Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Insectivorous birds eavesdrop on the pheromones of their prey

  • Irene Saavedra ,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    irene.saavedra.garces@gmail.com

    Affiliation Departamento de Ecología Evolutiva, Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales (CSIC), C/ José Gutiérrez Abascal, Madrid, Spain

  • Luisa Amo

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Ecología Evolutiva, Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales (CSIC), C/ José Gutiérrez Abascal, Madrid, Spain

Abstract

Chemical cues play a fundamental role in mate attraction and mate choice. Lepidopteran females, such as the winter moth (Operophtera brumata), emit pheromones to attract males in the reproductive period. However, these chemical cues could also be eavesdropped by predators. To our knowledge, no studies have examined whether birds can detect pheromones of their prey. O. brumata adults are part of the winter diet of some insectivorous tit species, such as the great tit (Parus major) and blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus). We performed a field experiment aimed to disentangle whether insectivorous birds can exploit the pheromones emitted by their prey for prey location. We placed artificial larvae and a dispenser on branches of Pyrenean oak trees (Quercus pyrenaica). In half of the trees we placed an O. brumata pheromone dispenser and in the other half we placed a control dispenser. We measured the predation rate of birds on artificial larvae. Our results show that more trees had larvae with signs of avian predation when they contained an O. brumata pheromone than when they contained a control dispenser. Furthermore, the proportion of artificial larvae with signs of avian predation was greater in trees that contained the pheromone than in control trees. Our results indicate that insectivorous birds can exploit the pheromones emitted by moth females to attract males, as a method of prey detection. These results highlight the potential use of insectivorous birds in the biological control of insect pests.

Introduction

Chemical communication is probably the most ancient and widespread form of communication [1, 2] and plays an important role in sexual selection [3, 4]. The chemical compounds emitted by animals and used in mate attraction and mate choice are known as pheromones [4]. In many cases, the chemical signals involved in mate choice may allow potential partners to evaluate an individual´s quality. Theoretical models have predicted that signals can only be evolutionarily stable if they are condition-dependent, or costly to the signaler, and if the cost is correlated with the signaler's quality [57]. Therefore, individuals can not afford to cheat, i.e., to signal at too high a level [8], and conspecifics can rely on the information provided by these honest signals.

Chemical signals can provide information about the individual quality (i.e., body condition, reproductive status, age, parasite load, health condition) [4]. However, chemical signals, as well as other signals, are not only costly to produce but they may imply survival costs. Signals are not only detected by potential partners but can also be eavesdropped by predators and parasites. Therefore, the emission of chemical signals can greatly increase the risk of predation or parasitism [9, 10]. Examples of predators that eavesdrop the chemical cues involved in mate attraction or signaling have been found in different taxa, from invertebrates to vertebrates such as amphibians, reptiles and mammals [2]. For example, there are numerous studies of natural predators that are able to detect the chemical cues of their bark beetle prey [11]. Smooth snakes (Coronella austriaca) can also detect the chemical cues of their lizard prey [12]. Mammalian predators often detect their prey by intercepting intraspecific reproductive cues, such as least weasels (Mustela nivalis) [1315], cats (Felis catus) and foxes (Vulpes vulpes) [16]. In the case of predatory birds, previous evidence suggests that some predatory species of raptors and shrikes could be visually attracted to the UV light reflected by the urine and faeces marks of their small mammal prey [17]. As some studies indicate, this detection of prey may not depend entirely on UV vision, because birds do not prefer UV areas lacking scent marks [1820]. To our knowledge, there is no other evidence that birds use olfaction to eavesdrop the chemical signals emitted by their prey.

The lack of studies in this area is probably due to the fact that birds were considered almost anosmic in the past. However, an increasing number of studies have shown that birds can detect odors in several ecological contexts. For example, birds can use their sense of smell in intraspecific relationships [21]. The crimson rosella (Platycercus elegans) can discriminate between subspecies using olfaction [22]. Antarctic prions (Pachiptila desolata) can recognize the scent of their partners [23]. Passeriformes can discriminate the sex of conspecifics [24, 25], and Sphenisciformes, Procellariiformes and Passeriformes use olfaction for kin recognition [26, 27, 28]. Moreover, house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) seem to be able to evaluate the quality of conspecifics using olfaction [29]. In interspecific contexts, blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) and European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) are known to use olfaction for detecting aromatic plants [3032]. Columbiformes and Procellariiformes use olfaction for orientation and navigation [33, 34]. For example, British storm-petrel (Hydrobates pelagicus) and blue petrels (Halobaena caerulea), can find their own burrows using olfaction [3536]. The ability to detect the chemical cues of predators and use them to ascertain predators has been demonstrated in Passeriformes [3739], Galliformes [40] and Anseriformes [41].

