Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Inhalation of volatile anesthetics via a laryngeal mask is associated with lower incidence of intraoperative awareness in non-critically ill patients

  • Pei-Jen Kuo ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Pei-Jen Kuo, Chia-Ling Lee

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Buddhist Tzu-Chi General Hospital, Hualien, Taiwan

  • Chia-Ling Lee ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Pei-Jen Kuo, Chia-Ling Lee

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Buddhist Tzu-Chi General Hospital, Hualien, Taiwan

  • Jen-Huang Wang,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Medical Research, Buddhist Tzu-Chi General Hospital, Hualien, Taiwan

  • Shiu-Ying Hsieh,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Buddhist Tzu-Chi General Hospital, Hualien, Taiwan

  • Shian-Che Huang,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Anesthesiology, Buddhist Tzu-Chi General Hospital, Hualien, Taiwan

  • Chen-Fuh Lam

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ed110208@edah.org.tw

    Affiliations Department of Anesthesiology, Buddhist Tzu-Chi General Hospital, Hualien, Taiwan, Department of Anesthesiology, E-Da Hospital/E-Da Cancer Hospital/I-Shou University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan

Abstract

Background

Increased incidence of intraoperative awareness was reported in critically ill patients during major operations, particularly under total intravenous (TIVA) or endotracheal general anesthesia (ETGA). However, the incidence and effect of anesthesia techniques on awareness in generally healthy, non-critically ill patients during operations have yet to receive significant attention.

Methods and results

This retrospective matched case-control study was conducted between January 2009 to December 2014. Surgical patients (ASA physical status I-III) whom reported intraoperative awareness during this study period were interviewed and their medical records were reviewed. The potential risk factors for awareness were compared with the non-case matched controls, who were randomly selected from the database. A total of 61436 patients were included and 16 definite cases of intraoperative awareness were identified. Patients who received ETGA and TIVA had significantly higher incidence of developing awareness compared to those who were anesthetized using laryngeal masks (LMA) (P = 0.03). Compared with the matched controls (n = 80), longer anesthesia time was associated with increased incidence of awareness (odds ratio 2.04; 95% CI 1.30–3.20, per hour increase). Perioperative use of muscle relaxant was also associated with increased incidence of awareness, while significantly lower incidence of awareness was found in patients who were anesthetized with volatile anesthetics.

Conclusions

The overall incidence of awareness was 0.023% in the ASA≤ III surgical patients who received general anesthesia. Anesthesia with a laryngeal mask under spontaneous ventilation and supplemented with volatile anesthetics may be the preferred anesthesia technique in generally healthy patients in order to provide a lower risk of intraoperative awareness.

Introduction

Intraoperative awareness is the unexpected recall of explicit memory during anesthesia. Most patients recall the intraoperative events without pain, some reported vague auditory recall or sensations of dreaming, while a small amount of serious cases experienced pain and tremendous stress [1]. The development of awareness during general anesthesia can be devastating to the patients and anesthesia team. A major concern is that patients whom reported experiencing intraoperative awareness are vulnerable to numerous psychological sequelae after anesthesia, including nightmares, depression, and post-traumatic stress disorder [2,3]. The occurrence of awareness may also implicate medico-legal problems [4], as awareness contributed to 1.9% of the American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) Closed Claim Project [5]. The reported incidence of intraoperative awareness varied from 0.0068% to 0.18% [68]. More recently, the 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) reported that the incidence of awareness in the United Kingdom was 1 in 19,000 (0.0052%) of anesthesia cases [9]. Historically, patients presenting with higher ASA physical status (ASA PS) and critically ill patients who received cardiothoracic traumatic resuscitation or other major/emergency surgeries are at significantly higher risk of developing intraoperative awareness [1012], probably due to suboptimal levels of anesthesia during the procedures [13]. It has also been proposed that the use of supraglottic device may mitigate the incidence and severity of awareness [14]. Since no previous studies have characterized the incidence and risk stratification of intraoperative awareness in the generally healthy or less critically ill population, we analyzed the incidence and characterized the associated risk factors for the occurrence of intraoperative awareness in patients with ASA PS I-III during non-critical surgeries under general anesthesia. The primary objective of our study was to identify the risk factors for accidental intraoperative awareness in generally healthy patients, and the second objective was to determine the effects of different anesthesia techniques on the development of awareness during operation.

