Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Developmental Changes in Adolescents' Olfactory Performance and Significance of Olfaction

  • Anna Oleszkiewicz ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Anna Oleszkiewicz, Ute Walliczek-Dworschak

    ania.oleszkiewicz@gmail.com

    Affiliations Interdisciplinary Center “Smell & Taste”, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany, Institute of Psychology, University of Wroclaw, Wroclaw, Poland

  • Ute Walliczek-Dworschak ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Anna Oleszkiewicz, Ute Walliczek-Dworschak

    Affiliations Interdisciplinary Center “Smell & Taste”, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Philipps-University, Marburg, Germany

  • Paula Klötze,

    Affiliation Interdisciplinary Center “Smell & Taste”, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Friederike Gerber,

    Affiliation Interdisciplinary Center “Smell & Taste”, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Ilona Croy,

    Affiliations Interdisciplinary Center “Smell & Taste”, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany, Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Thomas Hummel

    Affiliation Interdisciplinary Center “Smell & Taste”, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Abstract

Aim of the current work was to examine developmental changes in adolescents’ olfactory performance and personal significance of olfaction. In the first study olfactory identification abilities of 76 participants (31 males and 45 females aged between 10 and 18 years; M = 13.8, SD = 2.3) was evaluated with the Sniffin Stick identification test, presented in a cued and in an uncued manner. Verbal fluency was additionally examined for control purpose. In the second study 131 participants (46 males and 85 females aged between 10 and 18 years; (M = 14.4, SD = 2.2) filled in the importance of olfaction questionnaire. Odor identification abilities increased significantly with age and were significantly higher in girls as compared to boys. These effects were especially pronounced in the uncued task and partly related to verbal fluency. In line, the personal significance of olfaction increased with age and was generally higher among female compared to male participants.

Introduction

The human sense of smell develops in utero, odors are delivered to the fetus within the amniotic fluid [1]. Already at birth olfaction is functional and smells produce behavioral responses in newborns [2]. For example, newborns show signs of olfactory preferences [3] can distinguish between familiar and unfamiliar odors [4] and are even able to identify their mother by her smell, which is important for feeding and bonding with parents [5]. It is assumed that humans develop olfactory likes or dislikes by evaluative conditioning [6] starting during the prenatal period–although preferences also appear to be partly inherited [7]. The sense of smell helps infants, as well as adults, to gain information about food, environment or people [8]. Studies indicate that body odors influence various regulatory functions related to infants’ motoric functions [910] and physiological correlates of distress and arousal [1113]. Further, infants are able to discriminate the intensity and quality of non-human odors, such as acetic acid, asafoetida, phenylethyl alcohol or anise oil [14,15]. Those abilities seem to constantly develop between the age of 3 and 12 [16]. Studies involving older children have shown that olfactory identification increases further during early and late adolescence and the range of identifiable odorants broadens, partly due to the participants’ verbal abilities to name them [16] and partly due to gaining new experiences [17].

Olfactory identification performance seems to develop until the second decade of life, when it reaches the highest level and it starts to deteriorate after the end of the fifth decade. This might be caused by numerous health-related causes, such as cumulative effect of repeated infections on the olfactory epithelium [18,19].

As yet, most studies concentrated on the developmental aspects of olfactory abilities included infants, older children and adults, however, not many reports included adolescents. Adolescence seems to be the period of human development subjected to dynamic socio-biological changes that directly precedes the peak of olfactory ability. During this period of life, the human body is subjected to the profound biological changes including odor emission and perception [2023]; as well as changes concerning psychosocial functioning. For this reason adolescence seems to be important period of olfactory development, however the existing literature on olfaction in adolescence is scarce and needs to be supplemented.

