Figures
Abstract
Life history trade-offs have often been assumed to be the consequence of restrictions in the availability of critical resources such as energy and nutrients, which necessitate the differential allocation of resources to costly traits. Here, we examined endocrine (testosterone) and health (parasite burdens) parameters in territorial and non-territorial New Zealand fur seal males. We documented intra-sexual differences in sexual behaviours, testosterone levels, and parasitism that suggest a trade-off exists between reproductive success and physical health, particularly susceptibility to helminths and acanthocephalans, in males displaying different mating tactics (i.e., territorial and non-territorial tactics). Levels of testosterone were higher in territorial males and correlated positively with reproductive effort (i.e., intra- and inter-sexual interactions). However, these territorial males also exhibited high levels of parasitic infection, which may impair survival in the long-term. Our study, while limited in sample size, provides preliminary evidence for a link between male mating tactics, testosterone levels and parasite loads, and potential effects on reproductive success and life history that should be explored further.
Citation: Negro SS, Caudron AK, Dubois M, Delahaut P, Gemmell NJ (2010) Correlation between Male Social Status, Testosterone Levels, and Parasitism in a Dimorphic Polygynous Mammal. PLoS ONE 5(9): e12507. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012507
Editor: Dennis Marinus Hansen, Stanford University, United States of America
Received: February 25, 2010; Accepted: July 27, 2010; Published: September 13, 2010
Copyright: © 2010 Negro et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: SSN was funded by University of Canterbury and New Zealand International Doctoral Research (NZIDR). Research support for this project was obtained from the University of California Santa Cruz (UCSC) National Institutes of Health Minority International Research Training scheme and University of Canterbury. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Life history trade-offs have often been assumed to be the consequences of restrictions in the availability of critical resources such as energy and nutrients, necessitating differential allocation of resources to costly traits [1]. Increasing evidence implicates hormones as mediators of life history trade-offs [1], [2]. Hormones transduce environmental cues and regulate transitions between life-cycle stages (e.g. metamorphosis, maturation, and reproduction) in which organisms face developmental constraints [3]. Importantly, hormones can have pleiotropic and often antagonistic effects on morphological, physiological and behavioural characters [4]. Testosterone, the principal androgenic hormone, can raise male mating success by promoting the development of secondary sexual characters. Testosterone promotes aggressive behaviours associated with courtship and reproduction in vertebrates [5], [6], [7], while decreasing fitness at the same time by impairing traits such as parental care and immune system [8], [9]. Testosterone interacts with the immune system at the level of both individual cells involved in the humoral- and cellular-mediated immunity, and glands or tissues implicated in immune functions [10]. A schematic model of interactions between endocrine system, immune system, secondary sexual characters and parasites has previously been proposed in Folstad and Karter [9]. Testosterone may have a negative impact on the immune system when beyond a threshold level [11], [12]. Wingfield et al. [8] proposed a 3-level model of testosterone in male birds: a non-breeding androgen baseline (level A), the breeding androgen baseline induced by environmental cues (level B), and the physiological testosterone maximum that an individual can achieve during intra- and inter-sexual interactions (level C). Level B is necessary for spermatogenesis, expression of sexual behaviour, and the appearance of some secondary sexual characters, whereas level C is facultative and triggered by social stimuli or challenge during the breeding season. Level C corresponds to the frequency and intensity of territorial aggressiveness and female defence behaviour [8]. The above models described for birds are supported by other studies in pinnipeds [13], [14], [15].
New Zealand fur seals Arctocephalus forsteri (Lesson 1828) are polygynous and annual colonial breeders. Arctocephalus forsteri displays moderate polygyny where one male may mate with 4–15 females over the course of a single breeding season [16], [17]. The breeding season begins when adult males come ashore and establish territories in the austral spring. Females haul out from mid-November to late December to give birth [18]. Females enter oestrus approximately one week after parturition followed by alternation between foraging at sea and pup-nursing onshore. The breeding season ends when all females terminate their oestrous cycle and initiate their foraging cycles in February [18]. The life history traits of A. forsteri and other pinniped species are linked by several fundamental characteristics related to the evolution of their socio-sexual behaviour. They are marine feeders, but come onshore for parturition and postnatal pup care [19]. Reproduction in pinnipeds is characterised by highly synchronised pupping and mating (except for the tropical and subtropical species of seal such as Galapagos fur seals and monk seals which have a lower degree of synchrony), in addition to a delayed implantation of the blastocyst [20], [21]. Australian sea lions which have an asynchronous 18 month breeding cycle between colonies is the extreme exception to this common pattern [22].
