Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Can an experimental white noise task assess psychosis vulnerability in adult healthy controls?

  • Maider Gonzalez de Artaza ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    mgonlav@gmail.com (MGA); ana.catalanalcantara@osakidetza.eus (AC)

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, University of the Basque Country, Leioa, Basque Country, Spain

  • Ana Catalan ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    mgonlav@gmail.com (MGA); ana.catalanalcantara@osakidetza.eus (AC)

    Affiliations Department of Neuroscience, University of the Basque Country, Leioa, Basque Country, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain

  • Virxinia Angosto,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain

  • Cristina Valverde,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain

  • Amaia Bilbao,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Software, Supervision

    Affiliation Research Unit, Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain

  • Jim van Os,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, South Mental Health Research and Teaching Network, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands, King´s College London, King´s Health Partners, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom

  • Miguel Angel Gonzalez-Torres

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Neuroscience, University of the Basque Country, Leioa, Basque Country, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain

Abstract

Background

This is an extension of a paper published earlier. We investigated the association between the tendency to detect speech illusion in random noise and levels of positive schizotypy in a sample of 185 adult healthy controls.

Materials and methods

Subclinical positive, negative and depressive symptoms were assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE); positive and negative schizotypy was assessed with the Structured Interview for Schizotypy-Revised (SIS-R).

Results

Speech illusions were associated with positive schizotypy (OR: 4.139, 95% CI: 1.074–15.938; p = 0.039) but not with negative schizotypy (OR: 1.151, 95% CI: 0.183–7.244; p = 0.881). However, the association of positive schizotypy with speech illusions was no longer significant after adjusting for age, sex and WAIS-III (OR: 2.577, 95% CI: 0.620–10.700; p = 0.192). Speech illusions were not associated with self-reported CAPE measures.

Conclusions

The association between schizotypy and the tendency to assign meaning in random noise in healthy controls may be mediated by cognitive ability and not constitute an independent trait.

Introduction

The presence of psychotic features such as hallucinations, delusions or disorganized thinking is common in a wide range of mental disorders [1].

The prevalence of psychotic experiences in the population is difficult to assess given a range of methodological issues. However, numerous studies in recent years have emerged suggesting that psychotic experiences are more common in the general population than it was thought [2,3,4].

Some individuals without psychiatric history may manifest an attenuated form of hallucinations, of which only a minority will develop a psychotic or other mental disorder over time [5]. Such individuals may be relatives of patients with schizophrenia or individuals with psychometric schizotypy, or participants with physiological, neurological and cognitive characteristics similar to hallucinations found in patients with schizophrenia [6].

Psychotic experiences including auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH) may be phenomenologically and temporally continuous across different levels of clinical severity, ranging from subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population to full-blown psychotic disorder [7,8].

Contemporary models of psychosis suggest that an increase in mesolimbic dopaminergic neurotransmission occurs during the prodromal phase of psychosis, not corresponding to normal learning mechanisms [9]. Dysregulation of dopamine transmission, associated with alterations in top-down processing, causes neutral or irrelevant stimuli—associated with both external and internal representations—to be interpreted incorrectly. The tendency to assign altered meaning or emotional value to a neutral or irrelevant stimulus (aberrant salience) drives the individual to develop a cognitive scheme that alters its ability to process the experience and the surrounding environment appropriately [10,11].

Evidence suggests that alterations in salience attribution mediate the continuum of experiences of subclinical and attenuated psychoses to the sustained expression of psychotic disorder [4].

In order to explore the aberrant salience hypothesis in adults with psychotic disorders, their siblings and general population, a tool was developed to induce speech illusions, the "White Noise Task" (WN). It was first introduced in a study in 2011 [12]. Top-down processing was analyzed by detecting individuals who experienced speech illusions caused by this task and its association with variables of vulnerability to psychosis. The study found that the tendency to detect speech illusions was more frequent in the group of patients with psychotic disorder followed by the group at elevated familial risk. It also revealed that speech illusions were associated in healthy controls with high levels of positive schizotypy.

An attempt at replication was published by Catalan and colleagues, showing replication of the finding that patients had higher rates of WN than controls [13]. However, no association was found between WN and schizotypy in the control group.

In another study, speech illusions were examined in relation to psychotic phenomena in large general population sample of pre-adolescents, using an abbreviated version of the WN task. In this sample, speech illusions were associated with hallucinatory experiences [14].