Additionally, reports on sex-related differences in olfactory abilities in different age groups, including adolescence, are not conclusive. There are studies suggesting sex-related differences in olfactory abilities (for review see: [24]) and studies presenting contradictory findings [18,25,26]. Some of the tested models managed to explain the sex-related differences through the inclusion of additional variables, such as verbal abilities [16]. To expand the scope of knowledge on olfaction in adolescence, we propose two studies aimed at a) exploration of olfactory identification performance in adolescents and b) analysis of the influence and significance of odors in daily life’s context in this age group.

Material and Methods

Data were obtained from a total of 207 students (130 females, 77 males) in the age between 10 and 18. The study was performed in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki on Biomedical Studies Involving Human Subjects. Informed written consent was obtained from caretakers and children participating in Study 1, prior to their inclusion in the study. In Study 2 we only obtained oral consent, as this was an anonymous survey. The study design and consent approach was approved by the University of Dresden Medical Faculty Ethics Review Board (EK 416112014).

The participants constituted the sample of convenience. Statistical analyses was performed by means of SPSS v. 21 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) with p < 0.05 set as the level of significance.

Performance in Odor Identification–Study 1

Participants

In the study participated 76 students, aged from 10 to 18 years (M = 13.8, SD = 2.3). Of those, 45 were females between 10–17 years (M = 13.6, SD = 2.2) and 31 were males between 11–18 years (M = 14.1, SD = 2.4). To investigate age-related differences in odor identification performance, we divided participants into two age categories: 1) 10–14 years and 2) 15–18 years. This age criterion was made arbitrary and created two equinumerous groups, which was crucial for statistical reasoning. The study was conducted in boarding schools in Dresden, Germany.

Testing procedure

Six odors of the identification test from the Sniffin’ Sticks test (rose, leather, liquorice, clove, apple, fish) were presented to the participants. The first task was an uncued identification task of the odor, where they were asked to identify the odor without any descriptors given (uncued task). Scores ranged from either 2 points (correct identification) over 1 point (correct odor category, e.g. cloves odor "a “dentistry odor") to 0 points (incorrect identification or far miss, e.g. naming cloves “a spice”). In this task score ranged from 0 to 12 points. The next task was to identify each of the presented odors from a list of four verbal descriptors (cued identification). Here possible scores were 1 point (correct identification) or 0 points (incorrect identification). The range of possible scores was 0–6 points. We used odors from “Sniffin’ Sticks” battery because it is easily understood by participants, it is objective and validated [27]. Further the test is portable. Additionally, taking into account that most of the former studies on olfactory abilities in children used this test, we wanted to deliver comparable results. Results obtained with adult participants suggest, that verbal ability relates to olfactory function [28]. This relationship has been confirmed in self-reported olfactory behaviors [29], but the knowledge on this relationship in adolescents remains limited. To verify whether language development was related to olfactory performance in adolescents an initial letter verbal fluency test was included in this test part. Participants had to list as many words as possible beginning with the letter „b”within 60 seconds [30]. This method of testing language development relies on the assumption that words are organized in memory in first letter order [31]. Thus, performance in this task stems from ease in operating words. Additionally the initial letter verbal test is easy to understand for participants and confirmed to be reliable in studies involving children [30].