Previous evidence suggests that birds are able to perceive odors in the process of foraging. Vultures, such as turkey vultures (Cathartes aura) [42] and greater yellow-headed vultures (C. melambrotus) [43], appear to use olfaction to locate carcasses. The role of olfaction in foraging has also been suggested in honey-guides (family Indicatoridae) [44] and honey buzzards [45]. In addition, some species of parrots can find their food using olfaction. For example, kakapo (Strigops habroptilus), a flightless, nocturnal and vegetarian bird, identifies bins with food using olfaction [46]. Procellariiform seabirds use dimethyl sulphide (DMS) for foraging [47]. The DMS is produced when the zooplankton graze on the phytoplankton [47], thus signaling areas of high productivity in the oceans. By detecting this compound, Procelariiformes [36, 47] and Sphenisciformes[48, 49] can locate their prey. Insectivorous birds are also able to use olfaction to find their food. Kiwis (Apteryx australis) can use olfaction when foraging [50, 51]. Parids, such as great tits, can exploit the herbivore-induced volatiles that trees emit in response to lepidopteran caterpillar infestation for finding those caterpillars upon which they prey [52]. Attraction to trees infested with caterpillars has also been shown in different plant-insect-bird systems [5357].

Birds may not only use indirect cues to find their prey [47, 52, 56], but may be able to detect the chemical cues emitted by the prey itself. In many lepidopteran species, females release pheromones during the reproductive period in order to attract males [58, 59]. Birds could detect these pheromones and use them to locate their prey. In this way, they could maximize their foraging effort. However, to our knowledge, no study has examined whether insectivorous birds can use olfaction to detect the pheromones of adult lepidopteran. Therefore, the aim of this study was to analyze whether insectivorous birds can eavesdrop on the sex pheromones of lepidoptera females. We performed a field experiment to investigate whether insectivorous birds in the wild are attracted to the pheromones of one of their potential prey, O. brumata adults. The winter moth is considered a plague in many forests and orchards in Europe [60]. We measured whether the predation rates on artificial larvae located in Pyrenean oak trees containing an O. brumata pheromone dispenser differed from those containing a control dispenser. We expected that if birds can detect the pheromones of their prey and are attracted to them, the predation rate of artificial larvae by birds will be higher in the trees that contain a pheromone dispenser than in the trees that contain a control dispenser.

Materials and methods

Study area and species

The experimental study was carried out between May and June 2016 in a Pyrenean oak (Quercus pyrenaica) forest included in a Site of Community Interest (SCI), located in Sierra de Fonfría, in Teruel province, Spain (40°59′N, 1°05′W). In this forest, a population of insectivorous birds breeding in 100 wooden nest-boxes was established in 2011. Nest-boxes were occupied mainly by breeding pairs of blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) (around 45 pairs), and some pairs of great tits (Parus major) (around 10 pairs). Other insectivorous bird species were observed in the study area at lower densities, including common blackbird (Turdus merula), Eurasian blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla), Sardinian warbler (S. melanocephala) and common nightingale (Luscinia megarhynchos). Tits feed mainly on caterpillars, such as the O. brumata, during the breeding period [61, 62]. However, during the winter, when no caterpillars are available, parids like great tits and blue tits prey upon O. brumata adults [61, 63]. Thus, O. brumata adults constitute an important part of the diet when they are available in winter. Others species included in the winter diet of tits belong to the Hemiptera, Lepidoptera, Coleoptera and Hymenoptera orders [61]. O. brumata adults are present in the study area from November to February [64]. In this species only females produce pheromones during the reproductive period to attract males [65, 66]. In 1982, the pheromone of O. brumata was identified as 1,Z3,Z6,Z9-nonadecatetraene [65, 66]. A synthetic pheromone can be obtained from commercial supplier (ControlBio® from OPENNATUR, S.L.). The pheromone dispensers attached to a trap are effectively used in insect pest control in order to reduce male quantities. The pheromone dispensers contain 0.5 mg of 1,Z3,Z6,Z9-nonadecatetraene (Pherobank, B.V.). The emission lasts 40 days, and thus the emission rate is approximately 9 ng/min (Pherobank, B.V.). We performed the study outside of the reproductive period of this species to ensure that adult moths were absent, and therefore, bird attraction to the O. brumata pheromone could be attributed to the pheromone and not to the presence of males.