Here, we present a novel analysis of the expanded non-clinical sample of the adult population presented in the earlier study by Catalan and colleagues [13]. Thus, the original sample was n = 150; the sample in the current analysis was n = 185. The aim was to determine the rate of speech illusions with the WN task and to analyse the association between speech illusions and schizotypal traits. Given the possibility that any association between WN and indicators of psychosis risk may be mediated by known risk cognitive factors, in particular cognitive ability, analyses were adjusted for IQ.

Method

Procedure and sample

In order to recruit a representative general population sample to assess population reference values for white noise speech illusion, a control reference group of 185 participants between 17 and 65 years old was selected in Bilbao, Spain, through public advertisement during the period from July 2012 to April 2015.

Inclusion criteria were sufficient knowledge of the Spanish language, intelligence quotient IQ> = 70 according to the Weschsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) and no first-degree relatives with a psychotic disorder, as reported by the participant.

Ethical issues

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Clinical Research of Basurto University Hospital. Participants were given verbal and written detailed explanation about the study and its procedures. Before the start of the first assessment, written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Confidentiality of data was maintained using a unique research ID for each respondent, enabling analysis of individual data without the use of names or other identifiers.

Instruments

All the interviews and assessments were carried out at Basurto University Hospital by psychologists and psychiatrists, trained in the use of these specific instruments.

White noise task [12].

Participants wore earphones and were presented 1 of 3 different types of stimuli: (1) white noise only, (2) white noise + clearly audible neutral speech and (3) white noise + barely audible neutral speech. Stimuli 2 and 3 were not separate conditions; the intermixing of white noise stimuli with audible speech was presented in order to create a higher level of expectancy, thus occasioning levels of top-down processing. Participants were presented 25 fragments of each in random order and asked to respond to each by pressing 1 of 5 buttons hereafter referred to as: 1: positive speech illusion (endorsed hearing positive voice), 2: negative speech illusion (endorsed hearing negative voice), 3: neutral speech illusion (endorsed hearing neutral voice), 4: no speech heard and 5: uncertain; this latter option was included in order to make the ratings of 1–3 more conservative. The recordings were delivered using stimulations software E-prime 1.1 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania) and stimuli were reproduced in random order. The length of the task was approximately 15 min.

The rate of hearing a voice in the white noise-only condition (25 trials) was the variable of interest in the analyses. As white noise speech illusion scores for positive, negative and neutral voices were highly skewed, the 3 outcomes were analysed as dichotomous variables. A variable “any speech illusion” was constructed denoting the presence of at least two instances of any positive, negative or neutral voice perceived in white noise (speech illusion present versus not present), in agreement with previous work [13,14]. In order to examine whether the white noise task was sensitive particularly to affectively salient speech illusions rather than neutral speech illusions, a composite variable was constructed reflecting any positive or negative speech illusions.

Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III [15].

General cognitive abilities were assessed for an indication of intellectual functioning (IQ) using the short form of the WAIS-III that includes Information, Block Design, Digit Symbol and Arithmetic subtests.

Structured Interview for Schizotypy-Revised (SIS-R) [16].

The SIS-R is a structured interview used to determine a broad range of schizotypal symptoms and signs: the positive, negative and disorganization dimensions of the subclinical psychosis phenotype. Items can be scored on a 4-point scale from absent (score 0) to severe (score 3). Positive schizotypy covers the symptoms referential thinking (2 items), as well as magical ideation, illusions, psychotic symptoms and suspiciousness (total of 6 items). Negative schizotypy contains the signs of social isolation, introversion, restricted affect and poverty of speech (4 items). Mean schizotypy scores for these dimensions were calculated, resulting in a positive schizotypy and negative schizotypy score.

Community assessment of psychotic experiences (CAPE) [17].

This self-report questionnaire rates attenuated affective and non-affective psychotic experiences. The CAPE measures, on a dimensional scale, frequency of, as well as distress associated with, these subclinical psychotic experiences. The frequency score is measured on a 4-point scale from: 1 = never to 4 = nearly always. The degree of distress associated with the subclinical psychotic experience is also measured. The CAPE includes dimensions of positive (20 items), negative (14 items) and depressive (8 items) symptoms associated with the subclinical psychosis phenotype in the general population on a 4-point scale with labels ranging from1 = not distressed to 4 = nearly always. For the current analyses, mean scores of frequency of positive and negative symptoms were used.

