Peer Review History
Original SubmissionApril 10, 2021 |
---|
PONE-D-21-11911 The Hidden Side of Animal Cognition Research: Scientists’ Attitudes Toward Bias, Replicability and Scientific Practice PLOS ONE Dear Dr. Farrar, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. I sent it to two experts in this area and their feedback appears below. As you can see, both reviewers are positively disposed towards this work and see its potential to make a contribution. They nevertheless provide a series of recommendations aimed at improving it. Thus, after careful consideration, I feel that while your paper has merit it does not yet fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria. I therefore invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 30 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter. If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Mark Nielsen, Ph.D. Academic Editor PLOS ONE Journal Requirements: When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. 1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf 2. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts: a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent. b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice. [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: N/A Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: This manuscript reports the results of an online survey about animal cognition researchers’ attitudes towards bias, replicability and scientific practice. Most researchers have discussions about these topics on a regular basis in their smaller circles of colleagues, so these issues themselves – and the diversity of view points that we encounter- are hardly news to the community. However, I think a quantitative assessment of these attitudes spanning a larger sample of researchers, as provided by the current paper, can lend weight to a discussion of whether there is agreement that the field faces potential problems, where they lay and what could be done about them. Methods and results are clearly presented and fit with what the authors conclude from the findings. Data was available at the indicated repository. I have no major concerns with this paper. The free-text exemplars are interesting to read, but sometimes it could be made clearer according to which criteria the reported exemplars were chosen. I would also suggest to mention explicitly on p 10 (somewhere around 217-219) that the full range of answers the authors were allowed to share can be found in the OSF data set. On p 10-11 the authors report that the coders came up with several categories/common themes for the free text analysis. I wasn’t sure how this relates to the different areas of the questions, i.e. whether the referred to “common themes” span all areas or refer to within-area categorization? Sometimes there are categories with exemplars in tables (e.g. Table 4) and sometimes only exemplars are reported (e.g. Table 3). In this respect, I found the result presentation a bit inconsistent. I think the authors could make a stronger point in discussing the merit or potential use of their results. In line 534-535 they say the results might be used to guide discussions on how animal cognition research might improve in the future, but who should have these discussions - Researchers, universities, journals? For example, could the paper spark fruitful discussion among researchers with opposing views? Does it have the potential to serve as a reference point to inquire with journals, universities, and funding agencies, what their rejection and/or hiring practices are and whether they think their incentives serve the quality of scientific advances, given concerns of the interviewed researchers (e.g. that many researchers report they feel pressured to produce novel & sexy rather than solid scientific outputs)? Does this differ/align with attitudes in other scientific research fields? What initiatives are already out there trying to improve the situation and how can the current paper inform and complement these movements? When I finished reading the manuscript, I was left wondering: “so what?” For me, some more elaboration was missing on how the paper can help improve the situation and who is its target reader group. Typos & formatting issues: L 161: missing “t” in “…and he last 4 questions…” L 168: replace “I” with “they”? L 173: remove “.” L 258-259: misplaced line break L 280: misplaced line break L 347: misplaced line break Resolution quality of all figures on the pdf was quite bad, sometimes barely readable. .Tiff quality was fine. Fig 1-5: I am not sure the full questionnaire needs to be in the paper. I would maybe give a formatting example in the paper and provide the full questionnaire in ESM. Reviewer #2: Overall Impression: In this paper, the authors report a timely study on the beliefs of animal cognition researchers as they relate to research practices and bias in their field. The authors surveyed over 200 researchers and find: - evidence of publication bias, - that participants feel that results in their field can be biased (due to questionable research practices), although, researchers views of the field in general are more negative than their views of their own practices. - participants report the importance of replication although many feel that only 60-70% of findings in their field may replicate. Overall, this paper provides some interesting and important data on how animal cognition researcher view the state of their field in the context of wider scientific issues (and some more field-specific issues, i.e. Morgan's Canon). The paper is well written, with results clearly presented and I think this paper makes an important contribution to our understanding of how animal cognition research operates. Minor Comments. Abstract: Solid abstract covering methods and key results, but missing an overarching conclusion. Putting this evidence together, what do the authors feel the main take-home message is (regarding animal cognition researchers views on these issues?). Introduction: Solid introduction with appropriate referencing throughout. I wonder if a brief paragraph about the study aims (just before the methodology or description of key survey blocks) would help the reader understand exactly what the authors expect to achieve. It is easy to implicitly parse the aims from the intro (i.e., from line 62: "But how effectively these debates are reaching animal cognition researchers in general, and how they are received, has garnered little attention"), but I think a clearer statement would set up the methodology nicely. Methods - Free-text analysis: I am not an expert on qualitative methods, but I think the researchers are describing some form of thematic analysis. The procedure used to develop themes seems fairly robust but I think that results from this qualitative analysis should be reported consistently (see comments below regarding tables in results). Table 1. Percentage might a more meaningful descriptor for both variables (with Ns in brackets perhaps)? I.e. easier to assess distribution across bins at a glance? Tables (2-7). I am not 100% sure why table 2 is included. It presents a "selection" of