Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Regional Brain Glucose Hypometabolism in Young Women with Polycystic Ovary Syndrome: Possible Link to Mild Insulin Resistance

  • Christian-Alexandre Castellano ,

    Alexandre.Castellano@USherbrooke.ca

    Affiliations Research Centre on Aging, Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Department of Pharmacology and Physiology, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Jean-Patrice Baillargeon,

    Affiliations Department of Medicine, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Research Center of the Centre hospitalier universitaire de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Scott Nugent,

    Affiliations Research Centre on Aging, Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Department of Pharmacology and Physiology, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Sébastien Tremblay,

    Affiliations Sherbrooke Molecular Imaging Center, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Department of Nuclear Medicine and Radiobiology, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Mélanie Fortier,

    Affiliation Research Centre on Aging, Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Hélène Imbeault,

    Affiliation Health and Social Sciences Center–Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Julie Duval,

    Affiliations Health and Social Sciences Center–Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Department of Neurology, Centre hospitalier universitaire de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

  • Stephen C. Cunnane

    Affiliations Research Centre on Aging, Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Department of Pharmacology and Physiology, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada, Department of Medicine, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

Abstract

Objective

To investigate whether cerebral metabolic rate of glucose (CMRglu) is altered in normal weight young women with polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS) who exhibit mild insulin resistance.

Materials and methods

Seven women with PCOS were compared to eleven healthy female controls of similar age, education and body mass index. Regional brain glucose uptake was quantified using FDG with dynamic positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging, and its potential relationship with insulin resistance assessed using the updated homeostasis model assessment (HOMA2-IR). A battery of cognitive tests was administered to evaluate working memory, attention and executive function.

Results

The PCOS group had 10% higher fasting glucose and 40% higher HOMA2-IR (p0.035) compared to the Controls. The PCOS group had 9–14% lower CMRglu in specific regions of the frontal, parietal and temporal cortices (p ≤ 0.018). A significant negative relation was found between the CMRglu and HOMA2-IR mainly in the frontal, parietal and temporal cortices as well as in the hippocampus and the amygdala (p ≤ 0.05). Globally, cognitive performance was normal in both groups but scores on the PASAT test of working memory tended to be low in the PCOS group.

Conclusions

The PCOS group exhibited a pattern of low regional CMRglu that correlated inversely with HOMA2-IR in several brain regions and which resembled the pattern seen in aging and early Alzheimer’s disease. These results suggest that a direct association between mild insulin resistance and brain glucose hypometabolism independent of overweight or obesity can exist in young adults in their 20s. Further investigation of the influence of insulin resistance on brain glucose metabolism and cognition in younger and middle-aged adults is warranted.

Introduction

There is growing experimental and clinical evidence that in addition to controlling glucose homeostasis in peripheral tissues such as liver, muscle and adipose tissue, insulin modulates brain function and energy metabolism [1]. In humans, a close relationship has been shown between plasma insulin and brain glucose uptake using positron emission tomography (PET) with the glucose analog, 18F-flurodeoxyglucose (FDG) [2,3,4]. The brain's high energy requirement makes it vulnerable to impaired brain glucose uptake or metabolism, so it is not surprising that insulin/glucose dysregulation could be associated with cognitive impairment [5]. Indeed, insulin resistance, type 2 diabetes and changes in brain glucose metabolism are emerging as central factors in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [6]. Craft’s group reported an AD-like pattern of brain glucose hypometabolism in cognitively normal 74 year olds with pre-diabetes or type 2 diabetes [7]. A recent study reported that insulin resistance measured by the homeostasis model assessment (HOMA-IR) was associated with reduced brain glucose metabolism at 60 years old [8]. Indeed, insulin resistance [9] and regional brain glucose hypometabolism [10] are also common during normal aging.

Our aim in the present study was to assess a condition in which the possible influence of mild insulin resistance on brain glucose metabolism could be studied independently of aging and obesity. Polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS) is an endocrine disorder involving infertility, hyperandrogenism, and insulin resistance in women [11,12]. Many but not all women with PCOS are overweight. Normal weight PCOS is therefore a model of insulin resistance in a younger adult population that is distinct from the confounding effects of aging and obesity. The primary aim of the present study was to determine whether normal weight women with PCOS had altered cerebral metabolic rate of glucose (CMRglu) in gray matter of brain regions associated with AD. The secondary aims were to determine whether differences in CMRglu in PCOS were associated with insulin resistance, or altered cognitive performance. PCOS is commonly associated with higher body-mass index (BMI) but we specifically selected cases with normal BMI in order to reduce the potential confounding effect of higher body weight on CMRglu [13].

