Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sex differences in aggression: Differential roles of 5-HT2, neuropeptide F and tachykinin

  • Andrew N. Bubak,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of Colorado-Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, Colorado, United States of America

  • Michael J. Watt,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Current address: Department of Anatomy, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand

    Affiliation Center for Brain and Behavior Research, Basic Biomedical Sciences, University of South Dakota, Vermillion, South Dakota, United States of America

  • Kenneth J. Renner,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Biology Department, University of South Dakota, Vermillion, South Dakota, United States of America

  • Abigail A. Luman,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Integrative Biology, University of Colorado-Denver, Denver, United States of America

  • Jamie D. Costabile,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Integrative Biology, University of Colorado-Denver, Denver, United States of America

  • Erin J. Sanders,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Integrative Biology, University of Colorado-Denver, Denver, United States of America

  • Jaime L. Grace,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Bradley University, Peoria, Illinois, United States of America

  • John G. Swallow

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    john.swallow@ucdenver.edu

    Affiliation Department of Integrative Biology, University of Colorado-Denver, Denver, United States of America

Abstract

Despite the conserved function of aggression across taxa in obtaining critical resources such as food and mates, serotonin’s (5-HT) modulatory role on aggressive behavior appears to be largely inhibitory for vertebrates but stimulatory for invertebrates. However, critical gaps exist in our knowledge of invertebrates that need to be addressed before definitively stating opposing roles for 5-HT and aggression. Specifically, the role of 5-HT receptor subtypes are largely unknown, as is the potential interactive role of 5-HT with other neurochemical systems known to play a critical role in aggression. Similarly, the influence of these systems in driving sex differences in aggressive behavior of invertebrates is not well understood. Here, we investigated these questions by employing complementary approaches in a novel invertebrate model of aggression, the stalk-eyed fly. A combination of altered social conditions, pharmacological manipulation and 5-HT2 receptor knockdown by siRNA revealed an inhibitory role of this receptor subtype on aggression. Additionally, we provide evidence for 5-HT2’s involvement in regulating neuropeptide F activity, a suspected inhibitor of aggression. However, this function appears to be stage-specific, altering only the initiation stage of aggressive conflicts. Alternatively, pharmacologically increasing systemic concentrations of 5-HT significantly elevated the expression of the neuropeptide tachykinin, which did not affect contest initiation but instead promoted escalation via production of high intensity aggressive behaviors. Notably, these effects were limited solely to males, with female aggression and neuropeptide expression remaining unaltered by any manipulation that affected 5-HT. Together, these results demonstrate a more nuanced role for 5-HT in modulating aggression in invertebrates, revealing an important interactive role with neuropeptides that is more reminiscent of vertebrates. The sex-differences described here also provide valuable insight into the evolutionary contexts of this complex behavior.

Introduction

Serotonin (5-HT) appears to promote aggression in invertebrates [1,2], in contrast to the largely inhibitory effect seen in vertebrates ([3], but see [4]). Much of the empirical support for this dichotomy comes from studies using arthropod invertebrates, with increased expression of overt aggressive behavior and greater willingness to engage in conflict seen in decapod crustaceans [58], crickets [9], ants [10,11], and dipteran flies [1215] following pharmacological or genetic elevations of 5-HT at the systemic level. While these findings support the presumption that 5-HT has opposing effects on invertebrate aggression from vertebrates, there are critical gaps in knowledge that need to be considered before accurately stating that 5-HT exclusively modulates invertebrate aggression in a positive manner.

A more nuanced role for 5-HT in invertebrate aggression emerges when considering involvement of receptor subtypes. In vertebrates, differential binding of specific 5-HT receptors, predominantly 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, and 5-HT2 subtypes, has profound implications for aggressive behavior [16,17]. Notable sequence and functional homology for these subtypes have been described in invertebrates [18], and the limited information available suggests some similarity in their influence on insect aggression [19]. For example, specific pharmacological activation of 5-HT2 receptors has an anti-aggressive effect in both rodents [20] and Drosophila [19], suggesting 5-HT2 receptor function is conserved evolutionarily. In contrast, a divergent role is indicated for 5-HT1A receptors, activation of which largely dampens mammalian aggression [21] while enhancing aggressive behavior in Drosophila [19]. Whether these same similarities and differences in subtype function exist in invertebrates other than Drosophila remains to be determined.

It is also possible that 5-HT receptors have distinct functions in mediating the contextual expression of specific aggressive behaviors and their intensity, which in turn will direct how the conflict proceeds (i.e., initiation, escalation, and termination). For instance, while 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 receptors are generally inhibitory for vertebrate aggression, agonists of these receptors can promote high intensity aggression in mammals during certain situations such as maternal, territorial, and self-defense [21,22], demonstrating these subtypes can exert opposing effects according to context. In male Drosophila, overall aggression is heightened or decreased according to 5-HT1A or 5-HT2 receptor activation, respectively, but 5-HT1A predominantly affects expression of low intensity aggression (threat displays) while 5-HT2 mediates high intensity behaviors such as lunging [19]. Contextual modulation of aggression by 5-HT is also seen in male stalk-eyed flies (Teleopsis dalmanni). Specifically, smaller males display low intensity aggression and initiate less contests when faced with a larger opponent, but pharmacologically increasing 5-HT causes the smaller male to initiate and escalate fights by promoting high-intensity aggression [23]. Interestingly, contest duration remains the same whether the smaller male is treated with 5-HT or not [23]. In contrast, when contestants are of equal size, both aggression intensity and contest duration appear to be modulated as a function of the difference in brain 5-HT between opponents [2]. In other words, size-matched opponents with closer brain 5-HT concentrations will engage in prolonged high intensity contests regardless of whether 5-HT has been increased in one male, with exogenous manipulation only making contests shorter and less intense if it causes brain 5-HT to be substantially elevated above that of the opponent [2]. Thus, aggression in T. dalmanni provides a useful model for examining how 5-HT can discretely modulate behavioral expression according to specific contexts, which will in turn determine when conflicts are initiated/terminated and if there is an escalation in the intensity of aggression during the interaction. However, it is not known if these differential effects are dependent on 5-HT receptor specificity. This relationship between 5-HT receptor subtype and aggression in T. dalmanni was investigated in the current study.

The extent to which 5-HT modulates discrete aggressive behaviors in invertebrates may also be influenced by the actions of neuropeptide systems, as shown for vertebrates. For example, lesioning neurons containing tachykinin (Tk) receptors reduced violent attacks in rats but left milder attacks unaffected [24]. Similarly, high-intensity aggressive behavior during intrasexual contests is elicited by activation of Tk neurons in male Drosophila [25]. Overlap in function is also seen with neuropeptide Y (NPY) and its invertebrate homolog neuropeptide F (NPF), which decrease frequency of high intensity aggression in mice and Drosophila, respectively [12, 22]. These findings imply evolutionarily conserved roles for these neuropeptides in determining aggression intensity and conflict escalation.

