Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Describing the Relationship between Cat Bites and Human Depression Using Data from an Electronic Health Record

Abstract

Data mining approaches have been increasingly applied to the electronic health record and have led to the discovery of numerous clinical associations. Recent data mining studies have suggested a potential association between cat bites and human depression. To explore this possible association in more detail we first used administrative diagnosis codes to identify patients with either depression or bites, drawn from a population of 1.3 million patients. We then conducted a manual chart review in the electronic health record of all patients with a code for a bite to accurately determine which were from cats or dogs. Overall there were 750 patients with cat bites, 1,108 with dog bites, and approximately 117,000 patients with depression. Depression was found in 41.3% of patients with cat bites and 28.7% of those with dog bites. Furthermore, 85.5% of those with both cat bites and depression were women, compared to 64.5% of those with dog bites and depression. The probability of a woman being diagnosed with depression at some point in her life if she presented to our health system with a cat bite was 47.0%, compared to 24.2% of men presenting with a similar bite. The high proportion of depression in patients who had cat bites, especially among women, suggests that screening for depression could be appropriate in patients who present to a clinical provider with a cat bite. Additionally, while no causative link is known to explain this association, there is growing evidence to suggest that the relationship between cats and human mental illness, such as depression, warrants further investigation.

Introduction

The adoption of electronic health records (EHRs) and consequent storage of large quantities of medical data in a computable format has opened the possibility for new discoveries using information that was captured for routine clinical care. Data mining approaches that use powerful data analysis algorithms to unlock hidden patterns in large datasets have been increasingly applied to EHRs. [1] Using such techniques has opened the possibility for uncovering relationships that may ultimately lead to improved health [2], [3].

Data mining the electronic health record has uncovered many potential disease associations,[4][9] as well as associations between other aspects of the clinical record including medications and laboratory values. [10] Others have mined EHR data to predict mortality, [11] to detect adverse drug events, [12], [13] and to identify disease-gene associations [14].

In 2009 we reported on an association analysis of clinical concepts that leveraged a tool initially developed for identifying novel gene expressions patterns. [5] That analysis revealed many significant associations among 327,000 patients using free text diagnoses in the problem list of an EHR. While many of the associations were well-known, one intriguing and previously unknown association discovered was between the terms ‘cat bite’ and ‘depression’. The analysis was complicated by the unstructured and unconstrained approach that clinicians used in entering diagnoses.

A follow-up study from 2012 applied a similar analytic approach to a de-identified dataset containing 41.2 million international classification of disease, version 9 (ICD-9) codes from 1.6 million patients. [6] While a specific ICD-9 code exists for dog bites (ICD E906.0), there is no specific code for cat bites. However, one code in particular (ICD E906.3) is most often used to describe cat bites. We again found an association between depression (ICD 311) and animal bites (ICD E906.3), with most of the bites presumably from cats. This additional finding from a much larger dataset suggested that the association between depression and bites was not likely due to chance alone. However, with only a generic code for a variety of animal bites, and no other patient demographic information, little more could be discerned from the data.

Here we report on an extension of the findings from our recent study of ICD-9 codes. [6] Our main objective was to better describe the relationship between cat bites and depression. While the focus of this analysis was on cat bites, we also included dog bites for comparison. We conducted a retrospective chart review of patient records to explore this previously unreported association. We also discuss the potential implications from our findings. By doing so we demonstrate that data mining approaches applied to electronic health records are capable of uncovering novel relationships that can be validated with further study leveraging the data already captured in the EHR.

Methods

The University of Michigan Health System has had an EHR since 1998, including free text clinical documents created by clinicians through dictation and transcription or by directly typing them into the system. [15] A separate health system data warehouse (HSDW) database contains administrative data such ICD-9 billing codes as well as coded patient demographics. From our institution’s HSDW we identified all patients who were ≥18 years of age with one of twenty-six possible ICD-9 codes for depression (Table 1). We also used the HSDW to identify all patients ≥18 years of age with non-venomous animal bites or injuries. Such bites are represented by the codes E906.X, where X is any of eight categories (Table 2). Venomous animal bites, including bites from certain snakes and lizards, with ICD codes E905.X were not considered in this analysis. We then determined which patients had both depression and a history of a bite. We also characterized the population of patients in terms of age and gender.

thumbnail
Table 1. ICD-9 codes used to identify cases of depression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070585.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Patients identified with animal bites and injuries based on the E906.X codes, representing non-venomous animal bites and injuries.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070585.t002

From this cohort of patients with both depression and an animal bite or injury we then conducted a chart review, using a medical record search engine, [16][19] of all patients who had any of the E906.X billing codes to determine who had experienced a cat bite. Patients whose documentation described only a cat scratch were not considered to have had a bite. Variations of “bite” such as “bitten” and “bit” were used in the search. Dog bites were found using the same approach.

For patients with a documented cat bite, we recorded the type of relationship between the cat and the bite recipient (i.e, the patient). These were grouped into four categories: (1) Bites from the recipient’s own cat; (2) Bites from the cat of an acquaintance such as a neighbor, friend, or other family member. Bites sustained at a workplace such as a veterinarian’s office were also included in this category; (3) Bites from a stray or feral cat; and (4) Bites where the specific relationship was not mentioned.

Among those patients with either a cat or dog bite we also used the search engine to look through their clinical notes for any social history mentioning if the patient lived alone, suggesting the possibility of social isolation. This was done because nearly 27% of U.S. households (and 28% of Michigan households) are comprised of adults living alone. [20] For this we used the terms “lives” or “living” combined with “alone”, “by himself”, “by herself”, “on his own”, and “on her own”.

All statistical tests were conducted using R for Mac OS X version 2.13.2. Differences in proportions were calculated using the ‘2-sample test for equality of proportions with continuity correction. The University of Michigan Medical School’s institutional review board approved this study, along with a waiver of informed consent. It was determined that the study represented “no more than minimal risk”.

Results

Out of a total base population of 1.3 million patients, we identified 116,922 patients ≥18 years old with a diagnosis of depression, and 3,018 unique patients who had an animal bite or injury represented by the code E906.X. The most common ICD-9 code for depression was ICD 311, representing 91,258 patients, followed by ICD 296.30 (15,557 patients) and ICD 296.20 (15,252 patients). Tables 1 and 2 report the number of patients per ICD code.