Experimental design and procedure

O. brumata adults can only be found in winter. Nevertheless, one week before the experiment, we placed 5 moth traps with commercial O. brumata pheromone for one week in different locations in the study area to ensure there were no O. brumata adults or other insect species that could have been attracted by the same pheromone (e.g., predators or parasitoids). We found no O. brumata adults or other arthropods inside the traps. During the 27 days of the experiment, we placed one dispenser and ten artificial larvae on branches of 32 Pyrenean oak trees. The branches were approximately 1.5 m long with no evident signs of herbivory. The dispenser and artificial larvae were placed at similar average heights in the trees (approx. 1.5 m high). Dispensers were fixed to the branch with a pin. Ten artificial larvae were placed in the surroundings of the dispenser, from 2 to 50 cm from the dispenser. Thus, dispensers were situated in the middle of ten artificial larvae. Pheromone and control dispensers were brown and opaque (approx. 20x10 mm). Pheromone dispensers were made with natural rubber (Fig 1). Control dispensers were made of brown plasticine similar to the color of the pheromone dispenser. There were no significant differences between the reflectance spectra of the two types of dispensers (p > 0.05; see Fig A in S1 Supporting Information).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Photograph of the pheromone dispenser and several plasticine caterpillars, one of them with beak marks, indicating a predation event by an insectivorous bird.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190415.g001

We selected trees that were located within 10 meters from a nest-box, and therefore, within the breeding territory of a blue tit or a great tit. Thirty-one of the 32 nest-boxes close to experimental trees were occupied by blue tits. The artificial larvae were made of light green plasticine (similar to the natural color of real O. brumata larvae, at least by human-visual perception). Neither the plasticine caterpillars nor the dispensers emitted UV light. The plasticine larvae were approximately the size of a large fifth instar O. brumata larva (length 25–30 mm, Ø 3–4 mm). The plasticine larvae were attached with cyanoacrylate adhesive glue on branches of 32 forest oak trees. Experimental trees were separated by at least 40 meters. The trees were alternatively assigned to one of the treatments: commercial O. brumata pheromone dispenser (n = 16), or plasticine dispenser, simulating the shape of the commercial pheromone dispenser (odorless control) (n = 16). Thus, treatments were spatially inter-mixed in the oak trees.

To study the attraction of the insectivorous birds to the O. brumata pheromone, we checked the number of larvae with predation marks by birds in the trees. Artificial caterpillar models have previously been used to estimate insectivorous bird attraction [54, 6772]. A predation event was assigned to a tree when the tree contained at least one larva damaged by birds. Larva models were considered damaged when they had triangle-shaped marks and deep cuts made by the beak of the birds and when a part of their body was taken by the birds, as described in Mäntylä and collaborators [54, 69]. From the following day onwards we checked the condition of these plasticine larvae every two days for the first ten days. After ten days, we checked them twice, once at day 20 and again at day 27 from the beginning of the experiment. Each model showing a predation mark was replaced with a new one at the same location during the visits. The treatments were in place for 27 days, a period of time for which the effectiveness of the commercial pheromone is guaranteed, as it can last up to 40 days (Pherobank, B.V.). At the end of the experiment, we removed all plasticine larvae and the commercial pheromones and controls. The experiment was conducted under a license issued by the Instituto Aragonés de Gestión Ambiental (INAGA/500201/24/2015/11696).

Statistical analyses

We modeled the probability that at least one predation event occurs in a tree in relation to the treatment (pheromone vs control) with a generalized linear model (GLM) fit by the Laplace approximation with binomial errors and a logit link function. We also analyzed the probability that the proportion of damaged larvae per tree differed in relation to the treatment with a generalized linear model (GLM) fit by the Bernoulli distribution with binomial errors and a logit link function. We included the day of observation in the initial models but, as it was not significant (see S2 Supporting Information), it was removed from the final models. Data analyses were performed with the statistical program R 2.15.1 “stats” package [73].

Results

The number of trees that had at least one caterpillar with signs of avian predation differed between treatments (GLM: Z = 2.40, P = 0.02, Fig 2). Ten out of the 16 trees containing a pheromone dispenser had at least one avian predation event (i.e., at least one artificial caterpillar had signs of avian predation, Fig 1). In contrast, a predation event was observed in only 3 out of the 16 control trees. The proportion of larvae damaged by the birds differed between treatments (GLM: Z = -3.72, P = 0.0002; see Table A in S3 Supporting Information), being significantly higher in trees that contained a pheromone (Mean ± SE = 3.04% ± 1.48%) than in control trees (Mean ± SE = 0.71% ± 0.54%).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Number of trees that had at least one artificial larva with marks of avian predation when the tree contained an Operophtera brumata pheromone dispenser and when the tree contained a control dispenser.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190415.g002

Discussion

Our results show for the first time that insectivorous birds can exploit sex pheromones for prey location [74]. A greater number of trees were visited by birds (i.e., they had at least one artificial caterpillar with signs of avian predation) when they contained an O. brumata pheromone dispenser compared to a control dispenser (Fig 2). Additionally, a greater proportion of artificial larvae were predated when the tree in which they were located contained an O. brumata pheromone dispenser than when it contained a control dispenser.