In Mammals, little is known about the intra-sexual differences in physiology and the impacts of parasite infections on the reproductive success of male of different social status, employing different mating tactics [23], [24], [25], [26]. This study examined endocrine (testosterone) and health (parasite loads) parameters in A. forsteri males employing the territorial mating tactic and non-territorial mating tactics related to their relative reproductive success. Our first aim was to collect behavioural observations on individually identified males which allowed us to assign these males a mating tactic. The second aim was to measure testosterone levels in males displaying territorial and non-territorial mating tactics using non-invasive sampling methods. The third aim was to examine the parasitic load and species diversity in relation to the social status of the study males. Finally, we correlated the above results with the relative reproductive success of the territorial and non-territorial male mating tactics determined in a parallel study investigating the same focal area [17].
Materials and Methods
Study site
The Ohau Point seal breeding colony, ca. 2,200 individuals [27], located 26km north of the Kaikoura township, New Zealand (42°25′0″S, 173°40′60″E), is a narrow rookery colony (50–100m wide) that runs 1km along the Pacific Ocean coast and is situated close to fur seal foraging grounds [27]. Our study area was limited to the central portion of the colony (50–90m wide and 120m long, backed by a cliff on the west). A permit (Per/10/2002/01) to undertake marine mammal research (Marine Mammal Protection Act 1978) was given by the New Zealand Department of Conservation.
Behavioural sampling
We undertook 223 hours of field observations spread from mid-November 2004 to early January 2005 coinciding with the arrival of pregnant females and covering the period of highest intra- and inter-sexual interactions. Fifty-two males that spent more than one hour during the study period were readily identifiable by natural marking (e.g. body scars, flipper irregularities) or artificial marking (i.e. cattle ear tag or sheep tag from previous studies) and these were selected for further study. Three out of fifty two identified breeding males spent less than three hours at our study site and were excluded from the analyses. We recorded all interactions involving focal males by “all occurrences sampling” [28]. We categorised these interactions as described in Caudron et al. [17] with the following modifications. The intra-sexual interactions involved walking towards, following, chasing, attacking, lunging at, and fighting with another male. We attributed vocalizations to either apparently addressed to a male target or non-addressed. For each intra-sexual interaction, the study males were classified as aggressive (the male initiating the interaction and/or displacing or triumphing over the other male) or submissive (the target male and/or displaced male). The inter-sexual interactions involved walking towards, withholding, attempting to copulate and successful copulation, accepting or tolerating female advances (soliciting, mounting on the male's back, and biting the male's neck) and vocalizing to a female.
We used seven variables described in detail in Caudron et al. [17], to quantify the breeding behaviour of the study males: 1) the occurrence of aggressive intra-sexual interactions, 2) the occurrence of submissive intra-sexual interactions, 3) the occurrence of inter-sexual interactions, 4) the occurence of male vocalization to female, 5) the occurrence of male vocalization to addressed male target or non-addressed, 6) defence or non-defence of territory hosting females, and 7) the total number of days spent on the site. The number of copulations that could be observed during the study breeding season was very low. Previous studies in fur seals and several other pinnipeds have identified discrepancies between behavioural (i.e. copulations) and genetic (i.e. paternity) measurements of male reproductive success [29], [30]. We think that the number of copulations is not a reliable measure of male reproductive success. It was not possible to use male size and male reproductive success from Caudron et al. [17] because we had only four males from our study that were studied in Caudron et al. [17].
Testosterone assays
We used a non-invasive sampling approach to collect faecal (n = 10) and urine (n = 7) sample sizes for the testosterone assays from different individual males in our study area, for which extensive behavioural data were available. Our sample sizes were small because during the breeding season males fast and excrete at a low rate, and samples are only useful if fresh, thus sampling opportunities are understandably scarce. For reference samples, we also collected one faecal sample and several urine samples from females. The sample collections spread from mid-November 2004 to early January 2005 from the periphery or outside the male territories to minimise disturbance. This period corresponds to the period of highest intra- and inter-sexual interactions [17]. Territorial males generally urinated or defecated after chasing a subordinate male or on their way back to the centre of their territory. Samples were collected at the time of excretion with a sterile spoon or syringe from the rocky surface. Faecal and urine samples for the testosterone assays were stored at −20°C until assayed (ca. 3–4 months later).