Analyses

To study the association between SIS-R scales and CAPE scales on the one hand, and white noise on the other, logistic regression was used, with “any speech illusion” as dependent variable and SIS-R or CAPE scales as independent variables. Logistic regression models were also performed adjusting for age, sex and IQ. Associations derived from logistic models were expressed as odds ratio (OR) and their 95% confidence interval (CI).

Associations were considered significant at p<0.05, double sided.

Statistical analyses were performed using the STATA software programme, version 13 [18].

Results

Sample

Controls were 55.14% males and more than half were single (58.38%). Mean age of controls was 31.81 years old (SD = 11.56). The majority was from middle social class (76.76%) and had had full-time education (38.92%). The majority (92.97%) did not live alone, living either with their parents or with their own family (wife/husband and children). The majority (77.84%) had an occupation in the form of either a job (52.43%) or studies (25.41%). Mean WAIS-IQ was 109.01 (SD = 14.93) (Table 1).

Clinical variables were as follows: mean positive schizotypy score was 0.26 (SD = 0.28) and mean negative schizotypy 0.11 (SD = 0.23); CAPE positive dimension 0.22 (0.14) and negative dimension 0.50 (SD = 0.30). 12.97% of controls perceived at least two instances of any positive, negative or neutral voice in white noise (Table 2).

Schizotypy and speech illusions

Any speech illusion was associated with positive schizotypy (OR: 4.139, 95% CI: 1.074–15.938; p = 0.039) but not with negative schizotypy (OR: 1.151, 95% CI: 0.183–7.244; p = 0.881). However, the association of positive schizotypy with speech illusions disappeared after adjustment for age, sex and WAIS-III (OR: 2.577, 95% CI: 0.620–10.700; p = 0.192). Speech illusions were not associated with the CAPE positive (OR: 7.221, 95% CI: 0.471–110.497; p = 0.155) and negative scales (OR: 1.250, 95% CI: 0.294–5.315; p = 0.762) (Table 3).

Discussion

The degree to which WN reflects vulnerability for expression of psychosis in healthy participants, possible reflecting alterations in processing top-down or aberrant salience in healthy population, remains uncertain. No associations were apparent with self-reported measures of psychotic experiences. While WN was associated with interview-based measures of positive schizotypy, this appeared to be mediated to a large extent by other variables including cognitive ability.

In accordance with the current findings, a recent research [19] speculates that neurocognitive mechanism underlying perceptual abnormalities might differ between psychotic patients and the non clinical population, based on the findings showing no association between white noise speech illusion and subtle psychosis expression in a large general population adolescent and young adult twin cohort (n = 704).

The strengths of our study were, first, the use of both the SIS-R and CAPE scales, designed to measure the prevalence of positive experiences in the general population. Second, we included adequate control for cognitive ability. Lastly, the use of a representative sample in terms of age and education, reducing the risk of bias. A potential weakness is that an even larger sample may be required to detect the small association that may remain after adjustment for confounders. Similarly, sensitivity of the analyses may be enhanced if preferentially young people are included, given the high prevalence of psychotic experience around adolescents. This latter factor may explain the finding of a positive association between WN and hallucinatory experiences in an earlier study [14].

In conclusion, although there was an apparent association between the tendency to detect speech illusions in random noise and interview-based positive schizotypal traits, the association appeared to be mediated to a large extent by cognitive ability. Research in larger samples and/or uniformly young people may shed more light on this issue.

Acknowledgments

We would like to warmly thank all the volunteers who participated to the study.