Table 2 also describes a more detailed version of the ICD E906.X patients identified through the chart review. The most common injury code was ICD E906.0 (dog bite) for which there were 1,087 patients, followed by ICD E906.3 (including cat bites and other animals) for which there were 866 patients. Three patients in the dog bite category were misclassified and only had a cat bite, with an additional 23 having had both dog and cat bites. Likewise, there were five patients in the ICD E906.3 category that only had dog bites.

The most common types of bites in the ICD E906.3 category were cats (n = 701, 80.9%), squirrels (n = 45, 5.2%), bats (n = 20, 2.3%) and raccoons (n = 17, 2.0%), although there were a wide variety of animals mentioned in the clinical notes including moles, monkeys, and mice as well as parrots, pigs, piranhas, and prairie dogs. While ICD E906.3 is the traditionally accepted code for cat bites, we identified an additional 49 patients who had experienced a cat bite but were coded in a different ICD E906.X category. Likewise, we identified an additional 27 patients with dog bites that were not coded in the ICD E906.0 category.

Depression rates are shown in Table 3. Among the population of adults with depression, nearly two-thirds were women, although some of this difference may be attributable to more women (54.8%) than men (45.2%) seeking care at the health system. The highest depression rate was for patients who had both a dog bite and a cat bite, with nearly half (47.8%) having depression, all of them women. However, the number of patients with both bites in that category was low (n = 23). Among all patients with a cat bite (n = 750), 41.3% had a diagnosis of depression. This represents a significantly higher rate of depression than the 8.8% rate observed in the general population of adult patients (p<2.2×10−16).

The 41.3% depression rate among those with a cat bite is also higher than those who received any kind of bite or injury other than a cat (28.5%, p = 8.7×10−11), including dog bites (28.7%, p = 2.2×10−8). In fact, the proportion of men and women with depression having dog bites closely mirrored the gender proportion of depression in the general population, whereas this was not the case with cat bites. Among the 310 patients who had a cat bite and depression, 85.5% were women. This compares to 64.5% women among those who had a dog bite and depression, which is similar to the overall percentage of women in the depression population (65.3%). To state this differently, based on over 10 years of data, if a woman presented to our health system with a cat bite that was serious enough to be coded as such, there was a 47.0% chance that she will also be given a diagnosis of depression at some point in her life. For men, about half as many (24.2%) had depression. By contrast, the gender difference was still present but smaller for dog bites: if a woman presented with a dog bite, there was a 35.8% chance of having depression compared to 21.1% of men.

Regarding living situation, 204 out of 750 (27.2%) patients with cat bites were living alone compared 178 out of 1,108 (16.1%) patients with dog bites (p = 8.0×10−9). However, among those living alone there was no significant difference in depression rates, with 125/204 (61.3%) of cat bite patients and 104/178 (58.4%) of dog bite patients living alone having a diagnosis of depression (p = 0.64).

Age was not a major factor in our results, with very little difference in ages among those with or without depression, or by gender. Figure 1 shows a histogram displaying the ages of the patients who experienced a cat bite, divided into those with and without depression. More than half (58.5%) of the patients who had a cat bite were under the age of 50. The most common decade of life for patients to have experienced a cat bite were for patients age 40–49 (n = 163), followed by 50–59 (n = 156), 30–39 (n = 139), and 20–29 (n = 128). Notably, there were far fewer patients ages 60–69 (n = 76) with a cat bite, and even fewer bites among the older patients.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Stacked bar chart showing age versus total number of patients, for 750 patients with cat bites.

Dark blue bars represent patients with depression and light blue bars represent patients without depression. Ages are rounded to the nearest 5-year period.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070585.g001

Because some patients may have presented to the health system initially with a bite and only later were diagnosed with depression, we also looked at the temporal occurrence of bites with respect to depression diagnoses, shown in Table 4. There was a slightly significant difference in the proportion of patients being diagnosed with depression first. Whereas about a quarter (27.1%) of patients with cat bites and depression first had a cat bite followed by depression, about a third (36.2%) of dog bite patients had a bite that preceded the depression (p = 0.02).

thumbnail
Table 4. Temporal relationships among the time of first diagnosis of depression and cat or dog bites.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070585.t004

The majority (58.8%) of bites in our study were inflicted by the patients’ own cat, including 56.1% among those who had depression (Table 5). Being bitten by a stray or feral cat was least common (15.7% of all bites, and 15.2% of bites among those with depression). However, among those with depression who had bites from stray or feral cats, women far outranked men (93.6% vs 6.4%, respectively).

thumbnail
Table 5. Background of the biting cat, categorized by gender and depression of the human bite recipient.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070585.t005

Discussion

Depression remains a significant public health concern, has been linked to increased mortality, and is predicted to be one of the three leading causes of illness worldwide by 2030. [21] In the United States depression remains one of the top causes of disability, especially among females. [22] The national prevalence of 12-month and lifetime major depression, has been estimated to be 8.1% and 18.6%, respectively. [23] Yet, depression treatment can yield reasonable response rates if adequate follow-up occurs [24][26].

Screening interventions to detect depression, although somewhat controversial, [27][30] are often advised. [28], [31], [32] A small study from 1995 reported that primary care physicians may miss major depression in their patients up to 40% of the time. [33] Current detection rates might be higher in settings such as the Veterans Health Administration where yearly depression screening is now a requirement, [34] although routine depression screening is not universally done elsewhere [35].

While universal depression screening might be ideal, such screening will likely not occur in all medical settings, and targeted screening may be preferable with limited time and resources. [36] It may seem counterintuitive to consider screening for depression in someone who presents to a clinician with an acute injury from a household pet, but our findings suggest that it could be beneficial. Table 6 represents a reformulated subset of data from Tables 3 and 5 and reports on the probability of having depression given a patient’s gender and type of bite. These data demonstrate that depression seems to be higher in all patients with bites compared to the general population. Additionally, it suggest that cat bites, especially in women, might serve as a warning sign for depression. Depression has been identified in other populations using various criteria for detection. For example, it has been suggested that adolescents presenting to the emergency department with non-specific somatic complaints such as chest pain or headaches could have depression, [37] which should prompt further screening. [38] Similarly, patients presenting to the emergency department or cardiology clinic with chest pain may actually have a panic disorder. [39], [40] In our study population, the quarter of patients with cat bites that preceded their depression and the third of patients with dog bites that preceded their depression potentially represent a population of patients for which screening at the time of the bite might have detected the depression sooner. The literature is already replete with reviews that alert physicians to consider infections when patients present with cat bites, [41][43] so it may be reasonable that depression also be considered.

thumbnail
Table 6. Percent probability of having depression given the following conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070585.t006

Our study is not the first to have reported on the high prevalence of cat bites in women.[44][48] Several studies have reported an approximately 2-to-1 ratio of women to men with respect to cat bites, [49][52] a proportion not seen with dog bites. In our study population the ratio of women to men with cat bites was approximately 3-to-1. Another study reported that bites (including cat bites, but excluding dog bites) were ranked sixth in terms of frequent causes of unintentional injuries in adult women. [53] An association between pet ownership (dogs and cats) and depression has previously been reported among women but not men [54].