Materials and Methods

Participants

PCOS (n = 7) was diagnosed during a clinical examination using the Rotterdam criteria [14]. Both the Control (n = 11) and PCOS groups had similar age, anthropometry and metabolic parameters and differed only in that the PCOS group had 10% higher fasting glucose and 40% higher HOMA2-IR (Table 1). None of the participants (PCOS or Control) had diabetes or was taking insulin-sensitizing drugs or medications for diabetes. All participants were current users of oral contraceptives. Exclusion criteria included drug addictions, major depression, psychiatric illness, smoking, or overt evidence of heart, liver or renal disease. This study was conducted with the written informed consent of all the participants and was approved by the appropriate ethics committees (Health and Social Services Center–Sherbrooke University Geriatrics Institute and the Centre hospitalier universitaire de Sherbrooke). This study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov with identification number NCT02409914.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of healthy age-matched young women (Control, n = 11) versus women with polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS, n = 7).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144116.t001

Cerebral brain glucose consumption with neuroimaging

For each participant, brain positron emission tomography (PET) images were obtained on a PET/CT scanner (Gemini TF, Philips Medical System, Eindhoven, The Netherlands). Briefly, after a fasting period of 6–7 h, each participant was positioned in the PET-scanner in the early afternoon in a dark quiet environment. After intravenous administration of 5.6 ± 0.6 mCuries of FDG via a forearm vein catheter, dynamic scans (field of view = 25 cm, axial field = 18 cm and 2 mm isotropic voxels) were obtained over a total duration of 60 min (time frames = 12 ☓ 10 sec, 8 ☓ 30 s sec, 6 ☓ 4 min, and 3 ☓ 10 min). Several blood samples were obtained to detect FDG radioactivity with a gamma counter (Cobra, Packard, United States) in order to determine the plasma time–activity curves required for the quantification of CMRglu expressed as μmoles/100 g/min. In the regional analysis of CMRglu, the brain was segmented into 74 regions as defined by Freesurfer parcellation labels (Freesurfer Suite 5.0). CMRglu was calculated according to the Patlak method [15]. All regional analyses of CMRglu were performed using tools implemented in PMOD 3.3 (PMOD Technologies Ltd., Zurich, Switzerland), as previously described [16]. Calculation of CMRglu included a magnetic resonance (MR)-based correction for partial volume effect and differences in brain volume.

Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging

As previously described [10], T1-weighted brain MR images (TR = 16.00 ms, TE = 4.68 ms, field of view = 256 ☓ 240 ☓192 mm, matrix size = 256 ☓ 256 ☓ 164, flip angle = 20 and 1 mm isotropic voxels) were obtained for each participant using a 1.5 Tesla scanner (Sonata, Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany). Regional and whole brain volumes were determined using FreeSurfer Suite 5.0 (Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Cambridge, MA, USA). Cortical brain anatomy was automatically parcellated into 33 regions of interest according to the Desikan-Killiany atlas [17]. Subcortical structures were segmented into 40 regions of interest [18].

Biochemical analysis

Most plasma parameters were measured using an automated clinical chemistry analyzer (Dimension Xpand Plus; Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics, Deerfield, IL, USA). Plasma total testosterone which was assayed by radioimmunoassay (Diagnostic Systems Laboratories, Webster, TX). Plasma insulin was analyzed by commercial ultra-sensitive enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (Alpco, Salem, NH, USA) with a Victor X4 multi-label plate reader (Perkin Elmer, Woodbridge, ON, Canada). The homeostasis model assessment computational method was used to estimate insulin resistance (HOMA2-IR) from fasting plasma glucose and insulin [19].

Cognitive tests

Evaluation of working memory and attention was based on performance on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) [20] and the Verbal Digit Span from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) [21]. The Trail Making and Stroop Color-Word interference tests from the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), and the Digit Symbol Substitution tests from the WAIS-III and Verbal fluency provided information on executive functions and processing speed [21,22,23]. The 16-item Free and Cued Recall (RL/RI-16) is an episodic memory test [24] providing information about the mechanisms of encoding and retrieval/consolidation processes. Cognitive impairment was defined as score more than 1.65 standard deviations (SD) below the normative values for age and education [25]. Composite Z-scores for each cognitive domain were calculated as the mean of all Z-scores for each individual within cognitive domain.

