Round 1 Response to Editor and Reviewers
Note to Editor and all Reviewers: In line with recommendations for enhanced clarity,
we have changed the naming of our variables as follows: (1) instead of “Memory-Related
Distress”, we now use “Emotional Pain at First Recall”; (2) instead of “Task-Related
Distress”, we now use “Emotional Pain During the Task”; (3) instead of “Task-Related
Comfort”, we now use “Comfort During the Task.” Instead of using both “distress” and
“emotional pain” in various places in the manuscript, we now consistently use the
term “emotional pain” and offer a concise definition of this concept on page 3 of
the manuscript. Thank you for your recommendations to improve our manuscript.
Editor Comments
Comment 1: Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration,
we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria
as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the
manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Specifically,
all three reviewers point out that the framing of the study could be better linked
to the actual study, in terms of that targeting memory related pain instead of acute
pain (see reviewers 3&2), and it could be sharpened by more specificity in the data
analysis and interpretation based on potential confounds (see reviewer 1).
Response 1: We thank you and the reviewers for your thoughtful feedback and the opportunity
to improve upon our manuscript. We have responded to each comment provided by you
and the reviewers in what follows. We hope you’ll agree that this revised manuscript
is now ready to share with the scientific community.
To summarize, we have made the following major revisions:
- We have added greater detail to our introduction to clarify our focus on emotional
pain, specifically discussing why it is important to study and how this study meaningfully
extends the existing literature on consoling touch and physical pain.
- We have added more information regarding how we’ve operationalized emotional pain,
and why recalling painful memories is not an example of imagined pain, but rather
a way to reflect on and relive one’s own personal emotional pain (providing several
related citations).
- We have added more information regarding our data analysis and results, including
our gender differences analyses, relationship satisfaction moderation analyses, and
different possible covariates for the follow-up analyses.
- We have revised some of our language to be more consistent throughout, and updated
the naming of our variables for increased clarity (see “Note to Editor and all Reviewers”).
- Additional materials have been added to our OSF repository to enhance the reproducibility
of our work, including details of each of our measures, our story selection form (referenced
on page 8), our protocols for each session, and the variables used to create our composite
variables (see dataset). As a reminder, we do not have IRB permission to share the
data publically, so it is only available to view through a private link: https://osf.io/9wbua/?view_only=de32be8c75a94f1e9d3bdb9c7c21f19f.
Comment 2: If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols
in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results.
Response 2: We have added our protocols to the OSF repository for this study in order
to maintain a single location where all of our study materials are located.
Comment 3: Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements,
including those for file naming.
Response 3: Our manuscript and file names have been revised to meet PLOS ONE’s style
requirements.
Comment 4: Please change "Caucasian” to “White” or “of [Western] European descent”
(as appropriate).
Response 4: We have changed “Caucasian” to “White” on page 6.
Reviewer #1:
Comment 1: The research questions of the study have merit and I was enthusiastic to
read the findings from this study. I found the research methods—including data collection
procedures and statistical analyses—to be appropriate for the research questions.
However, I do have some major and minor concerns (each of these are listed below)
which I feel impact the validity of the findings. Overall, I think that there are
some issues that confound the interpretation of the findings and unless some follow-up
studies or extra data collection are not performed, I am not confident these findings
would hold.
Response 1: We thank you for your feedback and the suggestions for improving upon
our manuscript. We have taken steps to address your concerns, detailed below. While
additional data collection to follow-up on our results is currently not possible due
to the ongoing pandemic, nor did the Editor or other Reviewers request it, we agree
that future work can and should build on the findings reported in this paper to further
understand this phenomenon. Given the current situation as well as our responses to
your concerns below, we hope that you will agree with us that it is better to publish
these findings (with limitations clearly stated) to get them out to the scientific
community rather than wait possibly multiple years to follow up on them.
Comment 2: I am not convinced of the findings from the follow-up analyses. These results
seem to be at odds with the results obtained from the experimental portion. The data
from the experimental portion is much cleaner and less prone to confounds. The follow-up
data is a single question rather than a more robust scale. The time frame spanned
an average of 4 months. The way in which the authors determined “baseline pain” is
confounded because they are asking participants to recall their baseline (rather than
actually measuring it at baseline). Additionally, by this point the sample size is
a mere 31 dyads.
Response 2: Thank you for your thoughtful reflection on the follow-up study. While
the results from the follow-up study differ from the results obtained in the experimental
portion, we do not believe these findings are necessarily at odds with each other,
but rather that they may collectively paint a broader picture of the underlying phenomenon.
Previous research has demonstrated that emotional experiences can be regulated over
time such that some interventions do not confer immediate results, but benefit individuals
over time. For example, affect labeling is a form of emotion regulation that sometimes
confers no immediate benefits, but considerably benefits patients with phobia over
time (i.e. lower skin conductance response relative to other groups from immediate
posttest to 1-week posttest, but not from pretest to immediate posttest; Kircanski
et al., 2012).