The finding of a strong association between cat bites and depression, while intriguing, still does not point to why such a connection exists, and the relationship is likely complex. Here we discuss potential factors that may play a role, but emphasize that our study was not designed to elucidate the underlying mechanisms for why the association exists. Furthermore, such an association does not necessarily imply causation.

There is substantial evidence to suggest that pet ownership results in multiple health benefits, both physical and mental [55][69]. For example, pet ownership has been shown to reduce elevated blood pressure caused by mental stress even better than antihypertensive medications. [70] Pets can also provide substantial social support. [63] A study in Switzerland reported that among people living alone, cats could improve their mood. [71] As such, it may be that depressed individuals, especially women, are more likely to own cats for companionship. [72] Pet ownership has also been shown to moderate the effect of depression on mortality in patients who experienced a myocardial infarction [60].

But not all studies have reached similar conclusions, and the role of pets and human health remains controversial with multiple studies reporting inconclusive results [54], [59], [65], [66], [73][80]. For example, one study found a substantial survival benefit after myocardial infarction for dog owners but not cat owners. [74] It is also important to note that many health studies that considered pet ownership did not distinguish between cat and dog ownership.

A study in Canada reported that those living alone with a cat were just as lonely and depressed as those living alone without a cat, suggesting that the cat itself was not contributing significantly to an improved sense of well-being. [81] In this group, having high levels of social support decreased loneliness for dog owners but not for cat owners. Additionally, among those with low levels of social support, those who were more attached to their pets (both dogs and cats) had more depression than those with less attachment, a finding which has been corroborated elsewhere. [82] Another study reported that adults in Australia ages 60–64 with pets (dogs or cats) had more depression symptoms than non-pet owners [83] and a large study of Finnish adults found a small but significant increase in depression among those who owned a pet (12.6%) versus no pet (11.3%). [54] In one case the stress of caring for pet cats was a contributing factor in worsening a patient’s depression. [68] Other studies have shown no benefit to pet ownership among women in terms of reducing loneliness [72] or reducing emotional distress [84].

Perhaps most intriguing is the possibility that Toxoplasma gondii, a parasite commonly found in cats and known to infect humans, could be causing long-term changes to the cat owners’ brains. Recent studies have suggested that this parasite may actually contribute to human psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and obsessive compulsive disorder, as well as other brain pathologies [85], [86] and personality changes [87][90]. The Toxoplasma parasite has been linked to prenatal depression, [91] and a case report from 2004 discussed a patient with depression who was not responsive to anti-depressant medications until the parasite was eradicated. [92] Infections from the parasite have been associated with self-inflicted violence [93], [94] as well as increased suicide rates in women. [95] It has also been suggested that the inflammatory cytokines released during a T. gondii infection in the brain may be the cause of depression in some patients [96], [97].

The prevalence of T. gondii seroposivity in the United States has been dropping, but has been estimated to be around 11%. [98] It should be noted that cat bites, per se, are not thought to transmit the parasite. Rather, the parasite is typically shed in the feces of cats. Most transmission occurs orally through contact with contaminated food or drink, or in utero from mother to fetus. This is why pregnant women are advised not to change a cat’s litter box [99].

Not all evidence points to T. gondii, or their cat hosts, as being relevant to the depression relationship, however. The seroprevalence of T. gondii is generally higher in men than in women, [98], [100] and one study found that cat ownership was not a significant risk factor for being seropositive for the parasite. [100] Because of the nature of how the parasite is transmitted, it has been suggested that exposure to cats may not even be a significant risk for infection. [101] Indeed, some studies have not found a strong link between cat exposure and having an infection from or antibodies to T. gondii, [102], [103] with one study reporting a correlation only for those who had 3 or more kittens, but not for those with fewer. [104] Furthermore, while cats are necessary for the life cycle of the T. gondii parasaite, dogs have also been implicated in its transmission to humans [105].

Another interesting aspect of our findings is how they were discovered–that is, a non-directed, non-hypothesis driven data mining algorithm uncovered this unusual association between cat bites and depression. Further, we were able to corroborate the initial findings with a retrospective chart review. All of this was done using data that had been collected over the course of many years, by many clinicians, none of whom would likely have detected this pattern or relationship on their own. This secondary use of pre-existing clinical data is one component of the proposed ‘Learning Health System’, in which electronic data from institutions around the country, and potentially the world, could be pooled with the goal of enhancing discovery and improving patient outcomes. [106], [107] Others have also made discoveries through data mining approaches that were further confirmed with analyses of EHR data [108], [109].

There are challenges of finding these potentially novel associations among the many hundreds of thousands that are uncovered with large-scale data mining approaches, in part because the most significant associations are already known. [6] It is therefore necessary to search through the many less significant associations to detect new findings. In unpublished data from our original study of free text associations, the term ‘depression’ ranked 20 out of the 117 significant associations with ‘cat bite’, whereas ‘cat bite’ ranked 1,898 out of 3,879 significant associations with depression. [5] In the follow-up study of ICD-9 codes, ICD 311 (depression) ranked 26 out of 723 associations with ICD E906.3 (animal bite), whereas ICD E906.3 ranked 1,296 out of 6,667 among association with ICD 311 [6].

Our study does have several limitations, some of which are likely inherent in any large-scale analysis of EHR data. [110], [111] Like any retrospective study, we were limited by what was (or was not) reported by the clinicians in their clinical notes, or by which patients chose (or did not choose) to seek medical treatment. One study reported that nearly 40% of people with cat bites did not seek medical care. [46] It is also possible that our results could be skewed if depressed patients are more likely to seek care for a bite; depressed patients without a supportive social network may be more likely to be seen by a doctor for issues such as bites. While we attempted to determine who lived alone, we did not assess how much social support each individual had.