Statistical methods

Data are presented as mean ± SD. All statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS 17.0 software (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). The Mann–Whitney U-test was used for comparisons between the two groups with a statistical threshold of p≤0.05. All regional imaging data including CMRglu comparisons underwent a p ≤ 0.05 false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons. Linear regression modeling was used to test whether HOMA2-IR was associated with CMRglu.

Results

Fasting glucose and HOMA2-IR were significantly higher in the PCOS group compared to Controls (p0.035; Table 1). The PCOS group also had about 4 times higher plasma testosterone than normal and one year less education than the Controls (p0.02).

Cerebral glucose consumption (CMRglu)

Compared to Controls, CMRglu in gray matter as a whole was not significantly different but tended to be lower in the PCOS group (34.9 ± 3.0 vs. 38.0 ± 4.4 μmol/100 g/min; p = 0.090, FDR-corrected). In the regional analysis, the PCOS group had significantly lower CMRglu in the right superior frontal cortex (-12%; p = 0.018), left middle frontal cortex (-12%; p = 0.006), right middle frontal cortex (-13%; p = 0.018), left supramarginal cortex (-12%; p = 0.006) and left middle temporal cortex (-9%; p = 0.006) (Table 2; Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Brain regions with significantly lower glucose uptake in the PCOS compared to the Control group.

All results were FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons (p ≤ 0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144116.g001

thumbnail
Table 2. Cerebral metabolic rate of glucose (μmol/100 g/min; mean ± SD) in selected brain regions in healthy young women (Control, n = 11) and women with polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS, n = 7).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144116.t002

Regional brain volume and cortical thickness

In comparison to the controls, the PCOS group had 10–17% lower volume of several brain regions, mainly in the frontal and parietal cortex (all p ≤ 0.035 FDR-corrected): 10% smaller left superior frontal cortex (22.6 ± 2.0 vs. 25.0 ± 2.5 ml; p = 0.022), 14% smaller left supramarginal cortex (10.3 ± 1.4 vs. 12.0 ± 1.1 ml; p = 0.008) and 10% smaller right superior parietal cortex (13.1 ± 1.8 vs. 14.7 ± 2.2 ml; p = 0.035). Total intracranial volume, ventricle volume and cortical thickness were not different between the two groups (p = 0.075, p = 0.123, and p ≥ 0.105, respectively; FDR-corrected).

Cognitive profile of the PCOS group

Globally, the PCOS group did not show any clinically significant cognitive impairment, i.e. no one scored ≥1.65 SD below the normal range for age and education (Table 3). However, on the PASAT, two PCOS participants showed a clinically relevant deficit with a Z-score ± SD of -1.67 ± 0.06, and three others underperformed on this test (Z-score ± SD of -0.89 ± 0.08). On the delayed recall test from the RL/RI-16, five PCOS participants showed a sub-normal score (Z-score ± SD of -1.13 ± 0.61). Composite Z-scores were lower in working memory and episodic memory than in executive functioning (mean composite z-scores ± SD of -0.39 ± 0.47, -0.49 ± 0.39 and +0.38 ±0.52, respectively; Table 3). No correlation was found between insulin resistance (HOMA2-IR) and the different cognitive outcomes (p ≥ 0.151; data not shown).

thumbnail
Table 3. Cognitive test scores (mean ± SD) in women with polycystic ovary syndrome (n = 7) compared to the normal range.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144116.t003

Relationship between CMRglu and HOMA2-IR

CMRglu in several cortical regions and in the hippocampus and amygdala was significantly inversely related to higher HOMA2-IR (Table 4; Fig 2). Fasting plasma glucose was negatively associated with CMRglu in the middle temporal cortex and caudate nucleus (p ≤ 0.03; data not show). The potential relationship between plasma total testosterone and CMRglu in the PCOS group was assessed but no significant correlation was found between these two parameters (p ≥ 0.150; data not shown).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Scatter plot of calculated insulin resistance based on homeostatic model assessment (HOMA2-IR) and average cerebral metabolic rate of glucose (CMRglu; μmol/100 g/min) in AD-vulnerable brain regions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144116.g002

thumbnail
Table 4. Relationship between insulin resistance (HOMA2-IR) and cerebral metabolic rate of glucose (CMRglu) in the control and PCOS groups combined.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144116.t004