We performed the study during the spring, when there are no adults of O. brumata. The adults of this species emerge in November and can be observed in the field until February [64]. Therefore, when we placed the pheromones in the field, the attraction of males to this pheromone was not possible. Synthetic pheromones can be less specific that the natural female pheromones. Thus, we investigated the presence of adults in the study area to examine whether the pheromone attracts other insects. We placed five traps containing the pheromone a week before the beginning of the experiment and no moths or other arthropods were collected. Other arthropod species were never observed close to dispensers or artificial larvae in the study area. Moreover, we did not observe any damage to the caterpillars due to species other than birds. Therefore, the greater predation rate of artificial larvae does not appear to be due to the attraction of birds to the presence of O. brumata males or other arthropods close to the female pheromone.

Control and pheromone dispensers were made of different material, which may have induced differences in visual and odor cues available to birds between control and treated dispensers. It is, however, unlikely that visual cues account for the differences in predation rates because the color spectra of control and pheromone dispensers were not significantly different (see Figure A in S1 Supporting Information). Therefore, the lack of significant differences between dispensers in visual cues eliminates the possibility that birds were attracted to the dispenser's appearance. The artificial caterpillars and control dispensers were made with plasticine, and thus the volatiles emitted by plasticine would be present in both treatments. The similarity in the chemical composition between the caterpillars and the control dispensers would mean that bird attraction was not caused by differences in the volatiles emitted by the different materials of the two types of dispensers. Regardless, further experiments using the same dispensers in the control and pheromone treatments are needed to completely exclude the possibility of an artifact. The emission rate of the pheromone dispenser was around 9 ng/min. This emission rate is higher than that produced by a single female moth [75], and may be similar to that produced by 10 females. However, the concentration of this synthetic pheromone attracts male moths (L. Amo, personal observation), suggesting that the emission rate of the pheromone dispensers may be biologically relevant for male moths. Our results now show that vertebrate predators, such as insectivorous birds, are also attracted to the emission rate of this pheromone dispenser. However, birds could only detect female moths when the emission rate is 10-times higher than that of a single female. Thus, further studies will test whether birds can detect lower concentrations of the pheromone to disentangle whether birds can use the pheromone emissions to locate a single female moth or whether they are only attracted to female groups. Furthermore, female moths become active from sunset, when diurnal insectivorous birds such as blue tits decrease their foraging activity and search for roosting places. Therefore, additional studies are needed to elucidate whether birds use pheromones as a precise localization cue or to find good areas for foraging the subsequent day.

Previous studies have shown that the use of artificial caterpillars is a reliable measure of insectivorous bird attraction [e.g. 54, 57, 69, 72]. For example, Muiruri and collaborators observed several individuals of breeding bird species pecking the artificial caterpillars [76]. To prevent the same bird from visiting all the trees, we placed the caterpillars in trees that were separated by at least 40 meters. Blue tits normally feed within 20 meters from the nest-box in deciduous forests and up to 40 meters in mixed forests [77]. Additionally, results of another study show that 90% of foraging observations of blue and great tits were made within 45 m from the nest-box [78]. Therefore, it is unlikely that the same individuals visited more than one experimental tree. Furthermore, we expected that birds learned to recognize plasticine caterpillars as unpalatable, unprofitable prey. Thus, we did not expect birds to return to the same location numerous times.

The study area has had a blue and great tit population breeding in nest-boxes since 2011. Blue and great tits are the most abundant insectivorous birds in the area (more than 100 adults and their nestlings) [79]. The high abundance of these two species, as well as the fact that experimental trees were close to nest-boxes, suggest that these two species were likely responsible for the majority of the predation. Nonetheless, we cannot exclude the possibility that other insectivorous species or even omnivorous species that include moths in their diets and are present in the study area, may have been attracted to the pheromones.

Our results show that birds can exploit the pheromones emitted by O. brumata females. The attraction of birds to the pheromones of this species may help birds maximize their foraging effort. O. brumata females are wingless [80] and their brown coloration allows them to blend into the trunks of trees, such as the Pyrenean oak, which could make them cryptic and probably hampers visual detection by bird predators. Therefore, by using the chemical cues emitted by female moths, birds can enhance their probability of finding the camouflaged females, as well as the male moths that are attracted by the female pheromone. As a consequence, males may also suffer an increased risk of predation [10].

Bird predation on O. brumata adults during the winter may reduce the number of lepidopteran clutches and therefore, the number of caterpillars in spring. This can have important consequences for host trees in the subsequent spring. Previous studies using bird exclusion have shown a positive effect of birds on predation rates of arthropods in spring or summer [81]. However, the effect of avian predation on their prey population during the winter period has been less studied [60, 82, 83].

Insectivorous birds are predators of lepidopteran moths, eggs and caterpillars, such as the O. brumata [61, 84]. Thus, they may not only decrease the number of moth adults during the winter, but also caterpillar numbers during the spring. The nestling period of many insectivorous bird species coincides with the peak occurrence of most caterpillars, including the O. brumata larva. Thus, birds can greatly reduce the number of lepidopteran larvae feeding on trees [62]. Insectivorous birds, at least great tits, use olfaction to discriminate between trees infested with O. brumata caterpillars and uninfested trees [52], thanks to the herbivore-induced volatiles (HIPVs) that trees emit in response to herbivory [52]. The attraction of birds to caterpillar infested trees can decrease herbivore damage to trees [62, 85, 86], leading to increased growth and reduced mortality of the trees [8688].