Moreover, NPY has been shown in mammals to exert its effects by modulating 5-HT activity [22], and conversely, 5-HT directly influences NPY release in brain regions that mediate aggression [2629]. However, unlike mammals, 5-HT and NPF pathways appear to act independently in regulating Drosophila aggression [10]. Receptors for Tk are located on both 5-HT and non-5-HT neurons within the mammalian 5-HT cell body region (dorsal raphe), and can modulate neuronal firing and 5-HT release in terminal regions [30]. In contrast, Tk and 5-HT do not appear to be co-localized to the same neurons in the majority of invertebrates [3137], and while they have been shown to exert similar excitatory effects on crustacean cardiac and gut ganglia [38,39], it is unclear whether Tk and 5-HT influence each other to regulate invertebrate aggression. Combined, these findings highlight the need to consider multiple discrete measures of aggressive behavior (e.g., frequency and intensity plus contest duration) along with activity of other neuromodulators when determining the role of 5-HT as a whole.

Finally, a seemingly more troubling limitation in our knowledge of how 5-HT mediates aggressive behavior is the lack of studies conducted with female invertebrates. Although several studies have described neural pathways involved in aggression and related behaviors, the almost exclusive use of male subjects precludes determining whether these are sex-specific (for review see [40]). Given the stark contrast in aggression and related behaviors between the sexes of many invertebrate species, as well as morphological dissimilarities [1], it is reasonable to suggest significant sex differences in underlying neural mechanisms. For instance, in the stalk-eyed fly, T. dalmanni, both sexes possess elongated eyestalks and engage in intrasexual aggressive contests over food resources, but unlike for males, the relatively shorter eyestalks of females do not appear to be used for assessment of opponents [41,42]. Further, qualitative comparison across studies using either sex suggests females rarely engage in high intensity aggression (e.g., [1,41,42]). Pharmacologically increasing 5-HT in a smaller male will override the inhibition of aggression evoked by perception of a larger opponent with longer eyestalks [23], and expression of high intensity aggression by males in size-matched or mismatched contests is increased by 5-HT treatment [2]. The sex difference in opponent perception and aggression level, combined with the known effects of 5-HT on these behaviors in males, makes T. dalmanni an ideal model species for examining whether insect aggression is modulated by 5-HT in a sex-dependent manner.

Here, we sought to address some of the gaps in knowledge about 5-HT influences on invertebrate aggression by employing a range of complementary experimental approaches using both male and female T. dalmanni. Specifically, we determined 1) whether 5-HT receptor subtypes mediate expression of aggressive intensity and contest progression, 2) if there is a direct functional relationship between 5-HT subtypes and the neuropeptides Tk and NPF in regulating aggression, and 3) how these findings differ between sexes. Using a combination of socio-environmental manipulation (like vertebrates, social insects exhibit alterations in behavior in response to isolation [43], RNA interference (RNAi), and pharmacological treatment, we describe sex-specific roles for 5-HT2 receptors, Tk and NPF in behavioral expression and how this relates to contest progression and outcome. We also demonstrate a direct effect of 5-HT on neuropeptide expression that is posited to modulate male aggression. Our results reveal the impressive level of conservation with respect to neurochemical mechanisms among species as diverse as vertebrates and invertebrates, and highlights the need to consider multiple factors when determining potential taxonomic differences in how 5-HT mediates aggression.

Materials and methods

Subjects

The sexually dimorphic stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni, is native to South East Asia. All flies used in this study were descendants of pupae collected from wild populations in 2012, Gombak Field Station, housed at the University of Maryland, College Park. Individuals were housed communally in cages (45 cm x 22 cm x 19 cm) on a 12-h light:dark cycle with free access to food, water, and mates. Sexually mature flies in this study were briefly anesthetized using ice and eye span measured to the nearest 0.01 mm using cellSens standard software (Olympus). Size-matched individuals were determined as being < 1% difference in eye span. The high correlation of eye span to body size in this species makes eye span an accurate representation of body size [44, 45]. Predetermined opponents were given identifying paint marks between their thoracic spines and housed in separate communal enclosures prior to surgeries and fight contests. Isolated flies were housed individually for 7 days prior to fight contests.

Drug administration

Flies selected for 5-HT manipulation were fed sterilized, pureed sweet corn containing either 3 g of 5-hydroxy-L-tryptophan (5-HTP; H9772; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) in 100 mL media or vehicle/100 mL media for 4 days as previously described [14, 15]. Specifically, the vehicle media contained 100 mL of corn, 1 mL of methylparaben (Ward’s Science, Rochester, NY) as a mold inhibitor [45], and 25 mg of ascorbic acid (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) to act as a stabilizer [12].

Forced-fight paradigm and behavioral analysis

Opponent matchups for the experiments described below are as follows: socially isolated vs socially reared, 5-HT2 siRNA vs vehicle siRNA, and 5-HTP treated vs vehicle treated. Using previously published methods (e.g., [15]), size- and sex-matched opponents were placed in an arena (11 cm x 6.5 cm x 5 cm) containing a glass wall and ceiling, for filming, and a removable cardboard barrier that separated the individuals. Flies were starved for 12 hours prior to the contest to increase the incentive to fight over a piece of pureed corn that was placed in the arena center immediately following barrier removal. All contests that occurred during 10 mins of fighting were scored using the behavioral scoring software, JWatcher (UCLA). Scored behaviors for each individual were based on an existing ethogram adapted from Egge et al. [46]. Specific behaviors fall into three categories: contest initiation, escalation, and termination. Contest initiations were determined by one opponent approaching the other, which ultimately results in an aggressive behavioral exchange. Only one opponent was awarded an initiation per contest, none were awarded if there was ambiguity in determining the initiator. Escalations of fights were determined by high-intensity (HI) physical contact behaviors (see [15]). Termination and subsequent winners of contest were determined by retreats, with the individual with the fewest number of retreats after the 10 minute fighting period deemed the winner. Behavioral data counts can be access in supplemental files (S1 File).

RNA isolation and quantification

RNA isolation was conducted from dissected whole-brain tissue using the Direct-zolTM RNA MiniPrep (ZYMO Research, Irvine, CA) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Isolated RNA was reverse transcribed using the Maxima First Strand cDNA synthesis kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA). Quantitative PCR was performed by an Applied Biosystem’s StepOne machine (Foster City, CA) using TaqMan master mix (Applied Biosystems), the primer-probe pair for the endogenous control, Gapdh, and one of the following target gene primer-probe pairs: 5-HT1A, 5-HT2, 5-HT7, SERT, Tk, or NPFr (Integrated DNA Technologies, Coralville, Iowa; Table 1). We intentionally focused on the Tk ligand itself and not the receptor due to ambiguity in the specific receptor responsible for baseline aggression in dipteran flies [24]. Relative quantification (RQ) of the PCR product was conducted using the comparative CT method [47]. Values were presented as target gene expression relative to the endogenous control Gapdh).

thumbnail
Table 1. The primer-probe pair for the endogenous control, Gapdh, and the targeted genes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.t001

RNAi design and administration

Results from the RNA quantification revealed that expression of 5-HT2 receptors in male flies was approximately half that seen in females, while 5-HT1A expression was 1.5 times higher in males (see Results). More aggressive isolated male flies also had lower 5-HT2 expression than their socially-housed counterparts, while no difference was seen in 5-HT1A receptors). Studies in male Drosophila have indicated that 5-HT2 receptors have a specific inhibitory effect on high intensity aggression, while 5-HT1A receptors promote low intensity (non-contact) behaviors [19]. In addition, sex differences in T. dalmanni intrasexual aggression predominantly relate to lack of high intensity behaviors in females, which have higher 5-HT2 receptor expression than males. Therefore, we conducted loss-of-function studies targeting the 5-HT2 rather than the 5-HT1A receptor for examining how 5-HT receptor subtype modulates aggression and neuropeptide activity in this species.