Discussion

Our main observations are that in comparison with healthy women of similar age, education and BMI, women with PCOS had lower CMRglu in the frontal, parietal and temporal cortex, and that HOMA2-IR was significantly inversely associated with CMRglu in several brain regions. Our findings add young women with PCOS to previous reports of regional glucose hypometabolism in the parietal and temporal regions in middle-aged and older people with insulin resistance as defined by the HOMA-IR [7,8,26,27,28].

Insulin resistance is a well-recognised feature of PCOS [29]. In the present study, the women with PCOS had significant lower insulin sensitivity as determined by the HOMA2-IR. Few human studies have examined brain glucose uptake in an insulin-resistant but non-diabetic population. Higher HOMA-IR was associated with lower CMRglu in frontal, parieto-temporal, and cingulate regions in adults with pre-diabetes; a relationship that was not affected by age [7]. In a large late to middle-aged cohort (61 ± 6 y), Bedlin and collaborators also observed that HOMA-IR was associated with lower brain glucose uptake in the frontal, temporal and parietal regions [27]. In AD, higher HOMA-IR predicts lower brain glucose uptake in the lateral parietal, medial temporal, and prefrontal regions and hippocampus [26]. We observed a significant negative association between fasting glucose and CMRglu in the middle temporal cortex, a region involved in auditory processing and language, and in the caudate, which is part of the motor circuit and is related to body’s voluntary movements. Our results agree with a recent study showing a negative relationship between serum glucose and CMRglu in the primary auditory and motor cortices of non-diabetic young adults aged 28 ± 8 y [30].

Globally, our PCOS group did not show any clinically significant cognitive impairment. However, five of the seven participants underperformed on the PASAT, an indication of potential cognitive difficulties in working memory. Some PCOS participants also had low-normal scores for episodic memory retrieval according to results from the delayed recall test and as confirmed by low-normal composite cognitive Z-scores. Several PET studies in normal aging have reported a relation between deficits in these cognitive domains and functional changes in the prefrontal cortex [31]. The frontal cortex commonly exhibits 10–15% lower CMRglu during aging, a deficit that spreads to the parietal and temporal cortices early in AD [10,16,32,33,34]. Higher HOMA-IR was recently reported to be associated with both lower brain glucose metabolism in left medial temporal lobe and worse memory performance in adults aged 61 ± 6 y [28]. Schattmann and collaborators observed that 28 y old women with PCOS had lower scores in the backward span of the block tapping test, a measure of visuospatial working memory [35]. Women with PCOS may also have impaired performance in other cognitive domains, including reaction time, processing speed [36], visuospatial ability and executive function [37]. Future studies on PCOS should include a cognitive battery that includes an extensive evaluation of attention, working memory and executive function (e.g. mental rotation test, TAP Divided Attention, etc.) and episodic memory retrieval and consolidation (e.g. 24 h-delay recall test). More complex tests should be used in order to avoid any potential ceiling effect in episodic memory retrieval. In the present study, no statistically significant relationship was found between insulin resistance and the different cognitive domains evaluated. However, others have found that HOMA-IR is negatively correlated to executive function in healthy women aged 35 [38] and 45 years [39].

The present study was controlled for BMI and age, both of which negatively influence glucose uptake by the brain [40,41,42]. Our dynamic FDG-PET protocol combined with volumetric MRI acquisition and atrophy correction allowed for more accurate quantification of brain glucose uptake [10,43]. Nevertheless, this study had several limitations, notably small sample size and mild insulin resistance, i.e. all HOMA2-IR scores were <1.5. Although insulin resistance is widely accepted to play a key role in the pathogenesis of PCOS [11,29] and its prevalence is around 64% in women with PCOS [44], our PCOS group was not recruited on the basis of a particular threshold of HOMA2-IR but rather on the basis of being normal weight which is uncommon in PCOS but was important so as to avoid overweight or obesity as a confounder. Fulghesu and collaborators indicated that “a normal HOMA score is not sufficient to exclude early metabolic abnormalities such as hyperinsulinemia in young lean PCOS subjects” [45]. A previous study using the glucose-insulin clamp technique reported lower insulin sensitivity in women with PCOS [46]; in future, this would be the best way to characterize the degree of insulin resistance in women with PCOS. The PCOS group had higher testosterone levels and hyperandrogenism, both of which could potentially modulate both insulin sensitivity [47] and brain glucose metabolism [48]. No correlation was found between plasma total testosterone and CMRglu in the PCOS group but considering our small sample size, we could not adequately assess a potential effect of hypertestosteronemia on brain glucose uptake in this group.