Materials and methods

Patient database

This retrospective chart-review matched case-control study carried out in a tertiary teaching medical center located at Hualien City of Taiwan that consisted of 945 beds. The study was approved by the ethics committee and the institutional review board (IRB, Approval number IRB106-22-B). Since all the data were collected retrospectively from the routine clinical records and questionnaires were completed under the standard practice guidelines, requirement for written informed consent was waived by the committee. In-hospital patients who received anesthesia management for surgical and other medical interventions from January 2009 to December 2014 were visited at bedside within 24 hours after operation. During the routine postoperative visit, nurse anesthetists collected all perioperative adverse events from the patients or caregivers, which including awareness during operation.

Identification of accidental awareness

Intraoperative awareness is defined as an experience of consciousness under general anesthesia with subsequent recall of the experienced events after emergence [15]. Patients who received regional anesthesia, outpatient surgery, ASA class ≥IV, and those admitted to intensive care units after surgery were excluded from the review. All cases of self-reported intraoperative awareness were interviewed by an experienced nurse anesthetist, and the structured Brice interview was then retrospectively completed with the assistance of the nurse anesthetist [16]. The Brice interview questionnaire included the following five questions:

  1. What was the last thing the patient remembered happening before went to asleep?
  2. What was the first thing the patient remembered happening on waking?
  3. Did the patient dream or have any other experiences whilst he/she was asleep?
  4. What was the worst thing about the operation?
  5. What was the next worst thing?

A review committee, which consisted of four senior anesthesiologists, was organized. The events and severity of awareness were respectively identified by the review committee using the Michigan awareness instrument and the NAP5 severity [17]. Cases of definite intraoperative awareness had to be approved by all the committee members.

Matched case-controls

Control cases were randomly selected from the surgical patients who received general anesthesia without reporting intraoperative awareness during the study period, following exact matching with age, ASA classifications, and gender between the case (awareness) and control (non-awareness) patients in a 1 to 5 ratio (Fig 1). These three characteristic parameters are often used as matching variables because they are generally considered as strong confounders [18].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Study design and flow diagram.

ASA PS: American Association of Anesthesiologists physical status; EA: epidural anesthesia; ICU: intensive care unit; NB: nerve block; SA: spinal anesthesia.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.g001

Statistics

Since the anesthesia record of one awareness patient was missing from the chart file, the medication of this patient during the perioperative period was not reported. The values of continuous variables were compared using an independent two-sample t test or one-way ANOVA. Categorical variables were compared using chi-square or Fisher’s exact test. The potential risk factors included patient demographic and clinical variables, namely patient age groups, genders, ASA PS, types of anesthesia, duration of anesthesia, anesthetics for maintenance, use of NMBA, use of midazolam, dosage of fentanyl and ephedrine. Conditional logistic regression model was adopted to evaluate the association between these risk factors and intraoperative awareness. Statistical significance was accepted at a level of P< 0.05. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, North Carolina).

Results

General outcomes

During the study period, a total of 94444 in-hospital patients received anesthesia management for surgical or other invasive interventions, and of those, 61436 patients were included in this analysis after excluding cases with regional anesthesia, ASA PS≥ IV, or ICU admission after operation (Fig 1). The average annual completion rate (2009–2014) for bedside visiting within 24 h after operation was 97.8% (97.0–98.7%). Nineteen patients claimed to experience awareness during anesthesia, and the Brice interview questionnaire was completed for each patient. After committee review, three patients were excluded from the analysis due to unsustained evidence of intraoperative awareness (Fig 1). The characteristics and detailed descriptions for patients who experienced definite intraoperative awareness with explicit recall were summarized in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics and descriptions of intraoperative awareness.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.t001