We performed a preliminary extraction step [31], [32] followed by a purification step using immunoaffinity chromatography columns and reverse-phase high performance liquid chromatography prior to the testosterone radioimmunoassay. A detailed protocol of the testosterone assays is described in Negro [33]. The intra-assay coefficients of variation (i.e. variation of testosterone assays estimated from measuring all samples in duplicate in a same run) in urine and faeces were 20% and 25%, respectively. The limit of detection was 0.2 ng/ml in urine and 0.3 ng/g dry faeces for the faecal samples. Extraction efficiency was determined by the recovery of 3H-Testosterone (6,000 cpm) added to the samples prior to extraction and was 33±5% (mean±SD) for urine and 18±3% for faeces.
Parasitic load and species richness in faecal samples
We used the same faecal samples (n = 10) collected for the testosterone assays in addition to faecal samples collected from other known males (n = 2) to estimate the parasitic load and species diversity (trematodes, cestodes, nematodes, acanthocephalans). A modified McMaster quantitative method was used to estimate the number of parasite eggs per gram of faeces [34]. We added 56ml of saturated NaCl (density of 1200 kg.m−3 at 21°C) to 4 g of faecal material. After mixing, we filtered the solution through a 150µm sieve. We took a few ml of the suspension and filled in the two chambers (10×10 mm) of the McMaster slide and let the suspension rest for 5min before observing the slide with a microscope at 10×10 magnification. The egg count for each parasitic species was multiplied by 25 to determine the number of eggs per gram of faeces.
Statistical analyses
Parametric statistical methods (Student's and Welch's Two Sample t-tests) were used except when the assumption of normality (Shapiro Wilk test) of the data distribution was violated. In this situation, nonparametric tests (Mann-Whitney U Test, Spearman rank correlations) were used. For identifying male behavioural classes, we used an agglomerative hierarchical clustering based on similarities in their behavioural profile using Ward's linkage of Euclidean distances [35], [36]. The significance of the difference in testosterone levels in territorial and non-territorial males were tested by randomisation test (resampling based test). This robust statistical test was used because of the small sample sizes. The probability level used for significance was α = 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed in STATISTICA v8 [36] and R v1.7.1 [37].
Results
Behavioural sampling
We used seven variables to describe the breeding behaviours of the 49 focal males. The cluster analysis identified two classes, the territorial profile and the non-territorial profile (Figure 1). Two sub-classes are observed within the non-territorial class. One sub-class corresponds to non-territorial visitors wherein these transient males spent a few days (mean±SE; 2.19±0.33) on the site, but at various locations. The other sub-class corresponds to non-territorial residents wherein these resident males spent more days (12.50±2.36) on the site than the transient males and showed breeding site-fidelity [38], [39]. Caudron et al. [17] qualify males in these sub-classes as using alternative mating tactics. The descriptive statistics of behavioural observations are summarized in Table 1. We identified 12 topographically demarcated territories defended by territorial males.
The method builds the hierarchy from the individual elements by progressively merging clusters. The amount of clustering structure that has been found for each node is interpreted by the Agglomerative Coefficient (0.9759) (http://www.wessa.net/rwasp_agglomerativehierarchicalclustering.wasp/). The y-axis represents the male ID.
Territorial males showed a higher mean frequency of aggressive intra-sexual interactions (Mann-Whitney U Test; territorial vs resident, U = 5, p = 0.0001; territorial vs transient, U = 33, p<0.0001), a high frequency of male-female interactions (territorial vs resident, U = 5, p = 0.0001; territorial vs transient, U = 42, p<0.0001) and of vocalizations addressed and non-addressed (territorial vs resident, U = 10, p = 0.0007; territorial vs transient, U = 56, p<0.0001) compared to the non-territorial males (Table 1). Territorial males also showed a lower frequency of submissive intra-sexual interactions (territorial vs resident, U = 16, p = 0.0034; territorial vs transient, U = 130, p = 0.0146). Finally, the territorial males vocalised to females at a higher rate compared to non-territorial males (territorial vs resident, U = 27, p = 0.0283; territorial vs transient, U = 17, p<0.0001) (Table 1).