The attraction of birds to the pheromones of moth females adds birds to the list of predators that are able to eavesdrop the chemical cues emitted by their prey for mate attraction. This new evidence indicates the costs of the emission of chemical signals for females [89] as well as the costs of responding to such chemicals for males [10]. These results indicate the potential use of insectivorous birds in controlling Lepidopteran numbers in forests and orchards. Traditional control of adult numbers is based on the use of pheromone traps to collect males and remove them from the population, decreasing access to males by females, and therefore decreasing fecundity of females [90]. However, birds prey upon both females and males and may be much more efficient than pheromone traps in decreasing the number of adults.

Supporting information

S1 Supporting Information.

Fig A. Reflectance spectra of control (blue line) and pheromone (brown line) dispensers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190415.s001

(PDF)

S2 Supporting Information. Statistical analysis of data including the day of observation in the initial models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190415.s002

(PDF)

S3 Supporting Information.

Table A. Number of damaged and undamaged larvae in the control and pheromone treatment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190415.s003

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We especially thank L.M. Carrascal for statistical advice and I. López Rull for spectral measurements. We also thank José Luis Viejo Montesinos for entomological information and the Oficina Comarcal Agroambiental—Comarca del Jiloca for their help with O. brumata phenology. I. Saavedra was supported by a FPI grant from Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness. L. Amo was supported by Ramón y Cajal programme.