We agree that it is not ideal that the time frame between the in-lab session and follow-up
survey varied so much across participants. However, we did rerun our analyses of the
follow-up study controlling for the amount of time that passed between the in-lab
manipulation and the completion of the follow-up survey and the results held. Specifically,
our finding regarding the effect of touch on current emotional pain was consistent
across these analyses: when controlling for pain at the time of the original emotional
event, touch significantly affected current emotional pain, regardless of whether
we controlled for time between the in-lab study and follow-up survey. This provides
us with some evidence that the time frame was not a major confound in this analysis.
We have added additional details to describe these analyses on page 19:
“In addition, when controlling for emotional pain at the time of the original emotional
event, b = 0.50, t(53.38) = 5.33, p < .001, 95% CI =[0.32, 0.69], and the amount of
time that passed between the in-lab manipulation and the completion of the follow-up
survey, b = -0.00, t(28.35) = -0.78, p = 0.44, 95% CI =[-0.01, 0.00], we still found
a significant effect of touch on current emotional pain, b = -0.58, t(29.05) = -2.24,
p = 0.03, 95% CI =[-1.09, -0.07].”
To address your concern about the baseline pain measure utilized in the follow-up
analyses as a covariate, we re-ran our analyses for this follow-up study using the
same baseline as we used in our main analyses (emotional pain at first recall which
is a composite of 6 items rather than a single item). We found the same pattern of
results when using our baseline measure of emotional pain from the experimental study
as we do when using the one item baseline measure from the follow-up study. We have
added a description of these analyses on page 19:
“Furthermore, when using our measure of emotional intensity from the in-lab portion
of the study (i.e. emotional pain at first recall), b = 0.44, t(57.32) = 3.81, p <
.001, 95% CI =[0.21, 0.66], instead of participants’ self-reported emotional pain
at the time of the original event assessed at follow-up, we still found a significant
effect of touch on current emotional pain, b = -0.59, t(29.76) = -2.15, p = 0.04 95%
CI =[-1.12, -0.05].”
We agree that the experimental portion of this project is more robust than the follow-up
study since we were better able to control for potential confounds in the lab, and
indeed the sample size was larger for the in-lab portion of the study. But we believe
that these preliminary results may shed light on how consoling touch is shaping emotional
experiences over time. We have been careful to frame the results of the follow-up
study as preliminary (page 6, page 21), but believe these results are worth sharing
since they align with other research on the delayed benefits of some forms of affect
regulation (Kircanski et al., 2012), and thus can inform future studies on this topic
of research.
Comment 3: The relationship quality measure and analyses are so lacking in detail
that it can’t be interpreted as is. Potentially this can be resolved simply by more
details in the manuscript. However, as it is reported in the manuscript now, it is
not appropriate for the analyses conducted.
Response 3: Thank you for flagging this area of our manuscript that could use more
details. As suggested, we have appropriately renamed the Funk Scale as the Couples
Satisfaction Index (CSI), included information about the version and number of items
that we used, and provided the full scale in our OSF repository, described on page
13:
“As a measure of relationship satisfaction, participants completed the 32-item version
of the Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI), which includes items such as “I have a warm
and comfortable relationship with my partner” and “I really feel like part of a team
with my partner” [38]. The full scale can be accessed through our OSF repository.”
We have additionally added more information about the models tested on page 15:
“Both of these models (assessing the effect of consoling touch on participants’ emotional
pain and comfort) were then re-run with participants’ relationship satisfaction (i.e.
CSI) as a possible moderator of the relationship between consoling touch and emotional
pain/comfort. These models included valence, touch, relationship satisfaction, and
interactions between them as predictors, and emotional pain at first recall as a covariate.
We followed up on significant interaction terms that included relationship satisfaction
by obtaining estimated marginal means for our model using the “emmeans” package. This
method uses the given model to approximate the outcome variable at different levels
of a continuous moderator, adjusting for other variables in a model [39].”
Comment 4: Because of these confounds, this paper is essentially contributing 1) the
experimental manipulation of negative memories was more distressing than neutral memories—no
condition (handholding) differences, 2) participants felt more comfort from spousal
handholding than from control (squeeze-ball) during both negative and emotional memories.
Neither of these findings are particularly noteworthy nor do they meaningfully contribute
to the literature.
Response 4: Respectfully, we disagree that these findings do not meaningfully contribute
to the literature. There is a huge literature on the effects of consoling touch on
physical pain, as well as the overlap between physical and emotional pain. However,
only a few studies have studied consoling touch in the context of emotional pain (e.g.
Kraus, 2019), and no work to date has examined whether consoling touch actually reduces
subjective feelings of emotional pain as with physical pain. In everyday life, we
often seek and provide emotional support using touch, and emotional pain has tremendous
impact on individuals’ wellbeing. We have added more discussion of why it is valuable
to examine how touch imparts emotional benefits in the introduction on page 3:
“Three out of four people report that their most painful life experience was emotional
in nature, rather than physically painful [1]. Emotional pain, defined as an unpleasant
feeling (or suffering) associated with a psychological, non-physical origin, often
stemming from thwarted psychological needs [2], undergirds a range of psychiatric
issues, including depression, anxiety, borderline personality disorder, and suicidal
ideation [3,4]. Given the prevalence of emotional pain, and the negative outcomes
associated with such pain, it is crucial to examine how individuals effectively cope
with and process it.