Testosterone assays
Faecal testosterone was significantly higher for territorial males compared to non-territorial males (Welch's two sample t-test, t = 4.94, df = 3.21, p<0.01) (Table 2). Concerning urine, testosterone levels in territorial males were high compared to non-territorial males, but the statistical test was not worth conducting with the low number of territorial male samples (n = 2). Female testosterone levels in faeces (2.1 ng/g dry faeces) and urine (0.167 ng/ml) were used as reference samples. The significance of the difference between the mean of territorial and non-territorial males has been tested using a robust statistical test i.e. randomisation test. We prepared vectors for 1000 differences of randomised data (from real data). The 0.975 quantile (37.97) and normal 0.975 quantile (34.85) using the faecal testosterone were smaller than the difference between the mean of territorial and non-territorial males in real data (50.04). The 0.975 quantile (6.98) using the urine testosterone was slightly greater than the difference between territorial and non-territorial in real data (5.99), and the normal 0.975 quantile (5.94) approximated the difference between territorial and non-territorial in real data (5.99). Even though the sample sizes are small and hence our findings are somewhat equivocal, the testosterone data suggest that non-territorial resident males have an intermediate testosterone range i.e. lower than territorial males (significant for the faecal samples; t = 4.45, df = 3.07, p-value = 0.02; not significant for the urine samples, t = 1.56, df = 1.01, p-value = 0.36) but higher than transient males (significant for the faecal samples; t = 5.49, df = 2.70, p-value = 0.01; significant for the urine samples, t = 3.50, df = 2.78, p-value = 0.04) (Table 2). Levels of faecal testosterone were positively correlated with the frequencies of aggressive intra-sexual (Spearman rank correlation; rs10 = 0.7178, p = 0.0234) and of inter-sexual interactions (rs10 = 0.7915, p = 0.0088), the frequency of vocalization to addressed male targets and non-addressed (rs10 = 0.7316, p = 0.0202). The correlation between levels of testosterone and the frequency of vocalization to females (rs10 = 0.6074, p = 0.06) was inconclusive as the p-value approximate 0.05. Levels of faecal testosterone were negatively correlated with the frequencies of submissive intra-sexual interactions (rs10 = −0.9879, p<0.0001). We found no conclusive correlation between urine testosterone and intra- and inter-sexual interactions and pattern of vocalization.
Parasitic load and species richness in faecal samples
A total of 12 faecal samples from different individual males were screened for parasite species (trematodes, cestodes, nematodes, acanthocephalans; Figure 2). We had testosterone measurements for 10 faecal samples out of 12. The parasite species diversity and descriptive statistics of parasite loads are detailed in Table 3. Two out of five territorial males had impressive numbers of eggs per gram of faeces (Contracaecum sp., 1575 and 2625 eggs/g of faeces; Synthesium sp., 2975 eggs/g of faeces; Bolbosoma sp., 1550 eggs/g of faeces). The original sites of parasite infection and the pathogenic characteristics of the observed parasite species described in previous studies are summarised in Table 4. Territorial males tended to be parasitised by a higher number of species compared to non-territorial males (Welch two sample t-test, t = 4.85, df = 6.62, p<0.002). The mean number of parasitic species per individual was 6.2±0.66 for the territorial males (n = 5) and 2.5±0.38 for the non-territorial males (n = 7).
Discussion
We recognise that this study is limited by small sample sizes, but given the system worked on, a marine mammal, the type and level of data we have been able to collect is quite exceptional and despite the small sizes the patterns observed are fairly robust and based on good statistical practice. Thus even while so constrained we believe that the overall pattern identified here, which holds strongly with evolutionary theory, will hold up to further investigation.
Immunosuppression by testosterone may be the result of resource allocation among activities with competing demands e.g. resources necessary for the maintenance of the immune functions may be reallocated to the production of costly secondary sexual characters, which have higher priority to increase mating success [9]. We considered that a mating tactic is a consequence of a combination of secondary sexual characteristics, hence we associate the non-territorial tactic with reduced secondary sexual characters and territorial tactic with enhanced secondary sexual characters.
Territorial A. forsteri males observed in this study showed more aggressive and sexual (dominant) behaviours compared to non-territorial males, in agreement with observations from earlier studies. In contrast, non-territorial males behaved in a subordinate way towards territorial males. In a synthesis derived from rodent, avian, and primate studies, Demas et al. [40] conclude that testosterone boosts reproductive effort by heightening sexual and aggressive behaviours, which in turn may provide advantages to male mating success. On the other hand, body size is an important facet of social rank in many mammals and particularly so in polygynous species where it directly influences the outcome of intra-sexual conflicts over access to females and consequently, male reproductive success [41], [42]. McGlothlin et al. [7] demonstrated positive correlations between the magnitude of testosterone concentration and the size of plumage ornament, which is an important determinant for female choice and male-male competition, in a population of dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis). Female mate choice might also operate in A. forsteri where territorial dimorphic polygynous males can reach three times the size of adult females [43], [44]. Unfortunately, this study does not include any photogrammetry data and hence we are unable to investigate the association between intra-sexual size differences and testosterone levels directly. However, we did find that testosterone levels in territorial males were significantly higher than the testosterone levels in the non-territorial resident and transient males, further the few non-territorial resident males we sampled seemed to express higher testosterone concentrations compared to transient males. Social modulation of circulating hormone levels in males has been documented in a variety of species [45], [46], [47]. Sexual interactions or mere exposure to conspecific females increase sex hormone levels in a wide range of species [45]. Higher testosterone levels are generally encountered in territorial and high-ranking individuals across vertebrate taxa [14], [48], [49]. Non-territorial resident males spent a longer period of time in the study area compared to the transient males. Therefore, the resident males had a longer continuous exposure to receptive females and are expected to have higher testosterone levels than the transient males that had a shorter exposure to receptive females. Other studies in mammals and birds show positive effects of heightened testosterone on vocal displays and reproductive success [5], [14], [50]. We observed a higher mean frequency of vocalization to female and to male targets and of undirected vocalizations in territorial than in non-territorial males which we interpret as a higher degree of expression of secondary sexual characters. The main costs for a territorial otariid male might be related to the costs endured from male-male dispute and fights, to the permanent presence in its territory (maximal tenure estimated to 44 days in our study area), and to the depleted energy reserves from fasting during territory tenure rather then the vocal display itself. In A. forsteri, the occurrence of serious fights (e.g. biting resulting in deep wounds) is very low [44], which suggests that vocalizations could be one of the elements which regulate territorial conflict. The intra- and inter-sexual functions of vocalization might be to avoid costly confrontations since males might use vocal and visual cues to assess each other's current fighting ability and physical condition, while allowing females to assess a male's quality [51].