Dicer-substrate siRNAs (short interfering RNA) used in this study were synthesized by IDT (Integrated DNA Technologies, Coralville, IA). Probes targeting 5-HT2 were generated using cDNA sequences of T. dalmanni. Probes targeting green fluorescent protein (GFP) were generated from cDNA sequences of Aequorea coerulescens. The siRNA was coated (1:1 ratio) in the transfection reagent polyethylenimine (PEI) to aid in membrane penetration and stabilization. Injections were administered in the head cavity through the base of the proboscis. The brain was not penetrated with the needle.

Males and females were randomly selected from a social cage and size- and sex-matched to an opponent. Treatment groups were allocated randomly, with experimental males and females being injected with 100 nL of 5-HT2 siRNA solution and control flies injected with 100 nL of GFP siRNA solution. All subjects were returned to their previous housing conditions and allowed to recover for 48 hours with free access to food and water. Flies were then removed from their respective housing conditions/food for 12 hours and placed in the forced-fight paradigm described above.

5-HT quantification

Quantification of brain 5-HT was conducted using high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) with electrochemical detection as previously described [14]. Briefly, whole brains were dissected and frozen in 60 μL of acetate buffer containing the internal standard α-methyl-dopamine (Merck) and stored at -80°C. Samples were thawed and centrifuged at 17,000 rpms. A portion of the sample (45 μL) was injected into the chromatographic system and the amines were separated with a C18 4 μm NOVA-PAK radial compression column (Waters Associates, Inc. Milford, MA) and detected using an LC 4 potentiostat and a glassy carbon electrode (Bioanalytical systems, West Lafayette, IN). The CSW32 data program (DataApex Ltd., Czech Republic) calculated monoamine concentrations based on peak height values that were obtained from standards (Sigma-Aldridge, St. Louis, MO). The remaining sample was solubilized for protein analysis with 60 μL of 0.4 M NaOH [48]. The final amine concentrations are expressed as pg amine/ μg protein following appropriate corrections for injection vs preparation volume.

Statistical analysis

To account for behavioral data not meeting assumptions of normality and equal variance, separate two-tailed Wilcoxon matched pairs signed-rank tests were used to compare frequency of contest initiations, expression of high intensity aggression and contest outcome between treated and control groups. Separate two-tailed Student’s t-tests were applied to test for relative differences in mean mRNA expression for 5-HT1A, 5-HT2, 5-HT7, SERT, Tk, or NPFr between treatment groups, as well as for differences in mean 5-HT concentrations as quantified by HPLC. Alpha level was set at 0.05 throughout, and analyses performed using Prism (GraphPad Software, Inc; La Jolla, CA). We tested for dependences in the behavioral data using Spearman rank correlations. No significant relationships in frequency of behaviors were found.

Results and discussion

Expression of 5-HT1A, 5-HT2, 5-HT7 receptors and the serotonin transporter in male and female brains

Since 5-HT plays a prominent role in modulating invertebrate aggression [1], including increasing aggressive displays in male T. dalmanni, we first measured relative sex differences in mRNA expression of 5-HT1A, 5-HT2, and 5-HT7 receptors and the 5-HT transporter (SERT; Fig 1). Males had markedly lower expression levels of the 5-HT2 receptor subtype (Student’s t-test, p < 0.001) and SERT (Student’s t-test, p <0.001) but significantly higher levels of 5-HT1A (Student’s t-test, p < 0.01; Fig 1) compared to females. Males and females had similar expression levels of the 5-HT7 receptor subtype (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Relative mRNA expression between normally reared adult male and female stalk-eyed flies.

Males have a significantly lower expression of 5-HT2 (0.41 ± 0.03, n = 9 vs 1.0 ± 0.002, n = 9) and SERT (0.284 ± 0.02, n = 9 vs 1.0 ± 0.001, n = 9) but significantly higher expression levels of 5-HT1A (1.64 ± 0.17, n = 12 vs 1.0 ± 0.001, n = 9) compared to their female counterparts. There was no statistical difference in expression levels of 5-HT7 between males and females. Values are normalized to female expression levels and presented as mean ± SEM. (Two-tailed, Student’s t-test; p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g001

Expression of the neuropeptide Tk and the NPF receptor in male and female brains

We also investigated the expression of the NPF receptor in male and female flies, since NPY and the invertebrate analog NPF inhibits aggressive behavior in other animal species [12, 22]. We also measured the baseline expression of the neuropeptide Tk, which has been reported to increase aggression in vertebrates and Drosophila [24, 25]. Baseline relative mRNA expression levels of the NPF receptor (1 ± 0.07, n = 12 vs 0.86 ± 0.05, n = 12; Student’s t-test, p > 0.05) and Tk (1 ± 0.23, n = 7 vs 0.95 ± 0.17, n = 9; Student’s t-test, p > 0.05) were similar between females and males, respectfully.

Increased aggression corresponds with reduced 5-HT2 and heightened Tk expression in males but not in females

Social isolation increases aggression in both invertebrate and vertebrate species [19, 4951]. We first tested whether social isolation induced aggression in male stalk-eyed flies. Following 7 days of isolation, males had a higher probability of winning the aggressive confrontation compared to their socially reared opponents (Fig 2D; 85% vs 15%). Isolated males also initiated significantly more confrontations (Fig 2A; 7.21 ± 0.81 versus 4.05 ± 0.87, mean ± SEM, respectively; Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, p < 0.01) and performed more high-intensity (physical) behaviors than their socially-reared opponents (Fig 2C; 4.79 ± 1.12 versus 1.63 ± 0.58, mean ± SEM, respectively; Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, p < 0.05). When we repeated this experiment with female flies we found no difference in either initiations or escalations of aggression in socially-isolated females when compared to colony-raised females (Fig 2B). Females exhibited low levels of aggression making it difficult to ascertain a clear winner of a given contest. Thus, the probability of winning an aggressive contest was not computed. There was not a clear difference in aggressive behaviors between isolated and colony raised female flies.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Effects of social isolation on behavior.