Additionally, there are many confounding factors other than the higher prevalence of depression in women that might influence the context in which our findings should be interpreted, Such factors might include (1) the age of the cat; (2) the number of cats or pets in the household; (3) the living arrangements of the cat (e.g., indoor vs outdoor) (4) the length of time the cat and bite recipient knew each other; (5) the events occurring at the time of the bite (e.g., taking the cat to the veterinarian, breaking up a fight between two cats, giving the cat a bath, accidentally stepping on the cat); (6) the cat’s temperament and prior history of biting; (7) the health status of the cat; and even (8) the profession of the patient. Some of the bites were noted to happen with people working in animal shelters, pet stores, or veterinarian offices. Women are now entering veterinary medicine professions more than men, [112], [113] and women more than men veterinarians often work in small animal practices (i.e., those that treat cats as opposed to cows) [114].

The preponderance of cat bites in females may also be due to a preponderance of women owning, or caring for, cats. One study from the American Pet Association reported that 80% of cat owners were women [115] whereas another from Gallup survey from 2007 reported no gender differences in cat or dog ownership. [116] According to 2006 data from the American Veterinary Medical Society (AVMS), women are more often the primary caretakers of cats as compared to men (78.1% vs 21.9%). [117] What is harder to explain is that in our study nearly an equal proportion of women and men suffered from dog bites, yet according to the AVMS women are also more often the primary caretaker of dogs as compared to men (74.2% vs 25.8%). Furthermore, among those living alone, pet ownership of cats and dogs is roughly equal: 13.1% of single-occupant households own a dog only, 15.9% own a cat only, and 5.4% own a cat and dog. How these single-occupant households with pets differed by gender is unknown, but according to recent U.S. census data there are slightly more single households with women (14.6%) than men (11.2%) [20].

Compared to men, women have been reported to own cats more often in other countries as well including the United Kingdom (UK). [118], [119] One study from the UK reported that 27% of women and 20% of men owned a cat. [120] Another study from the UK reported women to be 3.5 times more likely than men to own a cat. [121] Women are about twice as likely than men to own dogs in the UK, [118], [119] although other studies have reported a smaller difference in dog ownership (24% of women and 22% of men, respectively) [120].

It has also been suggested that there are personality types associated with cat or dog owners. [122] One study reported that women comprised 68.1% of those who defined themselves among the “cat people” group and 58.6% among the “dog people” group. [123] Those in the “cat people” group tended to have higher levels of neuroticism compared to “dog people” based on personality testing. Interestingly, though, there were similar levels of neuroticism among men and women who defined themselves as “cat people”, whereas a larger difference existed between the genders among “dog people”. The neuroticism personality trait has been strongly associated with depression, [124], [125] although whether neuroticism represents a vulnerability for developing depression is unclear [126].

Some animals may bite more in response to changes in their owners' mental state or level of responsiveness. For example, depressed individuals often make less eye contact compared to those without depression. [127], [128] Some animals, such as dogs, horses, and pigs are known to respond to human social cues such as gestures, gaze, and focus.[129][133] Even cats may respond to respond to pointing gestures [134] and human gaze. [135] One study reported that the type of activity being undertaken by a test subject had a large impact on a cat's behavior. [136] And another study of individuals living alone with a cat found differences in interactions with cats that were correlated with self-reported moods of “depressiveness” [137].

It is also possible that the risk of bites is greater in homes with multiple pets. Breaking up fights between cats was noted in the medical record to be a reason some patients were bitten by cats. Households are more likely to own multiple cats as opposed to multiple dogs. A recent AVMS survey reported that the mean number of dogs per household was 1.7, and 37.8% of dog owning households had more than one dog. Of cat owning households the mean number of cats was 2.2, and 51.8% had more than one cat. Additionally, 41% of dog-owning households had at least 1 cat and 47% of cat-owning households had at least one dog. [117] Another study reported that 13% of UK households owned 1 cat and 11% owned 2 or more cats whereas 17% of UK households owned one dog and 5% owned more than one dog [120].

Generalizability may also be an issue since the patients and pets that are predominant in Southeastern Michigan may not reflect the makeup of patients or pets in other areas of the country, including more rural or urban settings. For example, a study of over 6,000 bites occurring in New York City reported about 70% dog bites and 13% cat bites. [138] In our dataset of bites using the ICD E906.X codes, dogs made up 37% of bites and cats about 25%. We also do not know what the overall prevalence of pet ownership was among our patient cohort. Household pet information is rarely captured in the EHR except for when it might impact on allergic symptoms. However, the overall ownership level by household of dogs (36.8%) and cats (32.5%) in Michigan closely matches the national average (37.2% dogs, 32.4% cats) [117].

Another limitation of our study is that ICD-9 codes are used primarily for billing purposes rather than clinical care, and these codes are often inaccurate, [139][141] including for depression. [142] This was evident even in our study in which dog and cat bites were misclassified among multiple ICD-9 categories. We attempted to compensate for inaccurate coding by conducting a chart review of the patients’ records with bites. We did not, however, confirm the depression diagnosis among the patients who had one of the ICD-9 codes representing depression. Further, we focused our search on patients who had an ICD-9 code for an animal bite, but there may have been other instances in which the bite was coded differently (e.g., as an open, penetrating, or puncture wound instead). The United States will shortly migrate from ICD-9 to ICD-10 codes, the latter of which does have a set of codes specific to cat bites (e.g., W55.01), which may make such analyses more accurate.

Nevertheless, our study did include a base population of over one million patients with over ten years of data collected in the EHR. While the total number of patients with cat bites in our study was relatively small, the consequences of untreated depression can be large. It may be that the relationship between cat bites and human depression is spurious and no true cause-and-effect exists. But if the relationship can be shown to hold true in other settings it suggests that, whatever the underlying reason, depression should be considered by health care practitioners when patients, especially women, present with cat bites. For at least a subset of these patients, the presentation of cat bite may be their initial contact with a healthcare provider, and screening these patients for depression could provide a means for early detection. Further research is needed to better understand this unusual relationship. We believe that, at the least, this study demonstrates that leveraging the power of data mining with follow-up chart reviews has the potential to improve health. As clinicians continue to collect electronic data that can be aggregated and explored with various data mining approaches, the possibilities for research and data discovery will continue to grow.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: DAH NR LSS. Performed the experiments: DAH. Analyzed the data: DAH. Wrote the paper: DAH NR LSS.