References

  1. 1. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SK. Principles of animal communication. 2nd ed. Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates, Inc., Publishers Sunderland; 1998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.014
  2. 2. Wyatt TD. Pheromones and animal behaviour: Communication by smell and taste. Cambridge University Press; 2003. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.00738
  3. 3. Darwin C. The descent of man. London: John Murray; 1871. https://doi.org/10.5962/bhl.title.2092
  4. 4. Johansson BG, Jones TM. The role of chemical communication in mate choice. Biol Rev. 2007;82(2): 265–289. pmid:17437561
  5. 5. Zahavi A. Mate selection-A selection for a handicap. ‎J Theor Biol. 1975;53(1): 205–214. pmid:1195756
  6. 6. Grafen A. Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol. 1990;144(4): 517–546. pmid:2402153
  7. 7. Zahavi A, Zahavi A. The handicap principle: a missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 1997. https://doi.org/10.1642/0004-8038(2000)117[0269:R]2.0.CO;2
  8. 8. Krebs JR, Dawkins R. Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. 2nd ed. In: Krebs J.R. and Davies N.B. (eds) Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Blackwell, Oxford; 1983. pp. 380–402.
  9. 9. Zuk M, Kolluru GR. Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. Q Rev Biol. 1998;73(4): 415–438.
  10. 10. Hughes NK, Kelley JL, Banks PB. Dangerous liaisons: the predation risks of receiving social signals. Ecol Lett. 2012;15(11): 1326–1339. pmid:22925009
  11. 11. Wood DL. The role of pheromones, kairomones, and allomones in the host selection and colonization behavior of bark beetles. Annu Rev Entomol. 1982;27: 411–446.
  12. 12. Amo L, López P, Martín J. Chemosensory Recognition of Its Lizard Prey by the Ambush Smooth Snake, Coronella austriaca. J Herpetol. 2004;38(3): 451–454.
  13. 13. Cushing BS. A selective preference by least weasels for oestrous versus dioestrous urine of prairie deer mice. Anim Behav. 1984;32: 1263–1265.
  14. 14. Sundell J, Eccard JA, Tiilikainen R, Ylönen H. Predation rate, prey preference and predator switching: experiments on voles and weasels. OIKOS. 2003;101(3): 615–623.
  15. 15. Ylönen H., Sundell J, Tiilikainen R, Eccard JA, Horne T. Weasels’ (Mustela nivalis nivalis) preference for olfactory cues of the vole (Clethrionomys glareolus). Ecology. 2003;84(6): 1447–1452.
  16. 16. Hughes NK, Price CJ, Banks PB. Predators are attracted to the olfactory signals of prey. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(9): 1–4. pmid:20927352
  17. 17. Koivula M, Viitala J. Rough-legged buzzards use vole scent marks to assess hunting areas. Avian Biol. 1999;30(3): 329–332.
  18. 18. Viitala J, Korpimäki E, Palokangas P, Koivula M. Attraction of kestrels to vole scent marks visible in ultraviolet light. Nature. 1995;373:425–427.
  19. 19. Probst R, Palice M. UV reflecting vole scent marks attract a passerine, the great grey shrike Lanius excubitor. J Avian Biol. 2002;33(4): 437–440.
  20. 20. Zampiga E, Gaibani G, Csermely D, Frey H, Hoi H. Innate and learned aspects of vole urine UV-reflectance use in the hunting behaviour of the common kestrel Falco tinnunculus. J Avian Biol. 2006;37(4): 318–322.
  21. 21. Caro SP, Balthazart J, Bonadonna F. The perfume of reproduction in birds: chemosignaling in avian social life. Hormon Behav. 2015;68: 25–42. pmid:24928570
  22. 22. Milahova M, Berg ML, Buchanan KL, Bennett ATD. Odour-based discrimination of subspecies, species and sexes in an avian species complex, the crimson rosella. Anim Behav. 2014;95: 155–164.
  23. 23. Bonadonna F, Nevitt GA. Partner-specific odor recognition in an Antarctic seabird. Science. 2004;306(5697): 835. pmid:15514149
  24. 24. Whittaker DJ, Richmond KM, Miller AK, Kiley R, Burns CB, Atwell JW, et al. Intraspecific preen oil odor preferences in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis). Behav. Ecol. 2011;22(6): 1256–1263.
  25. 25. Amo L, Avilés JM, Parejo D, Peña A, Rodríguez J, Tomás G. Sex recognition by odour and variation in the uropygial gland secretion in starlings. J Anim Ecol. 2012;81(3): 605–613. pmid:22220811
  26. 26. Krause ET, Krüger O, Kohlmeier P, Caspers BA. Olfactory kin recognition in a songbird. Biol Lett. 2012;8(3): 327–329. pmid:22219391
  27. 27. Coffin HR, Watters JV, Mateo JM. Odor-based recognition of familiar and related conspecifics: a first test conducted on captive Humboldt penguins (Spheniscus humboldti). PLoS ONE. 2011;6(9):1–4. pmid:21957471
  28. 28. Bonadonna F, Sanz-Aguilar A. Kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance in wild birds: the first evidence for individual kin-related odour recognition. Anim Behav. 2012;84(3): 509–513.
  29. 29. Amo L, López-Rull I, Pagán I, García CM. Male quality and conspecific scent preferences in the house finch, Carpodacus mexicanus. Anim Behav. 2012;84(6): 1483–1489.
  30. 30. Petit C, Hossaert-McKey M., Perret P, Blondel J., Lambrechts MM. Blue tits use selected plants and olfaction to maintain an aromatic environment for nestlings. Ecol Lett. 2002;5(4): 585–589.