Social isolation increased initiations, escalations, and winning probability in males but not females. A) Males, socially isolated, performed significantly more initiations compared to their socially reared opponents. B) Social isolation had no effect on initiations in female aggressive contests. C) Males, socially isolated, performed significantly more high-intensity aggressive behaviors compared to their socially reared opponents. Behavior values are normalized to control opponents with connecting black lines representing opponents. Red lines represent the mean slope of the differences in behaviors performed between paired opponents and the blue shaded areas represent the 95% confidence interval. (Two-tailed, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test; n = 19, p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***). D) Males that were socially isolated had a significantly higher winning percentage compared to their socially reared opponents (85% vs 15%; error bars represent 95% confidence interval).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g002

Next, we measured brain 5-HT2 and Tk expression to determine if expression levels covaried with the display of increased aggression. In male flies raised in social isolation, there was a decrease in 5-HT2 expression relative to colony-raised flies (Fig 3; Student’s t-test, p < 0.01). In contrast, Tk expression was markedly increased in brains obtained from socially isolated male flies relative to colony-raised males (Fig 3; Student’s t-test, p < 0.001). Expression of 5-HT1A, 5-HT7, and NPFr were not different between groups (Fig 3). In contrast, when the experiment was repeated in females, we found expression levels of 5-HT2 and Tk in socially isolated flies did not differ significantly from values obtained in colony-raised flies (Fig 3). Similar to males, females did not show differences in the expression of brain 5-HT1A, 5-HT7, or NPFr following isolation (data not shown).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effects of social isolation on gene expression.

Social isolation reduced 5-HT2 expression and increased Tk expression in males but not females. A) Expression of 5-HT2 reduced ~28% in males following social isolation (1.0 ± 0.074, n = 9 vs .72 ± 0.06, n = 12) whereas Tk expression was increased ~77% compared to socially reared males (1.0 ± 0.13, n = 9 vs 1.77 ± 0.04, n = 6). B) No change in expression value of 5-HT2 or Tk was measured in females following social isolation. Similar to males, females did not show differences in 5-HT1A, 5-HT7, or NPFr expression levels (data not shown). Values are normalized to socially reared expression levels and presented as mean ± SEM. (Two-tailed, Student’s t-test; p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g003

5-HT2 knock-down increases aggression in males but not in females

To functionally test the role of 5-HT2 in aggression, we designed siRNA to selectively knock-down the receptor subtype. Intracranial injections of 5-HT2 siRNA reduced 5-HT2 receptor expression by approximately 30% 48 hours after the injection compared to control-injected individuals (siRNA generated to target GFP; Fig 4; Student’s t-test, p < 0.05). Importantly, the designed 5-HT2 siRNA was specific, as it did not affect the expression of the closely related 5-HT1A receptor (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effects of 5-HT2 siRNA injections on 5-HT2 expression.

Individual flies injected with 5-HT2 siRNA had significantly reduced 5-HT2 expression levels compared to vehicle injected siRNA (1.0 ± .09, n = 9 vs 0.68 ± 0.06, n = 6). 5-HT2 siRNA did not affect expression levels of the closely related 5-HT1A receptor. Values are normalized to vehicle injected individuals and presented as mean ± SEM. (Two-tailed, Student’s t-test; p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g004

Socially-reared males injected with 5-HT2 siRNA initiated more confrontations compared to their vehicle-injected opponents (Fig 5; Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, p < 0.001). However, high-intensity behaviors in males did not differ between 5-HT2 siRNA injected and vehicle-injected opponents (Fig 5). Female opponents did not differ in either initiations or high-intensity behaviors, despite 5-HT2 siRNA-injected females having significantly lower 5-HT2 expression levels compared to their vehicle-treated opponents (Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effects of 5-HT2 siRNA injections on behavior.

5-HT2 siRNA injections increased initiations and winning probability in males but not females. A) Males, injected with 5-HT2 siRNA, performed significantly more initiations compared to their vehicle injected opponents. B) 5-HT2 siRNA had no effect on initiations in female aggressive contests. C) Males, injected with 5-HT2 siRNA, had no differences in high-intensity aggressive behaviors compared to their vehicle injected opponents. Behavior values are normalized to control opponents with connecting black lines representing opponents. Red lines represent the mean slope of the differences in behaviors performed between paired opponents and the blue shaded areas represent the 95% confidence interval. (Two-tailed, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test; n = 15, p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***). D) Males, injected with 5-HT2 siRNA, had a significantly higher winning percentage compared to their vehicle injected opponents (81% vs 19%; error bars represent 95% confidence interval).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g005

5-HT2 activity modulates NPFr expression in males but not females

To probe for potential interactions between 5-HT and neuropeptides in modulating aggression in males, we next measured brain NPFr and Tk expression levels following the siRNA-induced reduction in 5-HT2. Reducing expression of 5-HT2 using siRNA significantly reduced NPFr expression in males (Fig 6; Student’s t-test, p < 0.01). When this experiment was repeated in females, our results showed that reduction of 5HT2 receptors did not significantly affect NPFr expression (Fig 6). Expression of Tk did not differ in males or females treated with 5-HT2 siRNA (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. 5-HT2 siRNA injections.

A) Injections of 5-HT2 siRNA did not alter expression levels of Tk in either males or females compared to their vehicle injected counterparts. B) Injections with 5-HT2 siRNA reduced NPFr expression in males (0.69 ± 0.07, n = 9 vs 1.0 ± 0.06, n = 12) compared to vehicle injected individuals but not in females. Values are normalized to female expression levels and presented as mean ± SEM. (Two-tailed, Student’s t-test; p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g006

Elevating 5-HT concentrations increases Tk expression in males but not females

We have previously reported an increase in high-intensity aggressive behaviors in male stalk-eyed flies following administration of the 5-HT metabolic precursor, 5-HTP [15]. When we repeated this study in female stalk-eyed flies, we found that 5-HTP-treated individuals do not exhibit a significant difference in aggressive behaviors compared to their untreated opponents, despite having significantly elevated brain 5-HT concentrations (18.44 ± 2.99, n = 18 vs 9.8 ± 0.6, n = 19; pg/μg protein; Student’s t-test, p < 0.01). In fact, the observation of a fight containing a single high-intensity behavior was so rare that accurate measurements couldn’t be taken. We next explored whether this sex-difference in the expression of high-intensity behaviors following 5-HTP administration might be related to changes in Tk expression. Indeed, males treated with 5-HTP had significantly higher Tk expression compared to vehicle-treated males (Fig 7; Student’s t-test, p < 0.001). In contrast, females treated with 5-HTP did not have a significant difference in Tk expression when compared to controls (Fig 7A). Additionally, expression of NPFr was also significantly raised in males following 5-HTP treatment but not in females (Fig 7B; Student’s t-test, p < 0.001). We did not find significant differences in the expression of brain levels of 5-HT1A, 5-HT2, or 5-HT7, respectively, in either males or females treated with 5-HTP when compared to vehicle-treated controls.

thumbnail
Fig 7. 5-HTP treatment effects on gene expression.