References

  1. 1. Patnaik D, Butler P, Ramakrishnan N, Parida L, Keller BJ, et al.. (2011) Experiences with mining temporal event sequences from electronic medical records; San Diego, California, USA. 360–368.
  2. 2. Jensen PB, Jensen LJ, Brunak S (2012) Mining electronic health records: towards better research applications and clinical care. Nat Rev Genet 13: 395–405.
  3. 3. Bellazzi R, Zupan B (2008) Predictive data mining in clinical medicine: current issues and guidelines. Int J Med Inform 77: 81–97.
  4. 4. Roque FS, Jensen PB, Schmock H, Dalgaard M, Andreatta M, et al. (2011) Using electronic patient records to discover disease correlations and stratify patient cohorts. PLoS Comput Biol 7: e1002141.
  5. 5. Hanauer DA, Rhodes DR, Chinnaiyan AM (2009) Exploring clinical associations using '-omics' based enrichment analyses. PLoS One 4: e5203.
  6. 6. Hanauer DA, Ramakrishnan N (2013) Modeling temporal relationships in large scale clinical associations. J Am Med Inform Assoc 20: 332–341.
  7. 7. Kost R, Littenberg B, Chen ES (2012) Exploring generalized association rule mining for disease co-occurrences. AMIA Annu Symp Proc 2012: 1284–1293.
  8. 8. Holmes AB, Hawson A, Liu F, Friedman C, Khiabanian H, et al. (2011) Discovering disease associations by integrating electronic clinical data and medical literature. PLoS One 6: e21132.
  9. 9. Mullins IM, Siadaty MS, Lyman J, Scully K, Garrett CT, et al. (2006) Data mining and clinical data repositories: Insights from a 667,000 patient data set. Comput Biol Med 36: 1351–1377.
  10. 10. Wright A, Chen ES, Maloney FL (2010) An automated technique for identifying associations between medications, laboratory results and problems. J Biomed Inform 43: 891–901.
  11. 11. Marlin BM, Kale DC, Khemani RG, Wetzel RC (2012) Unsupervised pattern discovery in electronic health care data using probabilistic clustering models. 389.
  12. 12. Coloma PM, Schuemie MJ, Trifiro G, Gini R, Herings R, et al. (2011) Combining electronic healthcare databases in Europe to allow for large-scale drug safety monitoring: the EU-ADR Project. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf 20: 1–11.
  13. 13. Wang X, Hripcsak G, Markatou M, Friedman C (2009) Active computerized pharmacovigilance using natural language processing, statistics, and electronic health records: a feasibility study. J Am Med Inform Assoc 16: 328–337.
  14. 14. Denny JC, Ritchie MD, Basford MA, Pulley JM, Bastarache L, et al. (2010) PheWAS: demonstrating the feasibility of a phenome-wide scan to discover gene-disease associations. Bioinformatics 26: 1205–1210.
  15. 15. Zheng K, Mei Q, Yang L, Manion FJ, Balis UJ, et al. (2011) Voice-dictated versus typed-in clinician notes: linguistic properties and the potential implications on natural language processing. AMIA Annu Symp Proc 2011: 1630–1638.
  16. 16. Hanauer DA (2006) EMERSE: The Electronic Medical Record Search Engine. AMIA Annu Symp Proc: 941.
  17. 17. Seyfried L, Hanauer DA, Nease D, Albeiruti R, Kavanagh J, et al. (2009) Enhanced identification of eligibility for depression research using an electronic medical record search engine. Int J Med Inform 78: e13–18.
  18. 18. Yang L, Mei Q, Zheng K, Hanauer DA (2011) Query log analysis of an electronic health record search engine. AMIA Annu Symp Proc 2011: 915–924.
  19. 19. Zheng K, Mei Q, Hanauer DA (2011) Collaborative search in electronic health records. J Am Med Inform Assoc 18: 282–291.
  20. 20. Households and Families: 2010.
  21. 21. Mathers CD, Loncar D (2006) Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med 3: e442.
  22. 22. McKenna MT, Michaud CM, Murray CJ, Marks JS (2005) Assessing the burden of disease in the United States using disability-adjusted life years. Am J Prev Med 28: 415–423.
  23. 23. Gonzalez HM, Tarraf W, Whitfield KE, Vega WA (2010) The epidemiology of major depression and ethnicity in the United States. J Psychiatr Res 44: 1043–1051.
  24. 24. Arroll B, Elley CR, Fishman T, Goodyear-Smith FA, Kenealy T, et al.. (2009) Antidepressants versus placebo for depression in primary care. Cochrane Database Syst Rev: CD007954.
  25. 25. Arroll B, Macgillivray S, Ogston S, Reid I, Sullivan F, et al. (2005) Efficacy and tolerability of tricyclic antidepressants and SSRIs compared with placebo for treatment of depression in primary care: a meta-analysis. Ann Fam Med 3: 449–456.
  26. 26. Mulrow CD, Williams JW Jr, Chiquette E, Aguilar C, Hitchcock-Noel P, et al. (2000) Efficacy of newer medications for treating depression in primary care patients. Am J Med 108: 54–64.
  27. 27. Coyne JC, Palmer SC, Sullivan PA (2003) Screening for depression in adults. Ann Intern Med 138: 767; author reply 767–768.
  28. 28. Culpepper L (2012) Does screening for depression in primary care improve outcome? Curr Psychiatry Rep 14: 345–352.
  29. 29. Das AK, Gross R, Weissman MM (2003) Screening for depression in adults. Ann Intern Med 138: 767; author reply 767–768.
  30. 30. O'Connor EA, Whitlock EP, Gaynes B, Beil TL (2009)
  31. 31. Hickie IB, Davenport TA, Ricci CS (2002) Screening for depression in general practice and related medical settings. Med J Aust 177 Suppl: S111–116
  32. 32. Pignone MP, Gaynes BN, Rushton JL, Burchell CM, Orleans CT, et al. (2002) Screening for depression in adults: a summary of the evidence for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Ann Intern Med 136: 765–776.
  33. 33. Simon GE, VonKorff M (1995) Recognition, management, and outcomes of depression in primary care. Arch Fam Med 4: 99–105.
  34. 34. Desai MM, Rosenheck RA, Craig TJ (2006) Case-finding for depression among medical outpatients in the Veterans Health Administration. Med Care 44: 175–181.
  35. 35. Maradiegue AH, Khan F (2013) Missed opportunities in primary care: the importance of identifying depression through screening, family history, and chronic disease management. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv 51: 27–36.
  36. 36. Stromberg R, Wernering E, Aberg-Wistedt A, Furhoff AK, Johansson SE, et al. (2008) Screening and diagnosing depression in women visiting GPs' drop in clinic in Primary Health Care. BMC Fam Pract 9: 34.