Results

To identify the effects of sex, age on odor identification performance and verbal fluency we used multivariate analysis of variance with sex and age as between-subject factors and verbal fluency, cued and uncued identification tasks scores as dependent variables. Analysis revealed a significant main effect of participants’ sex on results in uncued task F(1, 72) = 5.7, p < .05, η2 = .07 indicating higher performance of girls (M = 7.5, SD = 2.9) in comparison to boys (M = 6.3, SD = 2.9). No sex-related difference was observed for the cued task (p = .39). Similarly, a significant main effect of age was found on uncued identification F(1, 72) = 10.5, p < .01, η2 = .13. Pair-wise comparisons indicated higher abilities in free identification among older participants (M = 8.3, SD = 2.6) than younger ones (M = 6.2, SD = 2.9). No such difference was observed for the cued task (p = .29). There were no significant interaction effects of participants’ sex and age on results in any of the identification tasks (uncued: p = .21; cued: p = .23) (Fig 1). We found main effect of sex on verbal fluency F(1, 72) = 5.3, p < .05, η2 = .07, indicating higher scores among girls (M = 11.4, SD = 4.9) as compared to boys (M = 10.3, SD = 2.8). We observed that the verbal fluency increased with age F(1, 72) = 25.8, p < .001, η2 = .26. Older children performed significantly better (M = 13.7, SD = 5) in verbal fluency task than younger children (M = 9.3, SD = 2.4). For this reason we added verbal fluency as a covariate to the previously tested model. Within the new model, participants’ sex and age had no influence on the results in uncued or cued task and no interaction effect between participants’ sex and age was observed in case of uncued (p = .58) or cued identification (p = .24) Verbal fluency as a covariate had a significant effect on the uncued task score F(1, 71) = 6.9, p < .05, η2 = .09 and no effect on cued task score (p = .90). Correlation between uncued task score and verbal fluency was r = .47, p < .001.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Proportions of correct odor identifications across age and sex groups (in percentages).

Error bars illustrate the values of standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157560.g001

Personal Significance of Olfaction–Study 2

Participants

The sample in this study constituted 131 participants, aged from 10 to 18 years (M = 14.4, SD = 2.2). Of those, 85 were females between 10–17 years (M = 14.2, SD = 2.1) and 46 were male between 11–18 years (M = 14.7, SD = 2.3). Participants were divided into two age categories: 10–14 years (N = 82) and 15–18 years (N = 76). This study was conducted in boarding schools in Meissen and Dresden, Germany.

Testing procedure

The participants filled in a questionnaire assessing the importance of olfaction in their daily life. Items from Croy’s et. al. questionnaire [32] were modified to make them easier to understand for children. The questionnaire consists of the three subscales (each with six items): “Association”, which reflects to emotions, memories and evaluations that are triggered by olfactory experiences (e.g., ‘Certain smells immediately activate lots of memories.’); “Application” referring to the extent to which a person uses his or her sense of smell in everyday life (e.g., ‘‘For tomatoes or pineapples, I find it important how they smell.”) and “Consequence” that reflects the role of olfaction in daily decisions (e.g., ‘‘When I don´t like the smell of a soap, I don´t use it.”) [32]. Each statement in the questionnaire was rated on the scale ranging from 0—I totally disagree to 3—I totally agree (see: Table 1). Uncompleted items were removed from the data set. Data was transformed in the way, that the higher values indicated higher significance of olfaction. For each subscale summed score was calculated individually.

thumbnail
Table 1. Significance of olfaction questionnaire (modified after [32]).

Ass–Association; App–Application; Con–Consequence.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157560.t001

Results

In order to examine effects of age and gender on personal significance of olfaction we ran multivariate analysis of variance with age and gender as between-subject factors and results on Association, Application and Consequence subscales of the questionnaires as dependent variables. Analysis revealed significantly higher ratings for girls compared to boys on each of the questionnaire subscales: “Association” F(1, 127) = 15.6, p < .001, η2 = .11 “Application” F(1, 127) = 10.8, p < .002, η2 = .08 and “Consequence” F(1, 127) = 16.6, p < .001, η2 = .12. Significant main effect of age group on “Application” F(1, 127) = 6.3, p < .05, η2 = .5 was found (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Significance of olfaction across age and sex groups (in units).