A) Treatment with 5-HTP increased expression levels of Tk in males compared to vehicle treated individuals (3.46 ± 0.36, n = 12 vs 1.0 ± 0.09, n = 9) but not in females. B) Treatment with 5-HTP increased expression levels of NPFr in males compared to vehicle treated individuals (1.48 ± 0.07, n = 12 vs 1.05 ± 0.11, n = 9) but not in females. Values are normalized to female expression levels and presented as mean ± SEM. (Two-tailed, Student’s t-test; p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g007

In these studies, we used the sexually dimorphic stalk-eyed fly as a model to study how aggression is mediated by both 5-HT and neuropeptides. This model offers several advantages for obtaining a better grasp, both functionally and evolutionarily, of the origin and regulation of this complex behavior across taxa and sexes. First, flies contain fewer neurons and neuronal connections compared to vertebrates while simultaneously maintaining the relevant behaviors. Additionally, the monoaminergic and peptidergic systems involved in these behavioral processes are highly conserved across taxa [18, 24, 25, 5255]. This is not surprising given the fitness benefits and selection pressures of engaging in aggressive conflicts in most living animal species. Finally, the extreme differences in both morphology and behavior between male and female stalk-eyed flies provides a valuable perspective of the proximate mechanisms that have been acted upon by selection forces to result ultimately in sex-specific aggression.

Here, we found that male and female flies raised in social colonies differentially express central 5-HT1A- and 5-HT2-like receptor subtypes and SERT. Specifically, males show higher expression of 5-HT1A receptors but lower expression of 5-HT2 receptors and SERT compared to females. In contrast, 5-HT7 receptor expression was equivalent in both sexes. Naturally reduced SERT in males would presumably result in lower 5-HT clearance and corresponding increases in extracellular 5-HT. Combined with the positive relationship between experimentally enhanced 5-HT and male aggression shown by ourselves [14, 15] and others [12, 13, 19], this points to inherently greater serotonergic activity as an explanation for observed sex differences in stalk-eyed fly aggression. Further, our findings suggest this is principally mediated through a balance of signaling in favor of 5-HT1A versus 5-HT2 receptor activation.

To test this, we investigated the effects of social isolation on aggression and expression of serotonin receptor subtypes in male and female stalk-eyed flies. Previous studies indicate that social vertebrates and invertebrates that are either reared in isolation or exposed to periods of isolation exhibit increases in aggression to conspecifics [19, 4951, 56]. In line with this, we found that socially isolated male stalk-eyed flies increase conflict initiations, fight escalation, and have a higher probability of winning a fight. This result suggests that the absence of socialization in male stalk-eyed flies may impair detection of social cues that dampen normal aggressive responses and prevent the escalation of a fight to potentially injurious intensities. The finding that isolated males exhibit higher levels of aggression is similar to isolation effects on Drosophila behavior [19]. In contrast, while isolation also increases aggression in female Drosophila [57], socially reared and socially-isolated females in our study exhibited similar levels of initiations and escalations in aggressive behavior. When we evaluated the expression of 5-HT-like receptors, there were no differences in either 5-HT1A or 5-HT7 receptors in either sex when compared to socially reared flies. However, the expression of 5-HT2 receptors decreased in males but not females when compared to socially reared controls. These results differ from earlier work in Drosophila which showed that socially-isolated males had decreased expression of 5-HT1A- and increased expression of 5-HT2-like receptor mRNA [19]. These differences may be species specific, but more likely may be related to the isolation protocol employed. In the Drosophila study, the flies were isolated 2 to 3 days post-eclosion and kept in constant light, whereas we isolated sexually mature flies for 7 days and used a 12:12 light dark cycle. Regardless, the isolation-induced reduction in 5-HT2 receptors seen here would allow for greater activation of 5-HT1A receptors even in the absence of altered expression of the latter subtype. Together with our behavioral findings, this further supports the notion that decreased and increased activation of 5-HT2 and 5-HT1A receptors, respectively, is a likely substrate for heightened aggression following either social isolation or enhanced serotonergic activity.

In addition to measuring 5-HT-like receptor subtypes, we also evaluated the expression of Tk and NPF receptors in socially-isolated and socially reared flies. Neither NPF receptors nor Tk in females was affected by social isolation, and expression of NPF receptors did not differ with rearing condition in males. In contrast, the expression of Tk was markedly increased in socially-isolated males. Recent work in Drosophila has shown that neurons expressing Tk regulate male-male aggression [25], suggesting the increase in Tk found in socially-isolated male stalk-eyed flies likely contributes to the increase in aggression displayed by these insects, similar to what is observed in vertebrate species [58]. Further, the finding that only males showed both increased Tk and reduced 5-HT2 receptors implicates the combination of these factors in determining sex-specific changes in aggression caused by social isolation.

The decreased expression of 5-HT2-receptors, increased Tk expression and heightened aggression following social isolation led us to investigate if there was a direct relationship among these factors, which we tested by genetic knockdown of 5-HT2 receptors. In line with our prediction, males exhibited increased aggression with 5HT2 receptor knockdown, suggesting that serotonergic activation of the 5HT2-like receptor normally acts to dampen aggression. This result is consistent with earlier work in Drosophila, which showed that treatments with 5-HT2 agonists decreased aggression [19], and further implicates the balance between 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 signaling in determining expression of aggressive behavior. Interestingly, 5-HT2 knockdown had no effect on male Tk expression, suggesting that the increase in Tk observed in socially isolated males is independent of the decrease in the expression of the 5-HT2 receptor subtype. While Tk was not affected by 5-HT2 knockdown, there was a significant reduction in expression of NPF receptors in male brains. In Drosophila, NPF decreases aggression [12], hence the decreased expression of NPF receptors (and subsequent dampening of NPF signaling) seen in our study may contribute to the increased aggression exhibited by male stalk-eyed flies following knockdown of the 5-HT2-like receptor. Work using genetically modified male Drosophila suggests that 5-HT and NPF function independently to modulate aggression in an additive manner [12]. Similarly, increased aggressive behavior displayed by neuropeptide Y1 receptor-knockdown mice is normalized by pharmacological activation of 5-HT1A receptors [22], implying independent actions of 5-HT and NPY in mediating vertebrate aggression. However, other behaviors mediated by central NPY activity in rodents, such as feeding and helplessness, have been shown to be directly linked with 5-HT2 receptor function [59, 60]. Our results suggest that aggression in stalk-eyed flies may be similarly modulated by 5-HT2 receptor-dependent interactions between 5-HT and NPF signaling, in contrast to the dissociated effects of 5-HT and NPF on aggression as shown in mice and Drosophila. Combined, it is tempting to speculate that activation of 5-HT2 receptors in T. dalmanni may directly increase NPF to act as a brake on expression of male aggression.

While only NPF receptor expression was affected by 5-HT2 knockdown, male flies showed increases in both Tk and NPF receptor expression following 5-HTP administration. The finding that enhancing 5-HT levels can increase NPF receptors is reminiscent of neurochemical interactions in the mammalian hypothalamus, where 5-HT can directly enhance release of the vertebrate homolog NPY [26, 27], and suggests similar mechanisms in stalk-eyed flies. The means by which 5-HT may be acting to increase Tk in male flies is not clear, given the lack of neuronal co-localization of Tk-like peptides and 5-HT in most invertebrates [3137]. However, 5-HT neurons do appear to synapse on to Tk-immunoreactive terminals in the brain of the desert locust, Schistocerca gregana [34], suggesting a direct functional relationship via synaptic contact that may also be present in T. dalmanni.

