNF genes: new evidence of gene–environment interactions on depressive symptoms in a general population. Psychological medicine. 2009;39(9):1425–32. pmid:19215635
  24. 24. White MG, Bogdan R, Fisher PM, Munoz K, Williamson DE, Hariri AR. FKBP5 and emotional neglect interact to predict individual differences in amygdala reactivity. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2012;11(7):869–78.
  25. 25. Zannas A, Binder E. Gene–environment interactions at the FKBP5 locus: Sensitive periods, mechanisms and pleiotropism. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2014;13(1):25–37.
  26. 26. Eagle DM, Lehmann O, Theobald DE, Pena Y, Zakaria R, Ghosh R, et al. Serotonin depletion impairs waiting but not stop-signal reaction time in rats: implications for theories of the role of 5-HT in behavioral inhibition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(5):1311–21. pmid:19005464
  27. 27. Gray JA, van Goozen S, Van de Poll N, Sergeant J. Framework for a taxonomy of psychiatric disorder. Emotions: Essays on emotion theory. 1994:29–59.
  28. 28. Crockett MJ, Clark L, Robbins TW. Reconciling the role of serotonin in behavioral inhibition and aversion: acute tryptophan depletion abolishes punishment-induced inhibition in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(38):11993–9. pmid:19776285
  29. 29. Kinnally E, Lyons L, Abel K, Mendoza S, Capitanio J. Effects of early experience and genotype on serotonin transporter regulation in infant rhesus macaques. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2008;7(4):481–6.
  30. 30. Miller JM, Kinnally EL, Ogden RT, Oquendo MA, Mann JJ, Parsey RV. Reported childhood abuse is associated with low serotonin transporter binding in vivo in major depressive disorder. Synapse. 2009;63(7):565–73. pmid:19288578
  31. 31. Veenema AH. Early life stress, the development of aggression and neuroendocrine and neurobiological correlates: what can we learn from animal models? Frontiers in neuroendocrinology. 2009;30(4):497–518. pmid:19341763
  32. 32. Roy A. Self-rated childhood emotional neglect and CSF monoamine indices in abstinent cocaine-abusing adults: possible implications for suicidal behavior. Psychiatry research. 2002;112(1):69–75. pmid:12379452
  33. 33. Hegerl U, Juckel G. Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotonergic neurotransmission: a new hypothesis. Biological psychiatry. 1993;33(3):173–87. pmid:8383545
  34. 34. Juckel G, Molnár M, Hegerl U, Csépe V, Karmos G. Auditory-evoked potentials as indicator of brain serotonergic activity first evidence in behaving cats. Biological psychiatry. 1997;41(12):1181–95. pmid:9171909
  35. 35. Lee IH, Yang YK, Chen PS, Huang HC, Yeh TL, Lu R-B, et al. Loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials (LDAEP) correlates with the availability of dopamine transporters and serotonin transporters in healthy volunteers—a two isotopes SPECT study. Psychopharmacology. 2011;214(3):617–24. pmid:21072504
  36. 36. Park Y-M, Lee S-H, Lee H-J, Kang S-G, Min J-A, Chae J-H. Association between BDNF gene polymorphisms and serotonergic activity using loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials in healthy subjects. PloS one. 2013;8(4):e60340. pmid:23593198
  37. 37. Beck I, Smits DJ, Claes L, Vandereycken W, Bijttebier P. Psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation system scales and the sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire in a sample of eating disordered patients. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47(5):407–12.
  38. 38. Corr PJ. Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28(3):317–32.
  39. 39. Kasch KL, Rottenberg J, Arnow BA, Gotlib IH. Behavioral activation and inhibition systems and the severity and course of depression. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2002;111(4):589. pmid:12428772
  40. 40. Meyer B, Johnson SL, Carver CS. Exploring behavioral activation and inhibition sensitivities among college students at risk for bipolar spectrum symptomatology. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1999;21(4):275–92. pmid:21765591
  41. 41. De Panfilis C, Meehan KB, Cain NM, Clarkin JF. Effortful control, rejection sensitivity, and borderline personality disorder features in adulthood. Journal of personality disorders. 2016;30(5):595–612. pmid:26623540
  42. 42. Rajchert J, Winiewski M. The behavioral approach and inhibition systems' role in shaping the displaced and direct aggressive reaction to ostracism and rejection. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016;88:272–9.
  43. 43. Rosen PJ, Milich R, Harris MJ. Why's everybody always picking on me? Social cognition, emotion regulation, and chronic peer victimization in children. Bullying rejection & peer victimization: Springer New York, NY; 2009. p. 79–100.
  44. 44. Woerner J, Kopetz C, Lechner WV, Lejuez C. History of abuse and risky sex among substance users: The role of rejection sensitivity and the need to belong. Addictive behaviors. 2016;62:73–8. pmid:27344009
  45. 45. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1994;67(2):319.
  46. 46. Kim KH, Kim WS. Korean BAS / BIS Scale. Korean Journal of Health Psychology. 2001;6(2):19–37.