Female patients were more likely to experience intraoperative awareness than males, but the difference was not statistically significant (62.5% vs 46.2%, P = 0.217; Table 2). Levels of ASA PS (I-III) and age of patients were similar between those with and without awareness (Table 2). In comparison to the non-case patients at risk, significantly fewer patients who developed intraoperative awareness were anesthetized with laryngeal mask anesthesia (LMA; P = 0.003) (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Characteristic analysis of intraoperative awareness in at-risk patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.t002

Comparison analysis with non-case matched controls

Body weight and level of education were not found to be significantly related to intraoperative awareness, however, the mean duration of anesthesia was found to be significantly longer for the case group (n = 16) compared to the controls (n = 80) (3.5±2.4 vs 1.9±1.2 h, case vs control group; P< 0.001; Table 3). Two patients (12.5%) in the awareness group and 4 patients (5%) in the matched control group expired within 1 year after their last case of anesthesia (P = 0.261; Table 3). The duration between the last case of anesthesia and expiration were 144–301 and 15–341 (range) days in the case and matched control groups respectively. Patient’s education level or personal religious belief was not different between the awareness cases and matched controls (data not shown). The use of propofol-based IVGA, neuromuscular blocking agents and higher doses of fentanyl during operation were associated with significantly higher incidence of developing awareness; while administration of midazolam or doses of ephedrine for treatment of intraoperative hypotension were not different between the awareness and matched control groups (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 3. Characteristic analysis of intraoperative awareness (cases vs matched controls).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Anesthetic-related factors for intraoperative awareness (cases vs matched controls).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.t004

Conditional logistic regression analysis

The crude odds ratios of experiencing intraoperative awareness were 11.75 (95% CI 1.47–93.74; P = 0.020) and 17.75 (95% CI 1.35–233.96; P = 0.028) respectively for patients who received ETGA/IVG and LMA, but the differences became insignificant after multivariate analysis (Table 5). The effects of anesthetics and other medications during perioperative period were also analyzed. Anesthesia with propofol-based IVGA and use of muscle relaxants during the operation was associated with significantly higher incidence of intraoperative awareness compared to volatile anesthetics and those received no muscle relaxation, respectively (Table 5). Perioperative use of midazolam and fentanyl did not affect the incidence of awareness in multivariate analysis. Administration of ephedrine for correction of hypotension during operation did not affect the development of awareness, but prolonged duration of anesthesia significantly increased the risk of awareness (AOR 4.26 per hour increase; 95% CI 1.33–13.63; P = 0.015).

thumbnail
Table 5. Conditional logistic regression analysis of the risk factors associated with awareness.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.t005

Discussion

By analyzing our post-anesthesia quality assurance records through 2009–2014, we determined the overall incidence of developing intraoperative awareness among surgical patients with ASA PS I-III to be 0.023%. General anesthesia using endotracheal tubes, propofol-based intravenous anesthesia techniques, and prolonged periods under anesthesia were especially associated with increased risk for developing awareness. Clinical research regarding the incidence and identification of the associated risk factors in accidental intraoperative awareness has been a challenge to epidemiologic studies, as intraoperative awareness is a relatively rare perioperative event and subject to high levels of discrepancy in clinical diagnostic criteria [19]. The reported incidence of intraoperative awareness was generally higher in prospective studies (1:250 to 1:1000) [6,7,20,21], which may have been confounded by false memories or dreaming [19]. In large-scale retrospective studies, the incidence of awareness was significantly lower (1:10000 to 1:19000) in general the surgical population and cancer patients [8,22,23]. It is clear that retrospective studies are subject to under-reporting, missing cases, and recall bias. A total of 19 patients claimed to awake during general anesthesia in our study, but 3 patients without explicit recall were excluded from the final analysis, as the review committee members believed that they were more likely to have perioperative dreaming rather than awareness [24]. Therefore, we identified an overall incidence of 0.023% (approximately 1:4300) for developing accidental intraoperative awareness in the generally healthy surgical patients (ASA PS class ≤ 3), which falls between the incidence reported in the prospective and retrospective studies. Nevertheless, we believed that the overall incidence of our study could actually be higher, as the critically ill patients and those who transferred to ICU after major operations were excluded from the analysis.