Caudron et al. [17] have shown that territorial A. forsteri males in the same study area tend to be larger and have higher reproductive success compared to non-territorial males. This work in conjunction with our own data suggests a strong positive, but unquantified, relationship between testosterone levels, territorial breeding tactic, size and reproductive success in our study males, thus the largest males, occupied the territorial positions, showed the highest levels of testosterone and were observed in more mating bouts. On the other hand, we found a positive relationship between male territoriality, testosterone levels and potentially parasitic loads. Note that all parasite species are not equally virulent, so that a larger number of parasite species in one host versus another does not necessarily equate with a greater impact of parasitism. In addition, the virulence of any particular parasite species is often dependent on the intensity of infection (Table 4). We found high parasite egg counts in some territorial males and based on Table 1–4, we can only speculate a positive association between dominance, high testosterone levels, parasitic infection, and its negative impacts on health.
Intra-sexual differences in parasite infections can be attributed to ecological (exposure) or physiological (susceptibility) causes. Ecological causes of such individual differences in parasitic infections include differential exposures to pathogens through diet, microhabitat choice and breeding behaviour [52]. The most plausible physiological (hormonal) explanations of such differences in parasitic infection are either the indirect effects of stress on the immune system, the direct effects of sex steroids on parasite growth and development, or the indirect effects of sex steroids on parasite establishment, growth, and development within the host through effect on immune system [52]. To our knowledge, no studies on the potential differences in diet between territorial and non-territorial male vertebrates are documented. Food-chain transmitted parasites are positively related to food consumption. Dominance status and body weight are often related and are indirectly a function of food consumption [53]. Consequently, somatic growth simultaneously increases an individual's probability of reaching high rank status and the potential for exposure to the food-chain transmitted parasites. Arctocephalus forsteri territorial males are larger and thus may have been feeding more intensively over the months preceding the breeding season. This increased feeding activity might have exposed them to an overall higher number of parasites and/or to more infective stages of the parasite species found in the faecal samples, because they are mainly acquired via food. In addition, A. forsteri males fast during breeding season, which may cause nutritional and physiological stresses placing an additional pressure on the immune functions and ultimately on health and survival in territorial males. One assumption of this study is that parasite burdens are independent of age. While Simokova et al. [54] show positive correlation between ectoparasite species richness and longevity among cyprinid fish species, studies in odd-toed hoofed mammals [55] show a negative correlation between parasite burdens and host longevity, while inconsistent patterns are observed among seabird species [56]. Non-territorial A. forsteri males include both sexually immature and mature males. Territorial status can be reached after both sexual and social maturity [43]. Furthmore, a male that is territorial during one breeding season might not hold territory in the following breeding season (Boren pers. communication).
In mammals, integrated studies on intra-sexual differences in behaviour, morphology, physiology, and parasite infections associated to the reproductive success of the different mating tactics are largely unexplored. A few studies examining the wild population of Soay sheep (Ovis aries) on the remote island of St Kilda have taken an integrated approach to this problem. The interactions between reproductive success, mating tactics, testosterone, and morphology have been examined [24], [25], while Coltman and collaborators [26] have looked at parasite burdens and their relationship to fitness in Soay sheep. Here, we provide valuable preliminary data suggestive of strong links between male mating tactics, testosterone levels and parasite loads, with potential effects on reproductive success and life history, as has been hypothesised based on life-history theory. This current study showed positive correlations between dominance, aggressive and sexual behaviour, testosterone levels, and potentially parasite loads. In addition, a joint study at the same study site showed a positive relationship between male territoriality, male body length, and reproductive success (four territorial males from reference 17 could be sampled for this study. Testosterone levels, reproductive efforts and parasite infections (in some territorial males) were higher in territorial males conferring these males with an increase in reproductive success, but may result in impaired health and ultimately lower survival in the long-term.