  37. 37. Porter SC, Fein JA, Ginsburg KR (1997) Depression screening in adolescents with somatic complaints presenting to the emergency department. Ann Emerg Med 29: 141–145.
  38. 38. Scott EG, Luxmore B, Alexander H, Fenn RL, Christopher NC (2006) Screening for adolescent depression in a pediatric emergency department. Acad Emerg Med 13: 537–542.
  39. 39. Huffman JC, Pollack MH (2003) Predicting panic disorder among patients with chest pain: an analysis of the literature. Psychosomatics 44: 222–236.
  40. 40. Foldes-Busque G, Marchand A, Chauny JM, Poitras J, Diodati J, et al. (2011) Unexplained chest pain in the ED: could it be panic? Am J Emerg Med 29: 743–751.
  41. 41. Dendle C, Looke D (2008) Review article: Animal bites: an update for management with a focus on infections. Emerg Med Australas 20: 458–467.
  42. 42. Griego RD, Rosen T, Orengo IF, Wolf JE (1995) Dog, cat, and human bites: a review. J Am Acad Dermatol 33: 1019–1029.
  43. 43. Philipsen TE, Molderez C, Gys T (2006) Cat and dog bites. What to do? Guidelines for the treatment of cat and dog bites in humans. Acta Chir Belg 106: 692–695.
  44. 44. Dahl E (1998) [Animal bites at the casualty department of the Oslo City Council]. Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen 118: 2614–2617.
  45. 45. Matter HC, Sentinella A (1998) The epidemiology of bite and scratch injuries by vertebrate animals in Switzerland. Eur J Epidemiol 14: 483–490.
  46. 46. Palacio J, Leon-Artozqui M, Pastor-Villalba E, Carrera-Martin F, Garcia-Belenguer S (2007) Incidence of and risk factors for cat bites: a first step in prevention and treatment of feline aggression. J Feline Med Surg 9: 188–195.
  47. 47. Patrick GR, O'Rourke KM (1998) Dog and cat bites: epidemiologic analyses suggest different prevention strategies. Public Health Rep 113: 252–257.
  48. 48. Wright JC (1990) Reported cat bites in Dallas: characteristics of the cats, the victims, and the attack events. Public Health Rep 105: 420–424.
  49. 49. Kizer KW (1979) Epidemiologic and clinical aspects of animal bite injuries. JACEP 8: 134–141.
  50. 50. MacBean CE, Taylor DM, Ashby K (2007) Animal and human bite injuries in Victoria, 1998–2004. Med J Aust 186: 38–40.
  51. 51. O'Neil ME, Mack KA, Gilchrist J (2007) Epidemiology of non-canine bite and sting injuries treated in U.S. emergency departments, 2001–2004. Public Health Rep 122: 764–775.
  52. 52. Ostanello F, Gherardi A, Caprioli A, La Placa L, Passini A, et al. (2005) Incidence of injuries caused by dogs and cats treated in emergency departments in a major Italian city. Emerg Med J 22: 260–262.
  53. 53. Mack KA (2004) Fatal and nonfatal unintentional injuries in adult women, United States. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 13: 754–763.
  54. 54. Koivusilta LK, Ojanlatva A (2006) To have or not to have a pet for better health? PLoS One 1: e109.
  55. 55. Anderson WP, Reid CM, Jennings GL (1992) Pet ownership and risk factors for cardiovascular disease. Med J Aust 157: 298–301.
  56. 56. Arhant-Sudhir K, Arhant-Sudhir R, Sudhir K (2011) Pet ownership and cardiovascular risk reduction: supporting evidence, conflicting data and underlying mechanisms. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 38: 734–738.
  57. 57. Barker SB, Wolen AR (2008) The benefits of human-companion animal interaction: a review. J Vet Med Educ 35: 487–495.
  58. 58. Beetz A, Uvnas-Moberg K, Julius H, Kotrschal K (2012) Psychosocial and psychophysiological effects of human-animal interactions: the possible role of oxytocin. Front Psychol 3: 234.
  59. 59. Enmarker I, Hellzen O, Ekker K, Berg AG (2012) Health in older cat and dog owners: The Nord-Trondelag Health Study (HUNT)-3 study. Scand J Public Health 40: 718–724.
  60. 60. Friedmann E, Thomas SA, Son H (2011) Pets, depression and long term survival in community living patients following myocardial infarction. Anthrozoos 24: 273–285.
  61. 61. Hall PL, Malpus Z (2000) Pets as therapy: effects on social interaction in long-stay psychiatry. Br J Nurs 9: 2220–2225.
  62. 62. Krause-Parello CA (2012) Pet ownership and older women: the relationships among loneliness, pet attachment support, human social support, and depressed mood. Geriatr Nurs 33: 194–203.
  63. 63. McConnell AR, Brown CM, Shoda TM, Stayton LE, Martin CE (2011) Friends with benefits: on the positive consequences of pet ownership. J Pers Soc Psychol 101: 1239–1252.
  64. 64. Oka K, Shibata A (2009) Dog ownership and health-related physical activity among Japanese adults. J Phys Act Health 6: 412–418.
  65. 65. Raina P, Waltner-Toews D, Bonnett B, Woodward C, Abernathy T (1999) Influence of companion animals on the physical and psychological health of older people: an analysis of a one-year longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc 47: 323–329.
  66. 66. Serpell J (1991) Beneficial effects of pet ownership on some aspects of human health and behaviour. J R Soc Med 84: 717–720.
  67. 67. Terra VC, Sakamoto AC, Machado HR, Martins LD, Cavalheiro EA, et al. (2012) Do pets reduce the likelihood of sudden unexplained death in epilepsy? Seizure 21: 649–651.
  68. 68. Wisdom JP, Saedi GA, Green CA (2009) Another breed of “service” animals: STARS study findings about pet ownership and recovery from serious mental illness. Am J Orthopsychiatry 79: 430–436.
  69. 69. Wood L, Giles-Corti B, Bulsara M (2005) The pet connection: pets as a conduit for social capital? Soc Sci Med 61: 1159–1173.
  70. 70. Allen K, Shykoff BE, Izzo JL Jr (2001) Pet ownership, but not ace inhibitor therapy, blunts home blood pressure responses to mental stress. Hypertension 38: 815–820.
  71. 71. Turner DC, Rieger G, Gygax L (2003) Spouses and cats and their effects on human mood. Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of People & Animals 16: 213–228.
  72. 72. Zasloff RL, Kidd AH (1994) Loneliness and pet ownership among single women. Psychol Rep 75: 747–752.