There is sufficient clinical evidence to suggest that patients presenting with high ASA PS (> III) and those admitted to ICU for postoperative care are at significantly higher risk of awareness [1,3,8]. Since high risk patients (ASA IV-V) contribute to only a limited portion (<10%) of the entire surgical population [25,26], the exclusion of these high-risk patients or major surgery (such as cardiac bypass or trauma resuscitation surgery) from our post-anesthesia quality assurance record may provide better insight into the other risk factors associated with intraoperative awareness in the majority population.

In patients with lower anesthesia risks, the class of ASA PS did not affect the occurrence of awareness. Age was also not a significant differentiating factor for patients who developed awareness and those who did not. Female gender has been considered a potential risk factor for awareness, as females are quicker to emerge from anesthesia [2729]. Our study showed that the proportion of female patients (62.5% vs 37.5%) was higher in the awareness group, even though there were more male patients included in the whole study population (53.8% vs 46.2%). Nonetheless, the difference in genders was not statistically significant (P = 0.217). In light of the underpowered statistical analysis, we suggest that gender should not be excluded as a risk factor for intraoperative awareness until more substantial clinical evidence is found.

The other potential associated risk factors were determined by comparing the characteristics and intraoperative parameters between the case (n = 16) and matched non-case controls (n = 80). Previous studies have reported a higher incidence of awareness in obese patients [22,30]. Our analysis showed that body mass index (BMI) was not an independent factor for awareness in patients with ASA PS I-III after case-control matching. Most interestingly, we found that total anesthesia time was significantly longer in patients who eventually developed awareness with a crude odd ratio of 2.04 (95% CI 1.30–3.20) per hour increase of anesthesia time. The effect of anesthesia duration on the development of intraoperative awareness has not been previously reported. In our opinion, chances of medication or equipment errors are more likely to happen during prolonged operations and gaps between working shifts [31].

Anesthesia-related medications, such as anesthetics, muscle relaxants and opioids, have been extensively reported to affect the incidence of intraoperative awareness [20,22]. In line with these previous studies, our study confirmed that anesthetized with volatile halogenated anesthetics was associated with significantly lower incidence of awareness than propofol-based IVGA. Minimal alveolar concentrations (MAC) of volatile anesthetics and the processed electroencephalographic bispectral index (BIS) are the two commonly recommended techniques in the monitoring of depth of anesthesia during operation to avoid anesthesia awareness [32]. In our institute, MAC is routinely monitored throughout the anesthesia period whenever an inhaled anesthetic is administered, but BIS monitoring for propofol-based IVGA is rarely used, as the accessories of the BIS system are not subsidized by the Taiwan National Health Insurance. In fact, the application of processed electroencephalogram (including BIS, E-Entropy, or Narcotrend) during general anesthesia is as low as the 2.8% reported in the United Kingdom [25]. Although our results were not able to establish a direct causal relationship, it is clinically reasonable to apply processed electroencephalogram for monitoring the depth of anesthesia, particularly during propofol-based IVGA, in order to reduce the occurrence of awareness [33].

One of the most important risk factors for intraoperative awareness is the use of neuromuscular blocking agents [22]. Loss of reflexive somatic responses to surgical stimuli following profound motor paralysis diminished the postural signs of awareness in patients who received general anesthesia [29]. Consistent with these previous studies, our study demonstrates that use of neuromuscular blocking agent significantly increased awareness events by an odds ratio of 4.64 (95% CI 1.19–18.04). The dosage of a short-acting opioid fentanyl administered during operation was also found to be positively correlated with the occurrence of awareness (OR 1.01 per μg increase, 95% CI 1.00, 1.02; P = 0.005). Since the total doses of fentanyl administrated during operation are inevitably increased for lengthier surgical procedures, doses of fentanyl might be a dependent variable of prolonged anesthesia time. On the other hand, the perioperative use of midazolam did not significantly affect the incidence of awareness.