Error bars illustrate the values of standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157560.g002

There were no significant interaction effects between age and sex on any of the three subscales (ps > .05). Descriptive statistics for all three subscales can be found in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for association, application and consequence (M: mean; SD: standard deviation; in arbitrary units; Min: minimum; Max: maximum).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157560.t002

Discussion

Results of the current study indicate that adolescent girls outperform boys in uncued odor identification tasks and that the abilities to name odorants without cues increase with age, which remains in line with former findings [33]. Our results confirmed the significant role of verbal fluency in such changes [16]. In line with these results on odor identification, we found that significance of olfaction in daily life, measured by questionnaire, was higher in girls than in boys and increased with age. This pattern of results indicate, that in adolescence odor identification performance and personal significance of olfaction develop together with cognitive abilities (as verbal fluency suggests) and daily life odor-related experiences gained with age, which are qualitatively different for girls and boys [34].

It seems that olfactory threshold scores stabilize at maximal point in the first decade of life [35], which is earlier in development than maximal identification performance, that follows around the second decade [18]. In line with former studies [24] we noted that in the second decade of life olfactory performance of male and female subjects of similar age was significantly different, in favor for the latter; and that also during adolescence development of olfactory abilities was observable [36], probably due to the older participants’ verbal abilities to name odors. However, the increase in odor identification was observed in uncued task, but not cued task.

Uncued odor identification requires some lexical knowledge and experience in categorizing abstract content [37,38] and hence has higher cognitive load on working memory compared to cued odor identification. It seems possible, that increased olfactory performance is related to higher memory categorization functions allowing to establish association between olfactory stimuli with its verbal descriptor. At the same time, growing mental capacities allow to operate with multiple associations of this kind (as the verbal fluency suggests) [28,39].

With regard to personal significance of olfaction, the young girls generally attached greater importance to olfaction than young boys and different odors affected their daily-life decisions to a greater extent. This finding is in line with adult questionnaire studies using similar [32] or different [40] instruments and a study conducted with 6–10 year old children [29] which indicate that females put more emphasis on olfaction from an early age on. We further observed a significant effect of age on the personal significance of olfaction in daily life context. Emotional association and application of olfaction in daily life increase during the second decade of life (compare: [16]), which is also time of increasing olfactory abilities. We assume the individual significance of olfaction it strongly linked to olfactory performance. It increases with development of olfactory performance and adolescence and it has been shown before that significance of olfaction deteriorates with disease related loss of olfactory functions [41].

In the current study no sex or age-related differences for cued identification task could be detected. In contrast to uncued task (where we observed sex-related differences), cued task does not require the ability to classify odors. Such ability might develop in girls together with their increased interest in odors. Alternatively cued identification task might have been easy for all the participants, regardless of their verbal fluency and odor-related experiences, however we did not observe the ceiling effect in this task (see: Fig 1).

A possible limitation of the current study might stem from the high level of individual differences in participants’ olfactory abilities, that can develop with different speed in puberty. The used age benchmark roughly reflects puberty, although there is high inter-individual and inter-sex variability, that was not controlled in this study.

Conclusion

Results of this study suggest that odor identification abilities develop with age and accompanying development of verbal fluency. Olfaction is subjectively perceived as more important among girls in comparison to boys.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: UWD IC TH. Performed the experiments: UWD PK FG. Analyzed the data: AO TH. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AO UWD IC TH. Wrote the paper: AO UWD.