some biases that researchers think exist, but how were these examples chosen ? Did they emerge from the most common themes? Are they the clearest and most coherent responses (if so, is this an adequate reason to include them here)? If the point of the table is to merely demonstrate a range of views, that is fine, but a clear description of how these specific views were chosen would be useful. I think Tables 3 and 5 have the same issue, while Tables 4, 6 and 7 are fine (as the method for developing themes has been made clear in the methodology and so the examples make sense). The benefit of reading a summary of themes (e.g. like in table 4) is that readers get an idea of both the range of issues that participants mention of their own accord, as well as the relative frequency of these comments. On the other hand, the quotes summarised in table 2 and 3 are less informative as they are not presented in a systematic fashion that help us pick out themes, and we don't get an idea of how popular these various views are. In short, I feel that the reporting of example text should be consistent and align with the analyses described. Lines 341- Where the authors report "many researchers also reported" I think an N would be useful (to get a better idea of how this comment generalises to the sample. Lines 399-403. When comparing outcomes on ordinal scales wouldn't it be useful to include a non-parametric test to demonstrate a different distribution of results? This isn't necessary for most sections, but it would be useful for a statement like this: "Predominantly, researchers somewhat agreed (34.0%) or somewhat disagreed (30.1%) that their area of animal cognition research would experience a replication crisis if attempts to 401 replicate most of its studies were conducted, however they either somewhat (43.7%) or strongly (29.3%) agreed that some other areas of animal cognition research would experience a replication crisis." Here, the "however" is important as there is a suggestion that the different responses on these measures are meaningful - a Wilcoxon (for example) would tell us whether this difference is likely to be due to chance. Line 436. Why highlight this quote? Again, I am not against using these examples but the rationale should be made clear. Line 454. Why are these themes presented differently from the earlier ones (i.e. in tables with N)? I think that if these themes are important enough to be mentioned they should be presented in a consistent manner (i.e. the number of participants who mentioned these issues should be mentioned, etc.). Discussion: Nice to see the authors address general issues with their methods head-on, but I think it would be worthwhile to discuss specific issues in the discussion too and how these may specifically limit interpretations/conclusions. For example, the authors write: "Rather, they must be interpreted considering the likely sampling biases in who participated in our survey and how their answers were limited by the way the questions were asked." Could these sampling biases and their implications be spelled out for us. Line 544: Be careful of using qualitative responses as evidence of a "general" beliefs in your participants without providing some qualifiers or justification (e.g. "Importantly, researchers’ qualitative responses suggested that they believe bias not to be uniform across the field, instead reporting that certain topics and researchers may be more likely to be affected by bias than others"). Obviously, the same applies to the use of quantitative results, but this is easier to justify given you are reporting raw proportions. Lines 597-599: The authors interpret the qualitative data relating to Morgan's canon using two themes (a) recognised the inherent ambiguity and multiple interpretations of Morgan’s canon, and b) cautioned against a blind application of Morgan’s canon), however, these themes are not used in table 3 (see comment above regarding consistency in qualitative analysis). Discussion - Overall: Good summary of key findings as they relate to the survey questions, and good use of free-response text to qualify (or add nuance) to your interpretation of this data. However, one nice thing about qualitative data is that it can go beyond your questions. I wonder if a section of your discussion could address this - for example, the "miscellaneous themes" extracted in your qualitative analysis only warrant a sentence or two in your discussion, but I feel that these findings help identify the next questions to be put to animal cognition researchers. The conclusion does a good job summarising the main results, but I would like to read about: a) taking the results together what are the take-home messages, b) are researcher practices/beliefs/etc in this field different from other disciplines, c) based upon these results what are the authors recommendations (if any) for the future directions of this type of research, education of researchers to reduce bias, changes to incentives that drive these issues. (in short, I think in the discussion the authors could think bigger) Very minor issues, typos, etc.: Line 74: "In the current study, we used surveyed researchers’ attitudes" Lines 153-158. The presentation order here is different from the order in the intro- probably best to be consistent unless there is a good reason. Line 161. "he" should be "the" Line 527-529: The following sentence is a bit long and could benefit from being rephrased: "Specifically, we do not believe that these data are a very accurate or representative data of all animal cognition researchers’ beliefs, or very accurate estimates of, for example, the absolute rate of questionable research practice use in the field (see e.g. 42])." Line 573/574/577. Should "Figs" be "figures"? ********** 6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: No [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 1 |
The Hidden Side of Animal Cognition Research: Scientists’ Attitudes Toward Bias, Replicability and Scientific Practice PONE-D-21-11911R1 Dear Dr. Farrar, Thank you for taking the time to revise your manuscript. I am satisfied with your responses to the reviewer feedback and the associated changes in the manuscript, and am therefore pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements. Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication. An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org. Kind regards, Mark Nielsen, Ph.D. Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Formally Accepted |
PONE-D-21-11911R1 The Hidden Side of Animal Cognition Research: Scientists’ Attitudes Toward Bias, Replicability and Scientific Practice Dear Dr. Farrar: I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org. If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org. Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access. Kind regards, PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff on behalf of Dr. Mark Nielsen Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Open letter on the publication of peer review reports
PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.
We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.
Learn more at ASAPbio .