Notably, the effects of 5-HT2 receptor knockdown on aggression and NPFr were specific to males, with treated females showing no change either aggressive behavior, Tk or NPFr compared to vehicle-treated females. Similarly, 5-HTP treatment, which reliably increases male aggression [14, 15], had no effect on female aggression, despite causing an increase in brain 5-HT. Effects of social isolation both on aggression and on expression of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 receptors and Tk were also restricted to males. When taken in conjunction with the fact that males have naturally lower 5-HT2 but elevated 5-HT1A receptor expression compared with females, along with reduced SERT, this implies that male aggression is much more tightly linked with elevated serotonergic signaling via specific subtypes. Specifically, we posit that reductions in 5-HT2 activation in favor of 5-HT1A function may form the mechanistic basis of sex differences in regulation of aggression in this species. Further, the effects of altered 5-HT activity in promoting male aggression are in turn modulated by Tk and NPF, whereas these neuropeptides appear to play no role in female aggression. Identifying these mechanisms is an important step in elucidating factors driving the evolution of sex-specific behaviors in invertebrates.

Comparing the effects of socio-environmental factors against those of genetic and pharmacological manipulation provided a valuable opportunity to elucidate how discrete components of an aggressive interaction are specifically mediated by monoaminergic and peptidergic activity (Table 2). For example, the increase in both aggression and NPF receptor expression following 5-HTP treatment in male flies seems counterintuitive, since NPF is posited to be inhibitory towards aggression. However, parsing out contest initiation, escalation and outcome across experimental procedures reveals that while 5-HTP administration increases expression of high intensity behaviors, it does not affect initiations [14,15]. In contrast, increases in initiations were seen after both social isolation and 5-HT2 receptor knockdown, and were associated with either no change or a decrease in NPF receptor expression, respectively. Increases in NPF signaling may therefore only be important in decreasing the motivation to engage in the opening stages of the contest (Fig 8), but have little to no effect in fight escalation, which differs from the direct suppression of high intensity aggression by this peptide seen in mice [22] and Drosophila [12]. Similarly, both 5-HTP treatment and 5-HT2 receptor knockdown enhance the probability of winning, yet each manipulation results in opposite changes to NPF receptor expression (Table 2), suggesting NPF has no role in determining contest outcome. Stage specific functions for 5-HT2 receptors and Tk were also indicated when comparing across experimental manipulations (Table 2), with a primary role suggested for 5-HT2 receptors in contest initiations, while Tk serves to modulate high intensity aggression (Fig 8). However, winning appears to be associated with both reduced 5-HT2 receptor expression and heightened Tk (Table 2). Together, findings suggest the motivation to engage in a contest is enhanced by decreased 5-HT2 receptor activation, which may be potentiated by reduced NPF signaling. As the contest progresses, reduced 5-HT2 activity is maintained but no longer influences behavior, with escalations in intensity driven largely by increases in Tk. The combination of increased initiations and high intensity aggression then serves to increase the probability of winning (Fig 8).

thumbnail
Fig 8. Schematic diagram illustrating proposed relationships between 5-HT / neuropeptide signaling and stages of aggressive conflict in male stalk-eyed flies.

The motivation to engage in aggression appears to be promoted by a reduction in 5-HT2 receptor activity, which may directly inhibit NPF signaling. Escalation in aggression as the contest proceeds is driven by an increase in Tk, with 5-HT2 receptors and NPF no longer influencing behavior. Greater willingness to engage and subsequent expression of high intensity aggression then facilitate the probability of winning.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.g008

thumbnail
Table 2. Effects of experimental manipulations on expression of 5-HT receptors, neuropeptides and aggressive behavior in male stalk-eyed flies, Teleopsis dalmanni.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.t002

It is generally presented that 5-HT has a largely inhibitory role in vertebrate aggression, including humans [6166] while facilitating invertebrate aggression [1, 6, 1215, 6769]. Here, we used a novel invertebrate model to show a more nuanced role for 5-HT and its receptor subtypes in modulating aggression according to the stage of conflict. Similarly differentiated functions of 5-HT receptor subtypes on the type of aggressive behavior are seen in mice and Drosophila, suggesting evolutionary conservation in neurochemical modulation of social behavior. However, it should be noted that our results with stalk-eyed flies point to a primary effect of NPF on contest initiation and not escalation, whereas the reverse pattern is seen in Drosophila [12]. Our findings also indicate the possibility for a direct functional relationship between 5-HT receptors and NPF signaling in regulating aggression, which does not appear to be present in Drosophila [12]. Such differences between two Dipteran fly species within the same taxonomic section (Diptera: Schizophora) cautions against generalizing findings from one species across others, but at the same time offers a valuable means of examining mechanisms leading to evolutionary divergence in behavior. We also show that the impact of 5-HT’s interactive role with neuropeptides on discrete stages of aggressive conflict can be inhibitory, stimulatory, or even absent depending on the particular neuropeptide and on the sex of the individual. Thus, stating opposing roles for 5-HT between vertebrates and invertebrates overlooks the intricate and conserved interactive role this monoaminergic system has with other neurochemical systems known to influence aggressive behavior, while also not accounting for effects of sex. Investigating whether these interactions exist and function similarly in a range of animal species undergoing diverse selection pressures will aid in uncovering the origins, and by extension mechanisms, of this complex behavior.

Supporting information

S1 File. Fight raw data counts.