Since light anesthesia secondary to perioperative hypotension has been recognized as a crucial risk factor for awareness [34], we compared the dosage of ephedrine (the most commonly used inotropic agent during anesthesia in Taiwan) between the case and non-case matched controls as a surrogate indicator for perioperative hypotension. Our results showed that the doses of ephedrine used during operation periods were similar between the two groups. However, without appropriate comparison of hemodynamic measurements, the actual effects of perioperative hypotension on the development of awareness in non-critically ill surgical patients have yet to reach a conclusion. In this study, we also observed that the 1-year mortality rate after the last case of anesthesia was increased in patients who developed intraoperative awareness (12.5% vs 5%, case vs matched control; P = 0.129). Since the numbers of postanesthesia mortality were too small and the time from the last case of anesthesia to the date of expiration ranged from 15 to 341 days, analysis of a larger scale database or prospective studies are warranted to determine the actual clinical implication of intraoperative awareness and early postoperative mortality.

The most important finding of analysis in this case-control matching study is that the occurrence of intraoperative awareness was significantly reduced in patients who received LMA compared to those that received ETGA and IVGA. After matching for age, gender, and ASA PS, the use of LMA was found to be associated with significantly reduced risk of developing intraoperative awareness compared to the use of ETGA and IVGA. Compared with LMA, ETGA and IVGA carried crude odds ratios of 11.75 (95% CI 1.47–93.74) and 17.75 (95% CI 1.35–233.98) respectively for intraoperative awareness.

Based on our analysis, we tried to address the important issue that whether different types of anesthetic techniques would affect the incidence of intraoperative awareness. The use of LMA supplemented by volatile anesthetics has become a widely used technique that can help avoid the administration of neuromuscular agents throughout the anesthetic period. The uptake of inhaled anesthetics depends on the patient’s spontaneous ventilation with constant monitoring of anesthetic depth by MAC levels. Since LMA is the preferred technique for the majority of less invasive surgical procedures in all age groups [3536], the results of our study support the ratiocination proposed by Drs. Avidan and Mashour that the use of supraglottis device anesthesia would mitigate both the incidence and severity of intraoperative awareness [14].

There are a number of limitations in our study. First, the retrospective study design limits our ability to establish a direct causal relationship between the measured variables and intraoperative awareness incidence. The study design is also subject to missing cases and memory loss, as awareness is not the primary parameter in our post-anesthesia quality assurance record. Hence, the results of our study might be prone to recall bias and confounded by other unmeasured risk factors. Second, although high completion rates of postoperative visits (98%) might provide sufficient power in detecting intraoperative awareness in the in-hospital surgical patients, the incidence of awareness in outpatient ambulatory surgery was not included in the analysis. Since all outpatients are cared for at least 1 hour after recovery from anesthesia in our hospital, any major postanesthesia complications should have been recorded by the staff in the post-anesthesia care unit. Third, the numbers of definite awareness reported in this study were relatively small, which made certain statistical analysis underpowered, in particular the gender and 1-year mortality analyses. However, considering the fact that intraoperative awareness is a rare event, the collection of large sample size of awareness is very strenuous in the retrospective, or even in the prospective studies [7,8,20]. Finally, patients with perioperative awareness were not routinely followed up after discharge from our hospital. Therefore, we are not able to present the long-term psychophysiological consequences of these patients.

In this study we determined the overall incidence of developing intraoperative awareness among non-critically ill patients to be 0.023%. General anesthesia using endotracheal tubes, intravenous propofol-based techniques, and prolonged periods under anesthesia were especially associated with increased risk for developing awareness. The use of neuromuscular blocking agents during anesthesia also carried significantly higher incidence of awareness. Base on these findings, we suggest that spontaneous ventilation via a laryngeal mask supplemented with volatile anesthetics may a preferable anesthesia technique in order to provide a lower risk of intraoperative awareness in generally healthy patients.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Minimal dataset for statistical analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186337.s001

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to express our deepest gratitude to Ms. Xiu-Zhu Liu (Department of Anesthesiology, Hualien Tzu Chi General Hospital, Taiwan) and Ms. Tzu-Ting Cheng (Department of Anesthesiology, Taipei Medical University Hospital, Taiwan) for assistance in data processing and preparation of manuscript. The advices provided by Mr. Young-How Lam (School of Medicine and Pharmacology, University of Western Australia) in the English language editing are also gratefully acknowledged.