  47. 47. Yu JH, Park JS, Park DH, Ryu SH, Ha JH. Validation of the Korean Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: The practical use in counseling and therapeutic intervention. THE KOREAN JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY. 2009;14(3):563–78.
  48. 48. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child abuse & neglect. 2003;27(2):169–90.
  49. 49. Häuser W, Schmutzer G, Brähler E, Glaesmer H. Maltreatment in childhood and adolescence: results from a survey of a representative sample of the German population. Deutsches Ärzteblatt international. 2011;108(17):287. pmid:21629512
  50. 50. Mizuki R, Fujiwara T. Association of oxytocin level and less severe forms of childhood maltreatment history among healthy Japanese adults involved with child care. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2015;9:138. pmid:26157369
  51. 51. Negele A, Kaufhold J, Kallenbach L, Leuzinger-Bohleber M. Childhood trauma and its relation to chronic depression in adulthood. Depression research and treatment. 2015;2015.
  52. 52. Beck AT. Measuring depression: The depression inventory. Recent advances in the psychobiology of the depressive illnesses. 1972:299–302.
  53. 53. Lee YH. The Relations between attributional style, life events, event attribution, hopelessness and depression. Seoul: Seoul National University; 1993.
  54. 54. Hahn D-W, Lee CH, Chon KK. Korean Adaptation of Spielberger's STAI (K-STAI). THE KOREAN JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY. 1996;1(1):1–14.
  55. 55. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE, Vagg PR. State-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). BiB. 2010;1970:180.
  56. 56. Downey G, Freitas AL, Michaelis B, Khouri H. The self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1998;75(2):545. pmid:9731324
  57. 57. Semlitsch HV, Anderer P, Schuster P, Presslich O. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology. 1986;23(6):695–703. pmid:3823345
  58. 58. Gudlowski Y, Özgürdal S, Witthaus H, Gallinat J, Hauser M, Winter C, et al. Serotonergic dysfunction in the prodromal, first-episode and chronic course of schizophrenia as assessed by the loudness dependence of auditory evoked activity. Schizophrenia research. 2009;109(1):141–7.
  59. 59. Park Y-M, Jung E, Kim HS, Hahn SW, Lee S-H. Differences in central serotoninergic transmission among patients with recent onset, sub-chronic, and chronic schizophrenia as assessed by the loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials. Schizophrenia research. 2015;168(1):180–4.
  60. 60. Carrillo-de-la-Peña MT. Effects of intensity and order of stimuli presentation on AEPs: an analysis of the consistency of EP augmenting/reducing in the auditory modality. Clinical neurophysiology. 1999;110(5):924–32. pmid:10400207
  61. 61. Hensch T, Herold U, Diers K, Armbruster D, Brocke B. Reliability of intensity dependence of auditory-evoked potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2008;119(1):224–36. pmid:18039590
  62. 62. West SG, Finch JF, Curran PJ. Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies. 1995.
  63. 63. Miller GA, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2001;110(1):40. pmid:11261398
  64. 64. Hayes AF, Preacher KJ. Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 2014;67(3):451–70. pmid:24188158
  65. 65. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146(4):348–61. pmid:10550486
  66. 66. Heim C, Meinlschmidt G, Nemeroff CB. Neurobiology of early-life stress. Psychiatric Annals. 2003;33(1):18–26.
  67. 67. Hensch T, Brocke B. Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials in anxiety related personality traits. Journal of Psychophysiology. 2004;18(4):238.
  68. 68. Hensch T, Herold U, Brocke B. An electrophysiological endophenotype of hypomanic and hyperthymic personality. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2007;101(1):13–26.
  69. 69. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301(5631):386–9. pmid:12869766
  70. 70. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological bulletin. 2009;135(6):885. pmid:19883141
  71. 71. Johnson SL, Turner RJ, Iwata N. BIS/BAS levels and psychiatric disorder: An epidemiological study. Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment. 2003;25(1):25–36.
  72. 72. Dayan P, Huys QJ. Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood. PLoS computational biology. 2008;4(2):e4. pmid:18248087
  73. 73. Bifulco A, Kwon J, Jacobs C, Moran PM, Bunn A, Beer N. Adult attachment style as mediator between childhood neglect/abuse and adult depression and anxiety. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology. 2006;41(10):796–805. pmid:16871369
  74. 74. Lee A, Hankin BL. Insecure attachment, dysfunctional attitudes, and low self-esteem predicting prospective symptoms of depression and anxiety during adolescence. Journal of clinical child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38(2):219–31.
  75. 75. Overbeck JR, Neale MA, Govan CL. I feel, therefore you act: Intrapersonal and interpersonal effects of emotion on negotiation as a function of social power. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2010;112(2):126–39.
  76. 76. Weiner B. Intrapersonal and interpersonal theories of motivation from an attributional perspective. Educational psychology review. 2000;12(1):1–14.
  77. 77. Corr PJ. The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality: Cambridge University Press; 2008.
  78. 78. Levita L, Bois C, Healey A, Smyllie E, Papakonstantinou E, Hartley T, et al. The Behavioural Inhibition System, anxiety and hippocampal volume in a non-clinical population. Biology of mood & anxiety disorders. 2014;4(1):4.