Behavioral data counts for total initiations and high-intensity behaviors between opponents: males socially reared versus isolation, females socially reared versus isolation, males siRNA versus vehicle, females siRNA versus vehicle.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203980.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Michael Greene for his helpful comments that improved the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Bubak AN, Grace JL, Watt MJ, Renner KJ, Swallow JG. Neurochemistry as a bridge between morphology and behavior: perspectives on aggression in insects. Current Zoology. 2014; 60(6); 778–790.
  2. 2. Bubak AN, Gerken AR, Watt MJ, Costabile JD, Renner KJ, Swallow JG. Assessment strategies and fighting patterns in animal contests: a role for serotonin? Current Zoology. 2016; 62(3): 257–263. pmid:29491913
  3. 3. Nelson RJ, Trainor BC. Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007; 8(7); 536–546. pmid:17585306
  4. 4. Olivier B, Mos J, Van Oorschot R, Hen R. Serotonin receptors and animal models of aggressive behavior. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1995; 28(S 2): 80–90.
  5. 5. Antonsen BL, Paul DH. Serotonin and octopamine elicit stereotypical agonistic behaviors in the squat lobster Munida quadrispina (Anomura, Galatheidae). J Comp Physiol A. 1997; 181: 501–510.
  6. 6. Huber R, Orzeszyna M, Pokorny N, Kravitz EA. Biogenic amines and aggression: experimental approaches in crustaceans. Brain, behavior and evolution. 1997; 50(Suppl. 1): 60–68.
  7. 7. Panksepp JB, Yue Z, Drerup C, Huber R. Amine neurochemistry and aggression in crayfish. Microsc Res Tech. 2003; 60(3): 360–368. pmid:12539165
  8. 8. Momohara Y, Kanai A. Nagayama T. Aminergic control of social status in crayfish agonistic encounters. PloS ONE. 2013; 8(9): p.e74489. pmid:24058575
  9. 9. Dyakonova VE, Krushinsky AL. Serotonin precursor (5-hydroxytryptophan) causes substantial changes in the fighting behavior of male crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus. J Comp Physiol A. 2013; 199(7): 601–609.
  10. 10. Bubak AN, Yaeger JD, Renner KJ, Swallow JG, Greene MJ. Neuromodulation of Nestmate Recognition Decisions by Pavement Ants. PloS ONE. 2016; 11(11): p.e0166417. pmid:27846261
  11. 11. Szczuka A, Korczyńska J, Wnuk A, Symonowicz B, Szwacka AG, Mazurkiewicz P, et al. The effects of serotonin, dopamine, octopamine and tyramine on behavior of workers of the ant Formica polyctena during dyadic aggression tests. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2013; 73: 495–520.
  12. 12. Dierick HA, Greenspan RJ. Serotonin and neuropeptide F have opposite modulatory effects on fly aggression. Nature genetics. 2013; 39(5): 678–682.
  13. 13. Alekseyenko OV, Lee C, Kravitz EA. Targeted manipulation of serotonergic neurotransmission affects the escalation of aggression in adult male Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5(5): e10806. pmid:20520823
  14. 14. Bubak AN, Swallow JG, Renner KJ. Whole brain monoamine detection and manipulation in a stalk-eyed fly. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2013; 219: 124–130. pmid:23891953
  15. 15. Bubak A.N, Renner KJ, Swallow JG. Heightened serotonin influences contest outcome and enhances expression of high-intensity aggressive behaviors. Behavioural brain research. 2014; 259: 137–142. pmid:24211450
  16. 16. Juárez P, Valdovinos MG, May ME, Lloyd BP, Couppis MH, Kennedy CH. Serotonin 2A/C receptors mediate the aggressive phenotype of TLX gene knockout mice. Behavioural brain research. 2013; 256: 354–361. pmid:23933143
  17. 17. Takahashi A, Quadros IM, de Almeida RM, Miczek KA. Brain serotonin receptors and transporters: initiation vs. termination of escalated aggression. Psychopharmacology. 2011; 213(2–3):183–212. pmid:20938650
  18. 18. Tierney AJ. Structure and function of invertebrate 5-HT receptors: a review. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A. 2001; 128(4): 791–804.
  19. 19. Johnson O, Becnel J, Nichols CD. Serotonin 5-HT(2) and 5-HT(1A)-like receptors differentially modulate aggressive behaviors in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuroscience. 2009; 158: 1292–1300. pmid:19041376
  20. 20. Sánchez C, Arnt J, Hyttel J, Moltzen EK. The role of serotonergic mechanisms in inhibition of isolation-induced aggression in male mice. Psychopharmacology. 1993; 110(1): 53–59.
  21. 21. Takahashi A, Quadros IM, de Almeida RM, Miczek KA. Behavioral and pharmacogenetics of aggressive behavior. In Behavioral neurogenetics (pp. 73–138). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. 2011.
  22. 22. Karl T, Lin S, Schwarzer C, Sainsbury A, Couzens M, Wittmann W, et. al. Y1 receptors regulate aggressive behavior by modulating serotonin pathways. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2004; 101(34): 12742–12747.
  23. 23. Bubak AN, Rieger NS, Watt MJ, Renner KJ, Swallow JG. David vs. Goliath: Serotonin modulates opponent perception between smaller and larger rivals. Behavioural brain research. 2015; 292: 521–527. pmid:26188180
  24. 24. Halasz J, Zelena D, Toth M, Tulogdi A, Mikics E, Haller J. Substance P neurotransmission and violent aggression: The role of tachykinin NK 1 receptors in the hypothalamic attack area. European journal of pharmacology. 2009; 611(1): 35–43.
  25. 25. Asahina K, Watanabe K, Duistermars BJ, Hoopfer E, González CR, Eyjólfsdóttir EA, et al. Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in Drosophila. Cell. 2014; 156(1): 221–235.
  26. 26. Dryden S, Frankish HM, Wang Q, Pickavance L, Williams G. The serotonergic agent fluoxetine reduces neuropeptide Y levels and neuropeptide Y secretion in the hypothalamus of lean and obese rats. Neuroscience. 1996; 72(2): 557–566. pmid:8737424
  27. 27. Dryden S, Frankish HM, Wang Q, Williams G. Increased feeding and neuropeptide Y (NPY) but not NPY mRNA levels in the hypothalamus of the rat following central administration of the serotonin synthesis inhibitor p-chlorophenylalanine. Brain Res. 1996; 724(2): 232–237. pmid:8828573
  28. 28. Smialowska M, Bajkowska M, Heilig M, Obuchowicz E, Turchan J, Maj M et al. Pharmacological studies on the monoaminergic influence on the synthesis and expression of neuropeptide Y and corticotropin releasing factor in rat brain amygdala. Neuropeptides. 2001; 35(2): 82–91. pmid:11384203
  29. 29. Glass JD, DiNardo LA, Ehlen JC. Dorsal raphe nuclear stimulation of SCN serotonin release and circadian phase-resetting. Brain Res. 2000; 859(2): 224–232. pmid:10719068
  30. 30. Maejima T, Masseck OA, Mark MD, Herlitze S. Modulation of firing and synaptic transmission of serotonergic neurons by intrinsic G protein-coupled receptors and ion channels. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2013; 7: 40. pmid:23734105
  31. 31. Osborne NN, Cuello AC, Dockray GJ. Substance P and cholecystokinin-like peptides in Helix neurons and cholecystokinin and serotonin in a giant neuron. Science. 1982; 216(4544): 409–411. pmid:6176023
  32. 32. Chamberlain SC, Pepper J, Battelle BA, Wyse GA, Lewandowski TJ. Immunoreactivity in Limulus. II. Studies of serotoninlike immunoreactivity, endogenous serotonin, and serotonin synthesis in the brain and lateral eye. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1986; 251(3): 363–375. pmid:2429996
  33. 33. Langworthy K, Helluy S, Benton J, Beltz B. Amines and peptides in the brain of the American lobster: immunocytochemical localization patterns and implications for brain function. Cell and tissue research. 1997; 288(1): 191–206. pmid:9042786