Acknowledgments
Thank you to Dr. Laura Boren, Sacha Dowell, Christopher Muller, Rachel Lord, Christina Hoefsmit, and Françoise Maréchal for assistance in the field or laboratory. Thank you to Dr Paul Sunnucks (Monash), Dr Robert Poulin (Otago) and Dr Jon Slate (Sheffield) who made useful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: SSN AKC. Performed the experiments: SSN MD. Analyzed the data: SSN MD. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: PD NJG. Wrote the paper: SSN. Substantially contributed to the interpretation of data: NJG.
References
- 1. Zera AJ, Harshman LG (2001) The physiology of life history trade-offs in animals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 32: 95–126.
- 2. Knapp R (2004) Endocrine Mediation of Vertebrate Male Alternative Reproductive Tactics: The Next Generation of Studies. Integr Comp Biol 43: 658–668.
- 3. Jacobs JD, Wingfield JC (2000) Endocrine control of life-cycle stages: a constraint on response to the environment? Condor 102: 35–51.
- 4. Sinervo B, Svensson E (1998) Mechanistic and selective causes of life history trade-offs and plasticity. OIKOS 83: 432–442.
- 5. Alatalo RV, Hoglund J, Lundberg A, Rintamaki PT, Silverin B (1996) Testosterone and Male Mating Success on the Black Grouse Leks. Proceedings: Biological Sciences 263: 1697–1702.
- 6. Wingfield JC, Lynn SE, Soma KK (2001) Avoiding the ‘costs’ testosterone: Ecological bases of hormone-behavior interaction. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 57: 239–251.
- 7. McGlothlin JW, Jawor JM, Greives TJ, Casto JM, Phillips JL, et al. (2008) Hormones and honest signals: males with larger ornaments elevate testosterone more when challenged. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21: 39–48.
- 8. Wingfield JC, Hegner RE, Dufty AM Jr, Ball GF (1990) The “Challenge Hypothesis”: Theoretical Implications for Patterns of Testosterone Secretion, Mating Systems, and Breeding Strategies. The American Naturalist 136: 829–846.
- 9. Folstad I, Karter AJ (1992) Parasites, Bright Males, and the Immunocompetence Handicap. The American Naturalist 139: 603.
- 10. Grossman CJ (1985) Interactions between the gonadal steroids and the immune system. Science 227: 257–261.
- 11. Muehlenbein MP, Bribiescas RG (2005) Testosterone-mediated immune functions and male life histories. American Journal of Human Biology 17: 527–558.
- 12. Martin LB, Weil ZM, Nelson RJ (2008) Seasonal changes in vertebrate immune activity: mediation by physiological trade-offs. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 363: 321–339.
- 13. Atkinson S, Gilmartin WG (1992) Seasonal testosterone pattern in Hawaiian monk seals (Monachus schauinslandi). Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 96: 35–39.
- 14. Bartsh SS, Johnston SD, Siniff DB (1992) Territorial behavior and breeding frequency of male Weddell seals (Leptonychotes weddeli) in relation to age, size, and concentrations of serum testosterone and cortisol. Canadian Journal of Zoology 70: 680–692.
- 15. Lidgard DC, Boness DJ, Bowen WD, McMillan JI (2008) The implications of stress on male mating behavior and success in a sexually dimorphic polygynous mammal, the grey seal. Hormones and Behavior 53: 241–248.
- 16.
Mattlin RH (1978) Population biology, thermoregulation and site preference of the New Zealand fur seal, Arctocephalus forsteri (Lesson, 1828), on the Open Bay Islands, New Zealand. PhD thesis, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
- 17. Caudron AK, Negro SS, Fowler M, Boren L, Poncin P, et al. (2009) Alternative mating tactics in the New Zealand fur seal Arctocephalus forsteri: When non-territorial males are successful too. Australian Journal of Zoology 57: 409–421.
- 18. Goldsworthy SD, Shaughnessy PD (1994) Breeding biology and haul-out pattern of the New Zealand fur seal (Arctocephalus forsteri) at Cape Gantheaume, South Australia. Wildlife Research 21: 365–376.