References

  1. 1. Schaal B. Human Foetuses Learn Odours from their Pregnant Mother’s Diet. Chem Senses. 2000;25(6):729–37. pmid:11114151
  2. 2. Schaal B, Hummel T, Soussignan R. Olfaction in the fetal and premature infant: functional status and clinical implications. Clin Perinatol. 2004;31(2):261–85. pmid:15289032
  3. 3. Soussignan R, Schaal B, Marlier L, Jiang T. Facial and Autonomic Responses to Biological and Artificial Olfactory Stimuli in Human Neonates. Physiol Behav. 1997;62(4):745–58. pmid:9284493
  4. 4. Romantshik O, Porter RH, Tillmann V, Varendi H. Preliminary evidence of a sensitive period for olfactory learning by human newborns. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96(3):372–6. pmid:17407459
  5. 5. Varendi H, Porter R. Breast odour as the only maternal stimulus elicits crawling towards the odour source. Acta Paediatr. 2007;90(4):372–5.
  6. 6. Herz R. I know what I like: understanding odor preferences [Internet]. The Smell Culture Reader. 2006. Available from: http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Rachel_Herz/publication/239919178_I_KNOW_WHAT_I_LIKE_UNDERSTANDING_ODOR_PREFERENCES/links/00b49532af991c02ed000000.pdf
  7. 7. Khan RM, Luk C-H, Flinker A, Aggarwal A, Lapid H, Haddad R, et al. Predicting odor pleasantness from odorant structure: pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world. J Neurosci. 2007;27(37):10015–23. pmid:17855616
  8. 8. Schaal B. Olfaction in infants and children: developmental and functional perspectives. Chem Senses. 1988;13(2):145–90.
  9. 9. Delaunay-El Allam M, Marlier L, Schaal B. Learning at the breast: Preference formation for an artificial scent and its attraction against the odor of maternal milk. Infant Behav Dev. 2006;29(3):308–21. pmid:17138287
  10. 10. Schaal B, Marlier L, Soussignan R. Responsiveness to the Odour of Amniotic Fluid in the Human Neonate. Neonatology. Karger Publishers; 1995 Jul 1;67(6):397–406.
  11. 11. Doucet S, Soussignan R, Sagot P, Schaal B. The “smellscape” of mother’s breast: effects of odor masking and selective unmasking on neonatal arousal, oral, and visual responses. Dev Psychobiol. 2007;49(2):129–38. pmid:17299785
  12. 12. Rattaz C, Goubet N, Bullinger A. The calming effect of a familiar odor on full-term newborns. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26(2):86–92. pmid:15827459
  13. 13. Schaal B, Montagner H, Hertling E, Bolzoni D, Moyse A, Quichon R. Olfactory stimulation in the relationship between child and mother. Reprod Nutr Dev. 1980;20(3B):843–58. pmid:7349450
  14. 14. Engen T, Engen E. Relationship between development of odor perception and language. Enfance. Persée—Portail des revues scientifiques en SHS; 1997;50(1):125–40.
  15. 15. Engen T, Lipsitt LP, Kaye H. Olfactory responses and adaptation in the human neonate. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1963;56(1):73–7.
  16. 16. Monnery-Patris S, Rouby C, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S. Development of olfactory ability in children: sensitivity and identification. Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51(3):268–76. pmid:19130505
  17. 17. Hudson R. From molecule to mind: the role of experience in shaping olfactory function. J Comp Physiol A Sensory, Neural, Behav Physiol. 1999;185(4):297–304.
  18. 18. Sorokowska A, Schriever VA, Gudziol V, Hummel C, Hähner A, Iannilli E, et al. Changes of olfactory abilities in relation to age: odor identification in more than 1400 people aged 4 to 80 years. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngology. 2015;272(8):1937–44.
  19. 19. Doty R, Shaman P, Applebaum S, Giberson R, Siksorski L, Rosenberg L. Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science (80-). American Association for the Advancement of Science; 1984;226(4681):1441–3.
  20. 20. Chopra A, Baur A, Hummel T. Thresholds and chemosensory event-related potentials to malodors before, during, and after puberty: differences related to sex and age. Neuroimage. 2008;40(3):1257–63. pmid:18302986
  21. 21. Dorries KM, Schmidt HJ, Beauchamp GK, Wysocki CJ. Changes in sensitivity to the odor of androstenone during adolescence. Dev Psychobiol. 1989;22(5):423–35. pmid:2759357