  34. 34. Ignell R. Monoamines and neuropeptides in antennal lobe interneurons of the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria: an immunocytochemical study. Cell and Tissue Research. 2001; 306(1): 143–156. pmid:11683175
  35. 35. Boyer C, Maubert E, Charnay Y, Chichery R. Distribution of neurokinin A-like and serotonin immunoreactivities within the vertical lobe complex in Sepia officinalis. Brain Res. 2007; 1133: 53–66. pmid:17184745
  36. 36. Boyan G, Williams L, Herbert Z. Multipotent neuroblasts generate a biochemical neuroarchitecture in the central complex of the grasshopper Schistocerca gregaria. Cell and tissue research. 2010; 340(1): 13–28. pmid:20151154
  37. 37. Herbert Z, Rauser S, Williams L, Kapan N, Güntner M, Walch A et. al. Developmental expression of neuromodulators in the central complex of the grasshopper Schistocerca gregaria. Journal of Morphology. 2010; 271(12): 509–1526.
  38. 38. Cruz-Bermúdez ND, Marder E. Multiple modulators act on the cardiac ganglion of the crab, Cancer borealis. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2007; 210(16): 2873–2884.
  39. 39. Rehm KJ, Deeg KE, Marder E. Developmental Regulation of Neuromodulator Function in the Stomatogastric Ganglion of the Lobster, Homarus americanus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008; 28(39): 9828–9839. pmid:18815267
  40. 40. Manoli DS, Fan P, Fraser EJ, Shah NM. Neural control of sexually dimorphic behaviors. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013; 23(3): 330–338. pmid:23680385
  41. 41. Al-khairulla H, Warburton D, Knell RJ. Do the eyestalks of female diopsid flies have a function in intrasexual aggressive encounters? Journal of Insect Behavior. 2003; 16(5): 679–686.
  42. 42. Bath E, Wigby S, Vincent C, Tobias JA, Seddon N. Condition, not eyespan, predicts contest outcome in female stalk‐eyed flies, Teleopsis dalmanni. Ecology and Evolution. 2015: 5(9): 1826–1836. pmid:26140199
  43. 43. Lihoreau M, Brepson L, Rivault C. The weight of the clan: even in insects, social isolation can induce a behavioral syndrome. Behav Processes. 2009; 82: 81–84. pmid:19615616
  44. 44. Burkhardt D, de la Motte I. How stalk-eyed flies eye stalk-eyed flies: observations and measurements of the eye of Cyrtodiopsis whitei (Diopsidae, Diptera). J Comp Physiol. 1983. 151: 407–421.
  45. 45. Wilkinson G.S. Artificial sexual selection alters allometry in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni (Diptera: Diopsidae). Genet Res, Camb. 1993; 62: 213–222.
  46. 46. Egge AR, Brandt Y, Swallow JG. Sequential analysis of aggressive interactions in the stalk-eyed fly Teleopsis dalmanni. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2011; 65(2): 369–379.
  47. 47. Schmittgen TD, Livak KJ. Analyzing real-time PCR data by the comparative CT method. Nature Protocols. 2008; 3(6): 1101–1108. pmid:18546601
  48. 48. Bradford MM. A rapid and sensitive method for the quantitation of microgram quantities of protein utilizing the principle of protein-dye binding. Analytical Biochemistry. 1976; 72: 248–54. pmid:942051
  49. 49. Twenge JM, Baumeister RF, Tice DM, Stucke TS. If you can't join them, beat them: effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001; 81(6): 1058. pmid:11761307
  50. 50. Wongwitdecha N, Marsden CA. Social isolation increases aggressive behaviour and alters the effects of diazepam in the rat social interaction test. Behavioural brain research. 1996; 75(1): 27–32.
  51. 51. Alexander RD. Aggressiveness, territoriality, and sexual behavior in field crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Behaviour. 1961; 17: 130–223.
  52. 52. Blenau W, Baumann A. Molecular and pharmacological properties of insect biogenic amine receptors: lessons from Drosophila melanogaster and Apis mellifera. Archives of Insect Biochemistry and Physiology. 2001; 48(1): 13–38. pmid:11519073
  53. 53. Martin CA, Krantz DE. Drosophila melanogaster as a genetic model system to study neurotransmitter transporters. Neurochemistry International. 2014; 73: 71–88. pmid:24704795
  54. 54. Vleugels R, Lenaerts C, Baumann A, Vanden Broeck J, Verlinden H. Pharmacological Characterization of a 5-HT1-Type Serotonin Receptor in the Red Flour Beetle, Tribolium castaneum. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8(5): e65052. pmid:23741451
  55. 55. Baker KG, Halliday GM, Hornung JP, Geffen LB, Cotton RGH. Distribution, morphology and number of monoamine-synthesizing and substance P-containing neurons in the human dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroscience. 1991; 42(3): 757–775. pmid:1720227
  56. 56. Stevenson PA, Rillich J. Isolation associated aggression–a consequence of recovery from defeat in a territorial animal. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8(9): p.e74965. pmid:24040368
  57. 57. Ueda A, Wu CF. Effects of social isolation on neuromuscular excitability and aggressive behaviors in Drosophila: altered responses by Hk and gsts1, two mutations implicated in redox regulation. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2009; 23(4): 378–394. pmid:19863269
  58. 58. Katsouni E, Sakkas P, Zarros A, Skandali N, Liapi C. The involvement of substance P in the induction of aggressive behavior. Peptides. 2009; 30(8): 1586–1591. pmid:19442694
  59. 59. Grignaschi G, Sironi F, Samanin R. Stimulation of 5-HT2A receptors in the paraventricular hypothalamus attenuates neuropeptide Y-induced hyperphagia through activation of corticotropin releasing factor. Brain Res. 1996; 708(1–2): 173–176. pmid:8720874
  60. 60. Aoki M, Watanabe Y, Yoshimoto K, Tsujimura A, Yamamoto T, Kanamura N, et. al. Involvement of serotonin 2C receptor RNA editing in accumbal neuropeptide Y expression and behavioural despair. Eur J Neurosci. 2016; 43(9): 1219–1228. pmid:26950265
  61. 61. Brown GL, Goodwin FK, Ballenger JC, Goyer PF, Major LF. Aggression in humans correlates with cerebrospinal fluid amine metabolites. Psychiatry Res. 1979; 1: 131–139. pmid:95232
  62. 62. Linnoila M, Virkkunen M, Scheinin M, Nuutila A, Rimon R, Goodwin FK. Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Sci. 1983; 33: 2609–2614. pmid:6198573
  63. 63. Mann JJ. Violence and aggression. In Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (eds), Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. New York, Raven Press, pp 1919–1928. 1995.
  64. 64. Mann JJ. Role of the serotonergic system in the pathogenesis of major depression and suicidal behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999; 21: 99–105.
  65. 65. Ferrari PF, Palanza P, Parmigiani S, de Almeida RM, Miczek KA. Serotonin and aggressive behavior in rodents and nonhuman primates: predispositions and plasticity. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2005; 526(1): 259–273.
  66. 66. Summers CH, Winberg S. Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2006; 209: 4581–4589. pmid:17114393
  67. 67. Livingstone MS, Harris-Warrick RM, Kravitz EA. Serotonin and octopamine produce opposite postures in lobsters. Science. 1980; 208(4439): 76–79. pmid:17731572
  68. 68. Edwards D.H, Kravitz E.A. Serotonin, social status and aggression. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1997; 7(6): 812–819. pmid:9464985
  69. 69. Alekseyenko OV, Chan YB, de la Paz Fernandez M, Bülow T, Pankratz MJ, Kravitz EA. Single serotonergic neurons that modulate aggression in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2014; 24(22): 2700–2707. pmid:25447998