  73. 73. Chur-Hansen A, Stern C, Winefield H (2010) Gaps in the evidence about companion animals and human health: some suggestions for progress. Int J Evid Based Healthc 8: 140–146.
  74. 74. Friedmann E, Thomas SA (1995) Pet ownership, social support, and one-year survival after acute myocardial infarction in the Cardiac Arrhythmia Suppression Trial (CAST). Am J Cardiol 76: 1213–1217.
  75. 75. Hirsch AR, Whitman BW (1994) Pet ownership and prophylaxis of headache and chronic pain. Headache 34: 542–543.
  76. 76. Lodrup Carlsen KC, Roll S, Carlsen KH, Mowinckel P, Wijga AH, et al. (2012) Does pet ownership in infancy lead to asthma or allergy at school age? Pooled analysis of individual participant data from 11 European birth cohorts. PLoS One 7: e43214.
  77. 77. Mayon-White R (2005) Pets–pleasures and problems. BMJ 331: 1254–1255.
  78. 78. McNicholas J, Gilbey A, Rennie A, Ahmedzai S, Dono JA, et al. (2005) Pet ownership and human health: a brief review of evidence and issues. BMJ 331: 1252–1254.
  79. 79. Parslow RA, Jorm AF (2003) Pet ownership and risk factors for cardiovascular disease: another look. Med J Aust 179: 466–468.
  80. 80. Wells DL (2009) Associations between pet ownership and self-reported health status in people suffering from chronic fatigue syndrome. J Altern Complement Med 15: 407–413.
  81. 81. Duvall Antonacopoulos NM, Pychyl TA (2010) An Examination of the Potential Role of Pet Ownership, Human Social Support and Pet Attachment in the Psychological Health of Individuals Living Alone Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of People & Animals. 23: 37–54.
  82. 82. Peacock J, Chur-Hansen A, Winefield H (2012) Mental health implications of human attachment to companion animals. J Clin Psychol 68: 292–303.
  83. 83. Parslow RA, Jorm AF, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Jacomb P (2005) Pet ownership and health in older adults: findings from a survey of 2,551 community-based Australians aged 60–64. Gerontology 51: 40–47.
  84. 84. Watson NL, Weinstein M (1993) Pet Ownership in Relation to Depression, Anxiety, and Anger in Working Women. Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of People & Animals 6: 135–138.
  85. 85. Thomas F, Lafferty KD, Brodeur J, Elguero E, Gauthier-Clerc M, et al. (2012) Incidence of adult brain cancers is higher in countries where the protozoan parasite Toxoplasma gondii is common. Biol Lett 8: 101–103.
  86. 86. Vittecoq M, Elguero E, Lafferty KD, Roche B, Brodeur J, et al. (2012) Brain cancer mortality rates increase with Toxoplasma gondii seroprevalence in France. Infect Genet Evol 12: 496–498.
  87. 87. Fond G, Capdevielle D, Macgregor A, Attal J, Larue A, et al. (2013) [Toxoplasma gondii: A potential role in the genesis of psychiatric disorders]. Encephale 39: 38–43.
  88. 88. Flegr J (2013) Influence of latent Toxoplasma infection on human personality, physiology and morphology: pros and cons of the Toxoplasma-human model in studying the manipulation hypothesis. J Exp Biol 216: 127–133.
  89. 89. Flegr J, Preiss M, Klose J, Havlicek J, Vitakova M, et al. (2003) Decreased level of psychobiological factor novelty seeking and lower intelligence in men latently infected with the protozoan parasite Toxoplasma gondii Dopamine, a missing link between schizophrenia and toxoplasmosis? Biol Psychol 63: 253–268.
  90. 90. Miman O, Mutlu EA, Ozcan O, Atambay M, Karlidag R, et al. (2010) Is there any role of Toxoplasma gondii in the etiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder? Psychiatry Res 177: 263–265.
  91. 91. Groer MW, Yolken RH, Xiao JC, Beckstead JW, Fuchs D, et al.. (2011) Prenatal depression and anxiety in Toxoplasma gondii-positive women. Am J Obstet Gynecol 204: 433 e431–437.
  92. 92. Kar N, Misra B (2004) Toxoplasma seropositivity and depression: a case report. BMC Psychiatry 4: 1.
  93. 93. Pedersen MG, Mortensen PB, Norgaard-Pedersen B, Postolache TT (2012) Toxoplasma gondii infection and self-directed violence in mothers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69: 1123–1130.
  94. 94. Zhang Y, Traskman-Bendz L, Janelidze S, Langenberg P, Saleh A, et al. (2012) Toxoplasma gondii immunoglobulin G antibodies and nonfatal suicidal self-directed violence. J Clin Psychiatry 73: 1069–1076.
  95. 95. Ling VJ, Lester D, Mortensen PB, Langenberg PW, Postolache TT (2011) Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and suicide rates in women. J Nerv Ment Dis 199: 440–444.
  96. 96. Arling TA, Yolken RH, Lapidus M, Langenberg P, Dickerson FB, et al. (2009) Toxoplasma gondii antibody titers and history of suicide attempts in patients with recurrent mood disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 197: 905–908.
  97. 97. Dantzer R, O'Connor JC, Freund GG, Johnson RW, Kelley KW (2008) From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 46–56.
  98. 98. Jones JL, Kruszon-Moran D, Sanders-Lewis K, Wilson M (2007) Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States, 1999 2004, decline from the prior decade. Am J Trop Med Hyg 77: 405–410.
  99. 99. Esch KJ, Petersen CA (2013) Transmission and epidemiology of zoonotic protozoal diseases of companion animals. Clin Microbiol Rev 26: 58–85.
  100. 100. Jones JL, Kruszon-Moran D, Wilson M, McQuillan G, Navin T, et al. (2001) Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States: seroprevalence and risk factors. Am J Epidemiol 154: 357–365.
  101. 101. Elmore SA, Jones JL, Conrad PA, Patton S, Lindsay DS, et al. (2010) Toxoplasma gondii: epidemiology, feline clinical aspects, and prevention. Trends Parasitol 26: 190–196.
  102. 102. Boyer K, Hill D, Mui E, Wroblewski K, Karrison T, et al. (2011) Unrecognized ingestion of Toxoplasma gondii oocysts leads to congenital toxoplasmosis and causes epidemics in North America. Clin Infect Dis 53: 1081–1089.
  103. 103. Vittecoq M, Lafferty KD, Elguero E, Brodeur J, Gauthier-Clerc M, et al. (2012) Cat ownership is neither a strong predictor of Toxoplasma gondii infection nor a risk factor for brain cancer. Biology Letters 8: 1042–1042.