- 19. Bartholomew GA (1970) A model for the evolution of pinniped polygyny. Evolution 24: 546–559.
- 20. Trillmich F, Trillmich KGK (1984) The mating systems of pinnipeds and marine iguanas: convergent evolution of polygyny. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 21: 209–216.
- 21. Atkinson S (1997) Reproductive biology of seals. Reviews of Reproduction 2: 175–194.
- 22. Gales NJ, Shaughnessy PD, Dennis TE (1994) Distribution, abundance and breeding cycle of the Australian sea lion Neophoca cinerea (Mammalia: Pinnipedia). Journal of Zoology 234: 353–370.
- 23. Mills SC, Grapputo A, Jokinen I, Koskela E, Mappes T, et al. (2009) Testosterone-Mediated Effects on Fitness-Related Phenotypic Traits and Fitness. American Naturalist 173: 475–487.
- 24. Preston BT, Stevenson IR, Pemberton JM, Coltman DW, Wilson K (2003) Overt and covert competition in a promiscuous mammal: the importance of weaponry and testes size to male reproductive success. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 270: 633–640.
- 25. Stevenson IR, Bancroft DR (1995) Fluctuating Trade-Offs Favour Precocial Maturity in Male Soay Sheep. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 262: 267–275.
- 26. Coltman DW, Pilkington J, Kruuk LEB, Wilson K, Pemberton JM (2001) Positive genetic correlation between parasite resistance and body size in a free-living ungulate population. Evolution 55: 2116–2125.
- 27. Boren LJ, Muller CG, Gemmell NJ (2006) Colony growth and pup condition of the New Zealand fur seal (Arctocephalus forsteri) on the Kaikoura coastline compared with other east coast colonies. Wildlife Research 33: 497–505.
- 28. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–267.
- 29. Amos W, Twiss S, Pomeroy PP, Anderson SS (1993) Male mating success and paternity in the grey seal, Halichoerus grypus: a study using DNA fingerprinting. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 252: 199–207.
- 30. Gemmell NJ, Burg TM, Boyd IL, Amos W (2001) Low reproductive success in territorial male Antarctic fur seals (Arctocephalus gazella) suggests the existence of alternative mating strategies. Molecular Ecology 10: 451–460.
- 31. Möstl E, Rettenbacher S, Palme R (2005) Measurement of Corticosterone Metabolites in Birds' Droppings: An Analytical Approach. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1046: 17–34.
- 32. Palme R (2005) Measuring fecal steroids: guidelines for practical application. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1046: 75–80.
- 33.
Negro SS (2008) Reproductive ecology and life history trade-offs in a dimorphic polygynous mammal, the New Zealand fur seal. PhD thesis, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
- 34.
Thienpont D, Rochette F, Vanparijs OFJ (1979) Diagnostic de verminose par examen coprologique. Beerse, Belgique: Janssen Research Foundation.
- 35. Johnson SC (1967) Hierarchical clustering schemes. Psychometrika 32: 241–254.
- 36.
StatSoft (2007) STATISTICA (data analysis software system), version 8.0. wwwstatsoftcom.
- 37. Ihaka R, Gentleman R (1996) R: A Language for data analysis and graphics. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics 5: 299–314.
- 38. Stirling I (1970) Observations on the behaviour of the New Zealand fur seal (Arctocephalus forsteri). Journal of Mammalogy 51: 766–778.
- 39. Bradshaw CJA, Davis LS, Lalas C, Harcourt RG (2000) Geographic and temporal variation in the condition of pups of the New Zealand fur seal (Arctocephalus forsteri): evidence for density dependence and differences in the marine environment. Journal of Zoology 252: 41–51.
- 40.
Demas GE, Cooper MA, Albers HE, Soma KK (2006) Novel mechanisms underlying neuroendocrine regulation of aggression: a synthesis of rodent, avian and primate studies. In: Blaustein JD, Lajtha A, editors. Handbook of neurochemistry and molecular neurobiology. Berlin, Germany: Springer. 21 p.
- 41.
Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE (1988) Reproductive success in male and female red deer. In: Clutton-Brock TH, editor. Reproductive success: studies of individual variation in contrasting breeding sustems. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 325–343.
- 42.
Le Boeuf BJ, Reiter J (1988) Lifetime reproductive success in northern elephant seals. In: Clutton-Brock TH, editor. Reproductive success: studies of individual variation in contrasting breeding sustems. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 344–362.
- 43.
Miller E (1971) Social and thermoregulatory behaviour of the New Zealand fur seal, Arctocephalus forsteri (Lesson, 1828). MSc thesis, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
- 44.