  31. 31. Gwinner H, Berger S. Starling males select green nest material by olfaction using experience-independent and experience-dependent cues. Anim Behav. 2008;75(3): 971–976.
  32. 32. Mennerat A. Blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) respond to an experimental change in the aromatic plant odour composition of their nest. Behav Processes. 2008;79(3): 189–191. pmid:18692552
  33. 33. Wallraff HG. Avian olfactory navigation: its empirical foundation and conceptual state. Anim Behav. 2004;67(2): 189–204.
  34. 34. Nevitt GA, Bonadonna F. Sensitivity to dimethyl sulphide suggests a mechanism for olfactory navigation by seabirds. Biol Lett. 2005;1(3): 303–305. pmid:17148193
  35. 35. Mínguez E. Olfactory nest recognition by British storm-petrel chicks. Anim Behav. 1997;53(4): 701–707.
  36. 36. Bonadonna F, Villafane M, Bajzak C, Jouventin P. Recognition of burrow’s olfactory signature in blue petrels, Halobaena caerulea: an efficient discrimination mechanism in the dark. Anim Behav. 2004;67(5): 893–898.
  37. 37. Amo L, Galván I, Tomás G, Sanz JJ. Predator odour recognition and avoidance in a songbird. Funct Ecol. 2008;22(2): 289–293.
  38. 38. Roth TCII, Cox JG, Lima SL. Can foraging birds assess predation risk by scent? Anim Behav. 2008;76(6): 2021–2027.
  39. 39. Amo L, Visser ME, Oers KV. Smelling out predators is innate in birds. Ardea. 2011;99(2): 177–184.
  40. 40. Zidar J, Løvlie H. Scent of the enemy: behavioural responses to predator faecal odour in the fowl. Anim Behav. 2012;84(3): 547–554.
  41. 41. Eichholz MW, Dassow JA, Stafford JD, Weatherhead PJ. Experimental evidence that nesting ducks use mammalian urine to assess predator abundance. The Auk. 2012;129(4): 638–644.
  42. 42. Houston DC. Scavenging efficiency of Turkey vultures in tropical forest. Condor. 1986;88(3): 318–323.
  43. 43. Graves GR. Greater yellow-headad vulture (Cathartes melambrotus) locates food by olfaction. J Raptor Res. 1992;26(1): 38–39.
  44. 44. Stager KE. Avian olfaction. Am Zool. 1967;7(3): 415–420.
  45. 45. Yang SY, Walther BA, Weng GJ. Stop and smell the pollen: The role of olfaction and vision of the oriental honey buzzard in identifying food. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(7): 1–18. pmid:26177533
  46. 46. Hagelin JC. Observations on the olfactory ability of the kakapo Strigops habroptilus, the critically endangered parrot of New Zealand. Ibis. 2004;146(1): 161–164.
  47. 47. Nevitt GA, Veit RR, Kareiva P. Dimethyl sulphide as a foraging cue for Antarctic Procellariiform seabirds. Nature. 1995;376(6542): 680–682.
  48. 48. Cunningham GB, Strauss V, Ryan PG. African penguins (Spheniscus demersus) can detect dimethyl sulphide, a prey-related odour. J Exp Biol. 2008;211(19): 3123–3127. pmid:18805811
  49. 49. Amo L, Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Barbosa A. Olfactory detection of dimethyl sulphide in a krill-eating Antarctic penguin. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2013;474: 277–285.
  50. 50. Wenzel BM. Olfactory Prowess of the Kiwi. Nature. 1968;220(5172): 1133–1134. pmid:5723611
  51. 51. Cunningham SJ, Castro I, Potter MA. The relative importance of olfaction and remote touch in prey detection by North Island brown kiwis. Anim Behav. 2009;78(4): 899–905.
  52. 52. Amo L, Jansen JJ, Van Dam NM, Dicke M, Visser ME. Birds exploit herbivore-induced plant volatiles to locate herbivorous prey. Ecol Lett. 2013; 16(11):1348–1355. pmid:24103093
  53. 53. Mäntylä E, Klemola T, Haukioja E. Attraction of willow warblers to sawfly-damaged mountain birches: novel function of inducible plant defences?. Ecol Lett. 2004;7(10): 915–918.
  54. 54. Mäntylä E, Alessio GA, Blande JD, Heijari J, Holopainen JK, Laaksonen T, et al. From plants to birds: higher avian predation rates in trees responding to insect herbivory. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(7): 1–8. pmid:18665271
  55. 55. Koski TM, Laaksonen T, Mäntylä E, Ruuskanen S, Li T, Girón-Calva PS, et al. Do insectivorous birds use volatile organic compounds from plants as olfactory foraging cues? Three experimental tests. Ethol. 2015;121: 1131–1144.
  56. 56. Mäntylä E, Kleier S, Kipper S, Hilker M. The attraction of insectivorous tit species to herbivore-damaged Scots pines. J Ornithol. 2017;158(2): 479.
  57. 57. Mrazova A, Sam K. Application of methyl jasmonate to grey willow (Salix cinerea) attracts insectivorous birds in nature. Arthropod Plant Interact. 2017. 1–8.
  58. 58. Cardé RT, Baker TC. Sexual communication with pheromones. In: Chemical Ecology of Insects; 1984. pp. 355–383. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3368-3
  59. 59. Yew JY, Chung H. Insect pheromones: An overview of function, form, and discovery. Prog Lipid Res. 2015;59: 88–105. pmid:26080085
  60. 60. Cross JV, Solomon MG, Chandler D, Jarrett P, Richardson PN, Winstanley D, et al. Biocontrol of pests of apples and pears in Northern and Central Europe: 1. Microbial Agents and Nematodes. Biocontrol Sci Technol. 1999:9(2): 125–149.
  61. 61. Betts MM. The food of Titmice in Oak Woodland. J Anim Ecol. 1955;24(2): 282–323.
  62. 62. Mols CMM, Visser ME. Great tits can reduce caterpillar damage in apple orchards. J Appl Ecol. 2002;39(6): 888–899.