Conclusions

Our results show that untreated normal weight women with PCOS had lower regional brain glucose uptake in a pattern that resembles that seen in older persons and, to a lesser extent, in early AD. Thus, mild insulin resistance and hyperandrogenism can be associated with regional brain glucose hypometabolism in young adults in their 20s. There was a non-significant trend towards potential cognitive difficulties that specifically involve working memory in the PCOS group. Further investigation and replication of the influence of mild insulin resistance on brain glucose metabolism and cognition in younger and middle-aged adults is warranted.

Acknowledgments

Excellent assistance was provided by Conrad Filteau, Marie-Maxime Lavallée, Éric Lavallée, and the staff of the clinical PET/MRI and the Sherbrooke Molecular Imaging Centre (CIMS).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CAC JPB SCC. Performed the experiments: CAC HI JD. Analyzed the data: CAC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CAC. Wrote the paper: CAC JPB SCC. Contributed to writing and revising the manuscript: SN MF ST HI JD.

References

  1. 1. Blázquez E, Velázquez E, Hurtado-Carneiro V, Ruiz-Albusac JM (2014) Insulin in the brain: its pathophysiological implications for states related with central insulin resistance, type 2 diabetes and alzheimer’s disease. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 5: 1–21.
  2. 2. Bingham EM, Hopkins D, Smith D, Pernet A, Hallett W, Reed L, et al. (2002) The role of insulin in human brain glucose metabolism: An 18fluoro-deoxyglucose positron emission tomography study. Diabetes 51: 3384–3390. pmid:12453890
  3. 3. Hirvonen J, Virtanen KA, Nummenmaa L, Hannukainen JC, Honka MJ, Bucci M, et al. (2011) Effects of insulin on brain glucose metabolism in impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes 60: 443–447. pmid:21270256
  4. 4. Craft S, Baker LD, Montine TJ, Minoshima S, Watson GS, Claxton A, et al. (2012) Intranasal insulin therapy for Alzheimer disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: A pilot clinical trial. Arch Neurol 69: 29–38. pmid:21911655
  5. 5. Cunnane S, Nugent S, Roy M, Courchesne-Loyer A, Croteau E, Tremblay S, et al. (2011) Brain fuel metabolism, aging, and Alzheimer's disease. Nutrition 27: 3–20. pmid:21035308
  6. 6. Bosco D, Fava A, Plastino M, Montalcini T, Pujia A (2011) Possible implications of insulin resistance and glucose metabolism in Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis. J Cell Mol Med 15: 1807–1821. pmid:21435176
  7. 7. Baker LD, Cross DJ, Minoshima S, Belongia D, Stennis Watson G, Craft S (2011) Insulin resistance and alzheimer-like reductions in regional cerebral glucose metabolism for cognitively normal adults with prediabetes or early type 2 diabetes. Arch Neurol 68: 51–57. pmid:20837822
  8. 8. García-Casares N, Berthier ML, Jorge RE, Gonzalez-Alegre P, Gutiérrez Cardo A, Rioja Villodres J, et al. (2014) Structural and functional brain changes in middle-aged type 2 diabetic patients: A cross-sectional study. J Alzheimers Dis 40: 375–386. pmid:24448784
  9. 9. Rowe JW, Minaker KL, Pallotta JA, Flier JS (1983) Characterization of the insulin resistance of aging. J Clin Invest 71: 1581–1587. pmid:6345585
  10. 10. Nugent S, Castellano C, Goffaux P, Whittingstall K, Lepage M, Paquet N, et al. (2014) Glucose hypometabolism is highly localized but lower cortical thickness and brain atrophy are widespread in cognitively normal older adults. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 306: E1315–1321. pmid:24735889
  11. 11. Dunaif A (1997) Insulin resistance and the polycystic ovary syndrome: mechanism and implications for pathogenesis. Endocr Rev 18: 774–800. pmid:9408743
  12. 12. Dunaif A, Segal KR, Shelley DR, Green G, Dobrjansky A, Licholai T (1992) Evidence for distinctive and intrinsic defects in insulin action in polycystic ovary syndrome. Diabetes 41: 1257–1266. pmid:1397698