  104. 104. Jones JL, Dargelas V, Roberts J, Press C, Remington JS, et al. (2009) Risk factors for Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States. Clin Infect Dis 49: 878–884.
  105. 105. Lindsay DS, Dubey JP, Butler JM, Blagburn BL (1997) Mechanical transmission of Toxoplasma gondii oocysts by dogs. Vet Parasitol 73: 27–33.
  106. 106. Friedman CP, Wong AK, Blumenthal D (2010) Achieving a nationwide learning health system. Sci Transl Med 2: 57cm29.
  107. 107. Friedman C, Rigby M (2012) Conceptualising and creating a global learning health system. Int J Med Inform.
  108. 108. Tatonetti NP, Denny JC, Murphy SN, Fernald GH, Krishnan G, et al. (2011) Detecting drug interactions from adverse-event reports: interaction between paroxetine and pravastatin increases blood glucose levels. Clin Pharmacol Ther 90: 133–142.
  109. 109. Tatonetti NP, Fernald GH, Altman RB (2012) A novel signal detection algorithm for identifying hidden drug-drug interactions in adverse event reports. J Am Med Inform Assoc 19: 79–85.
  110. 110. Botsis T, Hartvigsen G, Chen F, Weng C (2010) Secondary Use of EHR: Data Quality Issues and Informatics Opportunities. AMIA Summits Transl Sci Proc 2010: 1–5.
  111. 111. Hripcsak G, Knirsch C, Zhou L, Wilcox A, Melton G (2011) Bias associated with mining electronic health records. J Biomed Discov Collab 6: 48–52.
  112. 112. Koshgarian L, Goodman M, Curtis A, Vaisanen R, Wolf J (2008) Veterinary Medicine in New England: State-by-State Industry Characteristics and Economic Impacts.
  113. 113. Lofstedt J (2003) Gender and veterinary medicine. Can Vet J 44: 533–535.
  114. 114. Heath TJ (2007) Longitudinal study of veterinary students and veterinarians: family and gender issues after 20 years. Aust Vet J 85: 290–295.
  115. 115. Blakeley K (2009) Crazy Cat Ladies. Forbes.
  116. 116. Jones JM (2007) Companionship and Love of Animals Drive Pet Ownership. Gallup.
  117. 117. American Veterinary Medical Association. (2012) U.S. pet ownership & demographics sourcebook. Schaumburg, Ill.: American Veterinary Medical Association. xxi, 186 p.
  118. 118. Murray JK, Browne WJ, Roberts MA, Whitmarsh A, Gruffydd-Jones TJ (2010) Number and ownership profiles of cats and dogs in the UK. Vet Rec 166: 163–168.
  119. 119. Westgarth C, Pinchbeck GL, Bradshaw JW, Dawson S, Gaskell RM, et al. (2010) Factors associated with cat ownership in a community in the UK. Vet Rec 166: 354–357.
  120. 120. (2004) Pet Accessories and Healthcare - UK - March 2004. Mintel International Group Limited.
  121. 121. (2004) It's Official! Purring Puss Cats Enhance Men's Pulling Power, and Bring out Their Caring Fe-line Side! : PR Newswire.
  122. 122. Kidd AH, Kidd RM (1980) Personality Characteristics and Preferences in Pet Ownership. Psychological Reports 46: 939–949.
  123. 123. Gosling SD, Sandy CJ, Potter J (2010) Personalities of Self-Identified “Dog People” and “Cat People”. Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of People & Animals 23: 213–222.
  124. 124. Farmer A, Redman K, Harris T, Mahmood A, Sadler S, et al. (2002) Neuroticism, extraversion, life events and depression. The Cardiff Depression Study. Br J Psychiatry 181: 118–122.
  125. 125. Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL (2006) Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 1113–1120.
  126. 126. McWilliams L (2003) Neuroticism and depression. Br J Psychiatry 182: 80; author reply 80.
  127. 127. Hills PJ, Lewis MB (2011) Sad people avoid the eyes or happy people focus on the eyes? Mood induction affects facial feature discrimination. Br J Psychol 102: 260–274.
  128. 128. Segrin C (2000) Social skills deficits associated with depression. Clin Psychol Rev 20: 379–403.
  129. 129. Hemsworth PH, Gonyou HW, Dziuk PJ (1986) Human communication with pigs: The behavioural response of pigs to specific human signals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 45–54.
  130. 130. Maros K, Gacsi M, Miklosi A (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 11: 457–466.
  131. 131. Proops L, McComb K (2010) Attributing attention: the use of human-given cues by domestic horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 13: 197–205.
  132. 132. Reid PJ (2009) Adapting to the human world: dogs' responsiveness to our social cues. Behav Processes 80: 325–333.
  133. 133. Viranyi Z, Topal J, Gacsi M, Miklosi A, Csanyi V (2004) Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans' attentional focus. Behav Processes 66: 161–172.
  134. 134. Miklosi A, Pongracz P, Lakatos G, Topal J, Csanyi V (2005) A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. J Comp Psychol 119: 179–186.
  135. 135. Goodwin D, Bradshaw JWS (1997) Gaze and mutual gaze: its importance in cat/human and cat/cat interactions; July 24–25; Boston, Massachusetts, USA. 20–21.
  136. 136. Mertens C, Turner DC (1988) Experimental Analysis of Human-Cat Interactions During First Encounters. Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of People & Animals 2: 83–97.
  137. 137. Rieger G, Turner DC (1999) How Depressive Moods Affect the Behavior of Singly Living Persons toward their Cats. Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of People & Animals 12: 224–233.
  138. 138. Bregman B, Slavinski S (2012) Using emergency department data to conduct dog and animal bite surveillance in New York City, 2003–2006. Public Health Rep 127: 195–201.
  139. 139. O'Malley KJ, Cook KF, Price MD, Wildes KR, Hurdle JF, et al. (2005) Measuring diagnoses: ICD code accuracy. Health Serv Res 40: 1620–1639.
  140. 140. Reker DM, Rosen AK, Hoenig H, Berlowitz DR, Laughlin J, et al. (2002) The hazards of stroke case selection using administrative data. Med Care 40: 96–104.
  141. 141. Rhodes ET, Laffel LM, Gonzalez TV, Ludwig DS (2007) Accuracy of administrative coding for type 2 diabetes in children, adolescents, and young adults. Diabetes Care 30: 141–143.
  142. 142. Rawson NS, Malcolm E, D'Arcy C (1997) Reliability of the recording of schizophrenia and depressive disorder in the Saskatchewan health care datafiles. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 32: 191–199.