  63. 63. Veľký M, Kaňuch P, Krištín A. Food composition of wintering great tits (Parus major): Habitat and seasonal aspects. Folia Zool. 2011;60(3): 228–236.
  64. 64. Soria S. Operopthera brumata. In Lepidopteros defoliadores de Quercus pyrenaica, Willdenow, 1805. Boletin de Sanidad Vegetal. Secretaría Técnica. Ministerio de Agricultura, Pesca Y Alimentación. Madrid; 1987. pp. 79–84.
  65. 65. Bestman HJ, Brosche T, Koschatzky KH, Michaelis K, Platz H, Roth K, et al. Pheromone-XLII. 1, 3, 6, 9-nonadecatetraen, das sexual pheromon des frost-spanners Operophtera brumata (Geometridae). Tetrahedron Lett. 1982;23(39): 4007–4010.
  66. 66. Roelofs WL, Hill AS, Linn CE, Meinwald J, Jain SC, Herbert HJ, et al. Sex pheromone of the winter moth, a Geometrid with unusually low temperature precopulatory responses. Science. 1982;217(4560): 657–659. pmid:17817538
  67. 67. Posa MRC, Sodhi NS, Koh LP. Predation on artificial nests and caterpillar models across a disturbance gradient in Subic Bay, Philippines. J Trop Ecol. 2007;23(1):27–33.
  68. 68. Richards LA, Coley PD. Seasonal and habitat differences affect the impact of food and predation on herbivores: A comparison between gaps and understory of a tropical forest. Oikos. 2007;116(1): 31–40.
  69. 69. Mäntylä E, Blande JD, Klemola T. Does application of methyl jasmonate to birch mimic herbivory and attract insectivorous birds in nature?. Arthropod-Plant Interact. 2014;8(2): 143–153.
  70. 70. Remmel T, Davison J, Tammaru T. Quantifying predation on folivorous insect larvae: The perspective of life-history evolution. Biol J Linnean Soc. 2011;104(1): 1–18.
  71. 71. Tvardikova K, Novotny V. Predation on exposed and leaf-rolling artificial caterpillars in tropical forests of Papua New Guinea. J Trop Ecol. 2012;28(4): 331–341.
  72. 72. Sam K, Koane B, Novotny V. Herbivore damage increases avian and ant predation of caterpillars on trees along a complete elevational forest gradient in Papua New Guinea. Ecography. 2015;38(3): 293–300.
  73. 73. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna: Austria. R Core Team; 2015.
  74. 74. Alma PJ. A study of the activity and behaviour of the winter moth, Operophtera brumata L. (Lepidoptera: Hydriomenidae). Entomol mon mag. 1970;105(1265): 258–265.
  75. 75. Jain SC, Roelofs WL, Meinwald J. Synthetic of sex attractant pheromone from a geometrid moth Operopthera brumata (the winter moth). J. Org. Chem. 1983. 48(13): 2274–2276.
  76. 76. Muiruri EW, Rainio K, Koricheva J. Do birds see the forest for the trees? Scale-dependent effects of tree diversity on avian predation of artificial larvae. Oecol. 2016;180(3): 619–630. pmid:26201260
  77. 77. Stauss MJ, Burkhardt JF, Tomiuk J. Foraging flight distances as a measure of parental effort in blue tits Parus caeruleus differ with environmental conditions. J. Avian Biol. 2005;36: 47–56.
  78. 78. Naef-Danzer B, Keller L. The foraging performance of great and blue tits (Parus major and P. caeruleus) in relation to caterpillar devepolment, and its consequences for nestling growth and fledging weight. J. Animal Ecol. 1999; 68:708–18.
  79. 79. Amo L, Tomás G, López-García A. Role of chemical and visual cues of mammalian predators in nest defense in birds. ‎Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2017;71:49.
  80. 80. Heppner JB. Brachyptery and aptery in lepidoptera. Trop Lepid Res. 1991;2(1):11–40.
  81. 81. Mooney KA, Gruner DS, Barber NA, Van Bael SA, Philpott SM, Greenberg R. Interactions among predators and the cascading effects of vertebrate insectivores on arthropod communities and plants. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107(16): 7335–7340. pmid:20368418
  82. 82. Askenmo AC, Von Brömssen A, Ekman J, Jansson C. Impact of some wintering birds on spider abundance in Spruce. Oikos. 1977;28(1): 90–94.
  83. 83. Gunnarsson B. Bird Predation and vegetation structure affecting Spruce-Living Arthropods in a temperate forest. J Anim Ecol. 1996;65(3): 389–397.
  84. 84. Cowie RJ, Hinsley SA. Feeding ecology of Great Tits (Parus major) and Blue Tits (Parus caeruleus), breeding in suburban gardens. J Anim Ecol. 2017;57(2): 611–626.
  85. 85. Van Bael SA, Brawn JD, Robinson SK. Birds defend trees from herbivores in a Neotropical forest canopy. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2003;100(14): 8304–8307. pmid:12832618
  86. 86. Mäntylä E, Klemola T, Laaksonen T. Birds help plants: a meta-analysis of top-down trophic cascades caused by avian predators. Oecologia. 2011;165(1): 143–151. pmid:20852895
  87. 87. Marquis RJ, Whelan CJ. Insectivorous birds increase growth of White Oak through consumption of Leaf-Chewing insects. Ecology. 1994;75(7): 2007–2014.
  88. 88. Sipura M. Tritrophic interactions: willows, herbivorous insects and insectivorous birds. Oecologia. 1999;121(4): 537–545. pmid:28308363
  89. 89. Harari AR, Zahavi T, Thiéry D. Fitness cost of pheromone production in signaling female moths. Evolution. 2011;65(6): 1572–1582. pmid:21644949
  90. 90. Mcneil JN. Behavioral ecology of pheromone-mediated communication in moths and its importance in the use of pheromone traps. Annu Rev Entomol. 1991;36: 407–430.