  22. 22. Hummel T, Krone F, Lundström JN, Bartsch O. Androstadienone odor thresholds in adolescents. Horm Behav. 2005;47(3):306–10. pmid:15708759
  23. 23. Ferdenzi C, Schaal B, Roberts SC. Family scents: developmental changes in the perception of kin body odor? J Chem Ecol. 2010;36(8):847–54. pmid:20640943
  24. 24. Doty RL, Cameron EL. Sex differences and reproductive hormone influences on human odor perception. Physiol Behav. 2009;97(2):213–28. pmid:19272398
  25. 25. Hummel T, Rosenheim K, Konnerth C-G, Kobal G. Screening of olfactory function with a four-minute odor identification test: reliability, normative data, and investigations in patients with olfactory loss. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2001;110(10):976–81. pmid:11642433
  26. 26. Oleszkiewicz A, Taut M, Sorokowska A, Radwan A, Kamel R, Hummel T. Development of the Arabic version of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” odor identification test. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngology. 2015;273(5):1–6.
  27. 27. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G. “Sniffin” Sticks’: Olfactory Performance Assessed by the Combined Testing of Odour Identification, Odor Discrimination and Olfactory Threshold. Chem Senses. 1997;22(1):39–52. pmid:9056084
  28. 28. Hedner M, Larsson M, Arnold N, Zucco GM, Hummel T. Cognitive factors in odor detection, odor discrimination, and odor identification tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. Taylor & Francis Group; 2010;32(10):1062–7. pmid:20437286
  29. 29. Ferdenzi C, Coureaud G, Camos V, Schaal B. Human awareness and uses of odor cues in everyday life: Results from a questionnaire study in children. Int J Behav Dev. 2008;32(5):422–31.
  30. 30. Hurks PPM, Vles JSH, Hendriksen JGM, Kalff AC, Feron FJM, Kroes M, et al. Semantic category fluency versus initial letter fluency over 60 seconds as a measure of automatic and controlled processing in healthy school-aged children. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(5):684–95. pmid:16723317
  31. 31. Bryan J, Luszcz MA, Crawford JR. Verbal knowledge and speed of information processing as mediators of age differences in verbal fluency performance among older adults. Psychol Aging. 1997;12(3):473–8. pmid:9308094
  32. 32. Croy I, Buschhüter D, Seo H-S, Negoias S, Hummel T. Individual significance of olfaction: development of a questionnaire. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngology. 2010;267(1):67–71.
  33. 33. Dalton P, Mennella JA, Maute C, Castor SM, Silva-Garcia A, Slotkin J, et al. Development of a test to evaluate olfactory function in a pediatric population. Laryngoscope. 2011;121(9):1843–50. pmid:22024835
  34. 34. Lytton H, Romney DM. Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 1991;109(2):267–96.
  35. 35. Lehrner JP. Odor Identification, Consistency of Label Use, Olfactory Threshold and their Relationships to Odor Memory over the Human Lifespan. Chem Senses. 1999;24(3):337–46. pmid:10400452
  36. 36. Hummel T, Kobal G, Gudziol H, Mackay-Sim A. Normative data for the “Sniffin’Sticks” including tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds: an upgrade based on a group of more than. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngology. 2007;264:237–43.
  37. 37. Öberg C, Larsson M, Bäckamn L. Differential sex effects in olfactory functioning: The role of verbal processing. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;8(05):691–8.
  38. 38. Larsson M, Lövdén M, Nilsson L-G. Sex differences in recollective experience for olfactory and verbal information. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2003;112(1):89–103.
  39. 39. Larsson M, Finkel D, Pedersen NL. Odor Identification: Influences of Age, Gender, Cognition, and Personality. Journals Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2000;55(5):P304–10.
  40. 40. Martin GN, Apena F, Chaudry Z, Mulligan Z, Nixon C. The development of an attitudes towards the sense of smell questionnaire (SoSQ) and a comparison of different professions’ responses. N Am J Psychol. 2001;3(3):491–502.
  41. 41. Croy I, Landis BN, Meusel T, Seo H-S, Krone F, Hummel T. Patient adjustment to reduced olfactory function. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2011;137(4):377–82. pmid:21502477