  13. 13. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Goldstein RZ, Alia-Klein N, et al. (2009) Inverse association between BMI and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obesity 17: 60–65. pmid:18948965
  14. 14. The Rotterdam ESHRE/ASRM-Sponsored PCOS Consensus Workshop Group (2004) Revised 2003 consensus on diagnostic criteria and long-term health risks related to polycystic ovary syndrome. Fertil Steril 81: 19–25.
  15. 15. Patlak CS, Blasberg RG, Fenstermacher JD (1983) Graphical evaluation of blood-to-brain transfer constants from multiple-time uptake data. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 3: 1–7. pmid:6822610
  16. 16. Castellano C, Nugent S, Paquet N, Tremblay S, Bocti C, Lacombe G, et al. (2015) Lower brain 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose uptake but normal 11C-acetoacetate metabolism in mild Alzheimer’s disease dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 43: 1343–1353. pmid:25147107
  17. 17. Fischl B, Van Der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Ségonne F, Salat DH, et al. (2004) Automatically Parcellating the Human Cerebral Cortex. Cereb Cortex 14: 11–22. pmid:14654453
  18. 18. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, et al. (2002) Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33: 341–355. pmid:11832223
  19. 19. Levy JC, Matthews DR, Hermans MP (1998) Correct homeostasis model assessment (HOMA) evaluation uses the computer program. Diabetes Care 21: 2191–2192. pmid:9839117
  20. 20. Gronwall DMA (1977) Paced auditory serial addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Mot Skills 44: 367–373. pmid:866038
  21. 21. Wechsler D (1997) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  22. 22. Delis D, Kaplan E, Kramer J (2001) Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). In: Corporation P, editor. San Antonio, TX (USA).
  23. 23. Cardebat D, Doyon B, Puel M, Goulet P, Joanette Y (1990) Formal and semantic lexical evocation in normal subjects. Performance and dynamics of production as a function of sex, age and educational level. Acta Neurol Belg 90: 207–217. pmid:2124031
  24. 24. Van der Linden M, Coyette F, Poitrenaud J, GREMEM (2004) L’épreuve de Rappel Libre/Rappel Indicé à 16 items (RL/RI-16). In: Van der Linden SA M., & Agniel A., editor. L’évaluation des troubles de la mémoire épisodique (avec leur étalonnage). Marseille (France): Solal. pp. 25–47.
  25. 25. Poitrenaud J (1995) Les évaluations psychométriques. In: Eustache F, Agniel A, editors. Neuropsychologie clinique des démences: évaluation et prises en charge. Marseille (France): Solal.
  26. 26. Willette A, Modanlob N, Kapogiannis D, the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, (2015) Insulin resistance predicts medial temporal hypermetabolism in MCI conversion to Alzheimer’s disease. Diabetes 64: 1933–1940. pmid:25576061
  27. 27. Bendlin B, Birdsill A, Willette A, Johnson S, Christian B, Oh J, et al. (2013) Midlife insulin resistance is associated with hypometabolism on FDG-PET. Alzheimers Dement 9: 411–412.
  28. 28. Willette AA, Bendlin BB, Starks EJ, Birdsill AC, Johnson SC, Christian BT, et al. (2015) Association of Insulin Resistance With Cerebral Glucose Uptake in Late Middle-Aged Adults at Risk for Alzheimer Disease. JAMA neurology 72: 1013–1020. pmid:26214150
  29. 29. Moran J, Misso M, Wild R, Norman R (2010) Impaired glucose tolerance, type 2 diabetes and metabolic syndrome in polycystic ovary syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Hum Reprod Update 16: 347–363. pmid:20159883
  30. 30. Marano CM, Workman CI, Lyman CH, Kramer E, Hermann CR, Ma Y, et al. (2014) The relationship between fasting serum glucose and cerebral glucose metabolism in late-life depression and normal aging. Psychiatry Res 222: 84–90. pmid:24650451