References

  1. 1. Lahera G, Freund N, Sáiz-Ruiz J. Salience and dysregulation of the dopaminergic system. Rev Psiquiatr y Salud Ment (English Ed [Internet]. 2012;6(1):45–51. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rpsmen.2012.05.003
  2. 2. Stip E, Letourneau G. Normality and Pathology. La Rev Can Psychiatr. 2009;54(3).
  3. 3. Sommer IEC, Daalman K, Rietkerk T, Diederen KM, Bakker S, Wijkstra J, et al. Healthy individuals with auditory verbal hallucinations; who are they? Psychiatric assessments of a selected sample of 103 subjects. Schizophr Bull [Internet]. 2010 May 1 [cited 2016 Apr 23];36(3):633–41. Available from: http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/content/36/3/633.full pmid:18849293
  4. 4. Van Os J, Reininghaus U. Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry. 2016;15(2):118–24. pmid:27265696
  5. 5. Linscott RJ, van Os J, Aleman A, Nieuwenstein MR, Böcker KBE, De Haan EHF, et al. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med [Internet]. Cambridge University Press; 2013 Jun [cited 2016 Oct 21];43(6):1133–49. Available from: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0033291712001626 pmid:22850401
  6. 6. Larøi F, DeFruyt F, van Os J, Aleman A, Van der Linden M. Associations between hallucinations and personality structure in a non-clinical sample: Comparison between young and elderly samples. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;39(1):189–200.
  7. 7. van Os J. Is there a continuum of psychotic experiences in the general population? Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2003;12(4):242–52. pmid:14968483
  8. 8. Daalman K, Boks MPM, Diederen KMJ, de Weijer AD, Blom JD, Kahn RS, et al. The same or different? A phenomenological comparison of auditory verbal hallucinations in healthy and psychotic individuals. J Clin Psychiatry [Internet]. 2011 Mar [cited 2016 Apr 23];72(3):320–5. Available from: http://www.psychiatrist.com/_layouts/PPP.Psych.Controls/ArticleViewer.ashx?ArticleURL=/JCP/article/Pages/2011/v72n03/v72n0307.aspx pmid:21450152
  9. 9. Winton-Brown TT, Fusar-Poli P, Ungless MA, Howes OD, Carlsson A, al. et, et al. Dopaminergic basis of salience dysregulation in psychosis. Trends Neurosci [Internet]. Elsevier; 2014 Feb [cited 2016 Oct 21];37(2):85–94. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24388426 pmid:24388426
  10. 10. Kapur S. Psychosis as a State of Aberrant Salience: A Framework Linking Biology, Phenomenology, and Pharmacology in Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry [Internet]. 2003 Jan [cited 2016 Oct 26];160(1):13–23. Available from: http://psychiatryonline.org/doi/abs/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13 pmid:12505794
  11. 11. Reininghaus U, Kempton MJ, Valmaggia L, Craig TKJ, Garety P, Onyejiaka A, et al. Stress sensitivity, aberrant salience, and threat anticipation in early psychosis: An experience sampling study. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(3):712–22. pmid:26834027
  12. 12. Galdos M, Simons C, Fernandez-Rivas A, Wichers M, Peralta C, Lataster T, et al. Affectively salient meaning in random noise: a task sensitive to psychosis liability. Schizophr Bull [Internet]. 2011 Nov [cited 2016 Feb 24];37(6):1179–86. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3196950&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:20360211
  13. 13. Catalan A, Simons CJP, Bustamante S, Drukker M, Madrazo A, de Artaza MG, et al. Novel evidence that attributing affectively salient signal to random noise is associated with psychosis. PLoS One [Internet]. 2014 Jan [cited 2016 Feb 23];9(7):e102520. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=4097069&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:25020079
  14. 14. Rimvall MK, Clemmensen L, Munkholm A, Rask CU, Larsen JT, Skovgaard AM, et al. Introducing the White Noise Task in Childhood: Associations Between Speech Illusions and Psychosis Vulnerability. Psychol Med Rev. 2016;1–10.
  15. 15. Blyler CR, Gold JM, Iannone VN, Buchanan RW. Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res [Internet]. 2000 Dec 15 [cited 2016 Feb 4];46(2–3):209–15. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11120433 pmid:11120433
  16. 16. Vollema MG, Ormel J. The reliability of the structured interview for schizotypy-revised. Schizophr Bull [Internet]. 2000 Jan [cited 2016 Feb 4];26(3):619–29. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10993402 pmid:10993402
  17. 17. Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand [Internet]. 2006 Jul [cited 2016 Feb 4];114(1):55–61. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16774662 pmid:16774662
  18. 18. StataCorp. 2013. Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.
  19. 19. Pries L-K, Guloksuz S, Menne-Lothmann C, Decoster J, van Winkel R, Collip D, et al. White noise speech illusion and psychosis expression: An experimental investigation of psychosis liability. Takei N, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. Public Library of Science; 2017 Aug 23 [cited 2017 Dec 16];12(8):e0183695. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183695 pmid:28832672