References

  1. 1. Rule E, Reddy S (2014) Awareness under general anesthesia. Br J Hosp Med (Lond) 75: 573–577.
  2. 2. Osterman JE, Hopper J, Heran WJ, Keane TM, van der Kolk BA (2001) Awareness under anesthesia and the developmentof posttraumatic stress disorder. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 23: 198–204. pmid:11543846
  3. 3. Whitlock EL, Rodebaugh TL, Hassett AL, Shanks AM, Kolarik E, Houghtby J, et al (2015) Psychological sequelae of surgery in a prospective cohort of patients from three intraoperative awareness prevention trials. Anesth Analg 120: 87–95. pmid:25383719
  4. 4. Payne JP (1994) Awareness and its medicolegal implications. Br J Anaesth 73: 38–45. pmid:8038055
  5. 5. Domino KB, Posner KL, Caplan RA, Cheney FW (1999) Awareness during anesthesia: a closed claims analysis. Anesthesiology 90: 1053–1061. pmid:10201677
  6. 6. Sebel PS, Bowdle TA, Ghoneim MM, Rampil IJ, Padilla RE, Gan TJ, et al (2004) The incidence of awareness during anesthesia: a multicenter United States study. Anesth Analg 99: 833–839. pmid:15333419
  7. 7. Sandin RH, Enlund G, Samuelsson P, Lennmarken C (2000) Awareness during anesthesia: a prospective case study. Lancet 355: 707–711. pmid:10703802
  8. 8. Pollard RJ, Coyle JP, Gilbert RL, Beck JE (2007) Intraoperative awareness in a regional medical system: a review of 3 years' data. Anesthesiology 106: 269–274. pmid:17264720
  9. 9. Pandit JJ, Cook TM, Jonker WR, O'Sullivan E (2013) A national survey of anesthetists (NAP5 baseline) to estimate an annual incidence of accidental awareness during general anesthesia in the UK. Br J Anaesth 110: 501–509. pmid:23482998
  10. 10. Bogetz MS, Katz JA (1984) Recall of surgery for major trauma. Anesthesiology 61: 6–9. pmid:6742485
  11. 11. Phillips AA, McLean RF, Devitt JH, Harrington EM (1993) Recall of intraoperative events after general anesthesia and cardiopulmonary bypass. Can J Anaesth 40: 922–926. pmid:8222030
  12. 12. Ghoneim MM (2000) Awareness during anesthesia: Anesthesiology 92: 597–602. pmid:10691248
  13. 13. Ranta SO, Laurila R, Saario J, Ali-Melkkilä T, Hynynen M (1998) Awareness with recall during general anesthesia: incidence and risk factors. Anesth Analg 86: 1084–1089. pmid:9585303
  14. 14. Avidan MS, Mashour G (2013) The incidence of intraoperative awareness in the UK: under the rate or under the radar? Br J Anaesth 110: 494–497. pmid:23482997
  15. 15. Mashour GA, Orser BA, Avidan MS (2011) Intraoperative awareness: from neurobiology to clinical practice. Anesthesiology 114: 1218–1233. pmid:21464699
  16. 16. Brice DD, Hetherington RR, Utting JE (1970) A simple study of awareness and dreaming during anesthesia. Br J Anaesth 42: 535–542. pmid:5423844
  17. 17. Pandit JJ, Andrade J, Bogod DG, Hitchman JM, Jonker WR, Lucas N, et al (2014) 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental awareness during general anesthesia: protocol, methods, and analysis of data. Br J Anaesth 113: 540–548. pmid:25204695
  18. 18. Wacholder S, Silverman DT, McLaughlin JK, Mandel JS (1992) Selection of controls in case-control studies. III. Design options. Am J Epidemiol 135: 1042–1050. pmid:1595690
  19. 19. Pandit JJ, Cook TM, Jonker WR, O'Sullivan E (2013) A national survey of anesthetists (NAP5 Baseline) to estimate an annual incidence of accidental awareness during general anesthesia in the UK. Anesthesia 68: 343–353.
  20. 20. Errando CL, Sigl JC, Robles M, Calabuig E, García J, Arocas F, et al (2008) Awareness with recall during general anesthesia: a prospective observational evaluation of 4001 patients. Br J Anaesth 101: 178–185. pmid:18515816