  31. 31. Rajah MN, D'Esposito M (2005) Region-specific changes in prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. Brain: a journal of neurology 128: 1964–1983.
  32. 32. Dukart J, Mueller K, Villringer A, Kherif F, Draganski B, Frackowiak R, et al. (2013) Relationship between imaging biomarkers, age, progression and symptom severity in Alzheimer's disease. Neuroimage Clin 3: 84–94. pmid:24179852
  33. 33. Mosconi L (2005) Brain glucose metabolism in the early and specific diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: FDG-PET studies in MCI and AD. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 32: 486–510. pmid:15747152
  34. 34. Nugent S, Castellano C, Bocti B, Dionne I, Fulop T, Cunnane S (2015) Relationship of metabolic and endocrine parameters to brain glucose metabolism in older people: Does the cognitively-normal older person have a particular metabolic phenotype? Biogerontology: in press.
  35. 35. Schattmann L, Sherwin BB (2007) Testosterone levels and cognitive functioning in women with polycystic ovary syndrome and in healthy young women. Horm Behav 51: 587–596. pmid:17433328
  36. 36. Barnard L, Balen AH, Ferriday D, Tiplady B, Dye L (2007) Cognitive functioning in polycystic ovary syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32: 906–914. pmid:17659845
  37. 37. Barry JA, Parekh HSK, Hardiman PJ (2013) Visual-spatial cognition in women with polycystic ovarian syndrome: The role of androgens. Hum Reprod 28: 2832–2837. pmid:23945597
  38. 38. Bove RM, Brick DJ, Healy BC, Mancuso SM, Gerweck AV, Bredella MA, et al. (2013) Metabolic and endocrine correlates of cognitive function in healthy young women. Obesity 21: 1343–1349. pmid:23671055
  39. 39. Schuur M, Henneman P, Van Swieten JC, Zillikens MC, De Koning I, Janssens ACJW, et al. (2010) Insulin-resistance and metabolic syndrome are related to executive function in women in a large family-based study. Eur J Epidemiol 25: 561–568. pmid:20585974
  40. 40. Hu Y, Xu Q, Li K, Zhu H, Qi R, Zhang Z, et al. (2013) Gender differences of brain glucose metabolic networks revealed by FDG-PET: Evidence from a large cohort of 400 young adults. PLoS ONE 8.
  41. 41. Abete I, Parra D, Crujeiras AB, Goyenechea E, Martínez JA (2008) Specific insulin sensitivity and leptin responses to a nutritional treatment of obesity via a combination of energy restriction and fatty fish intake. J Hum Nutr Diet 21: 591–600. pmid:18759956
  42. 42. Duara R, Grady C, Haxby J (1984) Human brain glucose utilization and cognitive function in relation to age. Ann Neurol 16: 702–713.
  43. 43. Buvat I (2007) Understanding the limitations of SUV. Medecine Nucleaire 31: 165–172.
  44. 44. DeUgarte CM, Bartolucci AA, Azziz R (2005) Prevalence of insulin resistance in the polycystic ovary syndrome using the homeostasis model assessment. Fertil Steril 83: 1454–1460. pmid:15866584
  45. 45. Fulghesu AM, Angioni S, Portoghese E, Milano F, Batetta B, Paoletti AM, et al. (2006) Failure of the homeostatic model assessment calculation score for detecting metabolic deterioration in young patients with polycystic ovary syndrome. Fertility and Sterility 86: 398–404. pmid:16769061
  46. 46. Rabasa-Lhoret R, Bastard JP, Jan V, Ducluzeau PH, Andreelli F, Guebre F, et al. (2003) Modified Quantitative Insulin Sensitivity Check Index Is Better Correlated to Hyperinsulinemic Glucose Clamp than Other Fasting-Based Index of Insulin Sensitivity in Different Insulin-Resistant States. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 88: 4917–4923. pmid:14557474
  47. 47. Corbould A (2008) Effects of androgens on insulin action in women: Is androgen excess a component of female metabolic syndrome? Diabetes Metab Res Rev 24: 520–532. pmid:18615851
  48. 48. Miller KK, Deckersbach T, Rauch SL, Fischman AJ, Grieco KA, Herzog DB, et al. (2004) Testosterone administration attenuates regional brain hypometabolism in women with anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res 132: 197–207. pmid:15664791