Carey PW (1989) Behavioural thermoregulation and polygyny in the New Zealand fur seal. PhD thesis, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
- 45. Harding CF (1981) Social Modulation of Circulating Hormone Levels in the Male. American Zoologist 21: 223–231.
- 46. Oliveira RF, Almada VC, Canario AV (1996) Social modulation of sex steroid concentrations in the urine of male cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. Horm Behav 30: 2–12.
- 47. Goymann W, East ML, Wachter B, Honer OP, Mostl E, et al. (2001) Social, state-dependent and environmental modulation of faecal corticosteroid levels in free-ranging female spotted hyenas. Proc Biol Sci 268: 2453–2459.
- 48. Sapolsky RM (1986) Stress-induced elevation of testosterone concentration in high ranking baboons: role of catecholamines. Endocrinology 118: 1630–1635.
- 49. Shargal D, Shore L, Roteri N, Terkel A, Zorovsky Y, et al. (2008) Fecal testosterone is elevated in high ranking female ibexes (Capra nubiana) and associated with increased aggression and a preponderance of male offspring. Theriogenology 69: 673–680.
- 50. Ketterson ED, Nolan V Jr, Wolf L, Ziegenfus C, Duffy AM Jr, et al. (1992) Testosterone and Avian Life Histories: Effects of Experimentally Elevated Testosterone on Behavior and Correlates of Fitness in the Dark-Eyed Junco (Junco hyemalis). The American Naturalist 140: 980–999.
- 51. Kunc HP, Wolf JBW (2008) Seasonal changes of vocal rates and their relation to territorial status in male Galápagos sea lions (Zalophus wollebaeki). Ethology 114: 381–388.
- 52. Zuk M, McKean KA (1996) Sex differences in parasite infections: Patterns and processes. International Journal for Parasitology 26: 1009–1024.
- 53.
Krebs JR, Davis NB (1983) An introduction to behavioral ecology. London: Blackwell Scientific.
- 54. Simkova A, Lafond T, Ondrackova M, Jurajda P, Ottava E, et al. (2008) Parasitism, life history traits and immune defence in cyprinid fish from Central Europe. BMC Evolutionary Biology 8: 29–39.
- 55. Ezenwa VO, Price SA, Altizer S, Vitone ND, Cook KC (2006) Host traits and parasite species richness in even and odd-toed hoofed mammals, Artiodactyla and Perissodactyla. Oikos 115: 526–536.
- 56. Hughes J, Page R (2007) Comparative tests of ectoparasite species richness in seabirds. BMC Evolutionary Biology 7: 227.
- 57.
Lauckner G (1985) Diseases of mammalia: Pinnipeda. In: Kinne O, editor. “Diseases of Marine Animals” Vol IV, Pt 2. chap 5. Hamburg: Biologische Anstalt Helgoland. pp. 683–793.
- 58. Ionita M, Varela MG, Lyons ET, Spraker TR, Tolliver SC (2008) Hookworms (Uncinaria lucasi) and acanthocephalans (Corynosoma spp. and Bolbosoma spp.) found in dead northern fur seals (Callorhinus ursinus) on St. Paul Island, Alaska in 2007. Parasitology Research 103: 1025–1029.
- 59. Young P, Lowe D (1969) Larval nematodes from fish of the subfamily Anisakinae and gastrointestinal lesions in mammals. Journal of Comparative Pathology 301–313.
- 60. Ridgway S, Geraci JR, Medway W (1975) Diseases of pinnipeds. Rapp P V Reun Cons Int Explor Mer 169: 327–337.
- 61.
Fletcher D, Gulland FMD, Haulena M, Lowenstine LJ, Dailey M (1998) Nematode-associated gastrointestinal perforations in stranded California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). In “International Association for Aquatic Animal Medecine” 29th Annual Conference Proceedings, San Diego, California, 59.
- 62. Gulland FMD, Beckmen K, Burek K, Lowenstine L, Werner L, et al. (1997) Nematode (Otostrongylus circumlitus) infection of northern elephant seals (Mirounga angustirostris) stranded along the central California coast. Marine Mammal Science 13: 446–458.
- 63.
Dailey MD (2001) Parasitic diseases. In: Dierauf LA, Gulland FMD, editors. “CRC Handbook of Marine Mammal Medecine” Second Edition. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. pp. 357–379.
- 64. Fauquier D, Gulland F, Haulena M, Dailey M, Rietcheck RL, et al. (2004) Meningoencephalitis in Two Stranded California Sea Lions (Zalophus californianus) Caused by Aberrant Trematode Migration. Journal of Wildlife Diseases 40: 816–819.