  21. 21. Xu L, Wu AS, Yue Y (2009) The incidence of intra-operative awareness during general anesthesia in China: a multi-center observational study. Acta Anesthesiol Scand 53: 873–882.
  22. 22. Pandit JJ, Andrade J, Bogod DG, Hitchman JM, Jonker WR, Lucas N, et al (2014) 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental awareness during general anesthesia: summary of main findings and risk factors. Br J Anaesth 113: 549–559. pmid:25204697
  23. 23. Cascella M, Viscardi D, Schiavone V, Mehrabmi-Kermani F, Muzio MR, Forte CA, et al (2016) A 7-Year Retrospective Multisource Analysis on the Incidence of Anesthesia Awareness With Recall in Cancer Patients: A Chance of Collaboration Between Anesthesiologists and Psycho-Oncologists for Awareness Detection. Medicine (Baltimore) 95: e2757.
  24. 24. Leslie K, Skrzypek H (2007) Dreaming during anesthesia in adult patients. Best Pract Res Clin Anesthesiol 21: 403–414.
  25. 25. Sury MR, Palmer JH, Cook TM, Pandit JJ (2014) The state of UK anesthesia: a survey of National Health Service activity in 2013. Br J Anaesth 113: 575–584. pmid:25236896
  26. 26. Hopkins TJ, Raghunathan K, Barbeito A, Cooter M, Stafford-Smith M, Schroeder R, et al (2016) Associations between ASA Physical Status and postoperative mortality at 48 h: a contemporary dataset analysis compared to a historical cohort. Perioper Med (Lond) 5: 29.
  27. 27. Leslie K, Myles PS, Forbes A, Chan MT, Swallow SK, Short TG (2005) Dreaming during anesthesia in patients at high risk of awareness. Anesthesia 60: 239–244.
  28. 28. Buchanan FF, Myles PS, Leslie K, Forbes A, Cicuttini F (2006) Gender and recovery after general anesthesia combined with neuromuscular blocking drugs. Anesth Analg 102: 291–297. pmid:16368846
  29. 29. Ghoneim MM, Block RI, Haffarnan M, Mathews MJ (2009) Awareness during anesthesia: risk factors, causes and sequelae: a review of reported cases in the literature. Anesth Analg 108: 527–535. pmid:19151283
  30. 30. Gaszynski T, Wieczorek A (2016) A comparison of BIS recordings during propofol-based total intravenous anesthesia and sevoflurane-based inhalational anesthesia in obese patients. Anesthesiol Intensive Ther 48: 239–247.
  31. 31. Al-Hakim L, Gong XY (2012) On the day of surgery: how long does preventable disruption prolong the patient journey? Int J Health Care Qual Assur 25: 322–342. pmid:22755483
  32. 32. Avidan MS, Zhang L, Burnside BA, Finkel KJ, Searleman AC, Selvidge JA, et al (2008) Anesthesia awareness and the bispectral index. N Engl J Med 358:1097–1108. pmid:18337600
  33. 33. Escallier KE, Nadelson MR, Zhou D, Avidan MS (2014) Monitoring the brain: processed electroencephalogram and peri-operative outcomes. Anesthesia 69: 899–910.
  34. 34. Ghoneim MM (2007) Incidence of and risk factors for awareness during anesthesia. Best Pract Res Clin Anesthesiol 21: 327–343.
  35. 35. Xu R, Lian Y, Li WX (2016) Airway Complications during and after General Anesthesia: A Comparison, Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Using Flexible Laryngeal Mask Airways and Endotracheal Tubes. PLoS One 11: e0158137. pmid:27414807
  36. 36. Drake-Brockman TF, Ramgolam A, Zhang G, Hall GL, von Ungern-Sternberg BS (2017) The effect of endotracheal tubes versus laryngeal mask airways on perioperative respiratory adverse events in infants: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 389: 701–708. pmid:28108038