When we experience negative events or hardships, support from others can mitigate
the harmful effects of those experiences and buffer us from trauma or prolonged distress
[5]. For example, talking through our problems or finding a welcome distraction during
a tough time can be valuable in helping us to regulate our emotions [6]. But there
are also powerful forms of social support that more implicitly communicate understanding
and concern, such as when a loved one holds our hand [7,8]. This type of physical
support, often referred to as consoling touch, is observed across species and across
cultures [9], and has been shown to reliably reduce the experience of physical pain
[10–12]. Notably, however, research has yet to experimentally assess whether touch
reduces the subjective experience of emotional pain in the same way that it reduces
the subjective experience of physical pain.”
Intuitively, we may think that consoling touch reduces negative affect. On the contrary,
however, our findings suggest that touch does not reduce the subjective experience
of emotional pain as it does the subjective experience of physical pain. We have underscored
this point in our discussion on page 19-20:
“A robust body of work demonstrates that consoling touch can reduce the affective
experience of physical pain [10–12], and that physical pain and emotional pain share
a common neural system [22–24, c.f. 25]. Intuitively, then, we may assume that consoling
touch reduces subjective reports of emotional pain. Surprisingly, however, our results
indicate that consoling touch does not decrease the immediate subjective experience
of emotional pain relative to holding a squeeze ball in the presence of one’s romantic
partner. But, it does lead individuals to feel more comforted by their partner than
holding a squeeze ball, particularly when they have greater relationship satisfaction
with their partner. This finding is in line with other work suggesting that consoling
touch increases subjective feelings of comfort during emotional pain [28], and that
relationship satisfaction plays an important role in how we feel during consoling
touch [10].”
One reason this set of findings is noteworthy is that it highlights a possibly important
difference between emotional and physical pain. It might not be adaptive to fully
attenuate emotional pain, especially the deeply personal pain evoked in this study,
because feeling this pain may allow people to process these experiences and more successfully
cope with them over time. Meanwhile, it may be more adaptive to down-regulate the
experience of physical pain, particularly when people elect to experience that pain
in a lab setting. We elaborate on this idea on page 21:
“This set of findings suggests a potentially important difference between emotional
pain and physical pain in terms of how negative experiences are regulated in these
two contexts. During physical pain, it may be adaptive to down-regulate immediate
distress, particularly in a lab setting where the pain is not necessarily helpful
in recognizing and escaping some sort of threat. However, during emotional pain, down-regulating
immediate distress may not always be adaptive since such distress may be stemming
from personally meaningful events that need to be processed and reflected on over
time. Indeed, research suggests that individuals often feel motivated to experience
negative emotional states because they are helpful in navigating certain experiences
(e.g. anger when preparing for a conflict, sadness in coping with a loss), even if
those emotional states feel unpleasant [40–42]. Thus, subjective distress may be necessary
to some extent to process emotional memories in a way that supports adaptive long-term
outcomes and resolution. This idea is consistent with research demonstrating that
exposure to negative emotional stimuli (e.g. spiders for phobic patients) can be instrumental
in reflecting on and changing harmful cognitions associated with those stimuli over
time [43,44]. In other words, we may need to feel certain emotions in order to process
and learn from them, allowing us to heal and regulate over time.”
Comment 5: The introduction has some holes as it relates to the research questions
investigated. One of the primary stated hypotheses has to do with relationship quality,
however, I was greatly wanting for any literature that discusses this topic. The only
info listed is a study from nearly 15 years ago... This is not sufficient (other intro
holes are addressed in the minor concerns below).
Response 5: We believe it is a fairly intuitive hypothesis that handholding with one’s
relationship partner could be influenced by their satisfaction with that partner.
Indeed, studies involving romantic couples typically attain some measure of relationship
satisfaction/quality (e.g. Reddan, 2020; Conradi et al., 2020). We have clarified
this in the introduction, and included additional references to support this hypothesis
on page 5-6:
“Since relationship satisfaction often moderates the effect of social support on wellbeing
outcomes [29–31], including the effect of handholding on the experience of physical
pain [10], we additionally hypothesized that relationship satisfaction would play
a moderating role in the effect of touch on emotional pain and comfort, such that
greater relationship satisfaction would enhance the soothing effects of touch.”
Comment 6: Your sample size was vastly shrinking by the end of the study. It started
out at a decently sized 60 dyads, then down to 47 for the session one and two analyses
and finally only 31 dyads for the follow-up survey analyses. That is a pretty small
sample size for the follow-up analyses, which is where the contribution of this paper
hinges. Because of this, you are no longer examining your primary, controlled experiment
data. These follow-up analyses would require more power to detect true differences
because of the time elapsed and the self-report recollection nature of the reporting.
This is a major confound in your results. Rather than the current rationalization
for your sample size (and you can’t really conduct a power analysis to determine your
sample size, since that is done), I suggest you run and report an analysis of how
likely it was that you observed a significant effect, given your sample, and given
your expected effect sizes.
Response 6: We recognize that our sample size is on the smaller side, and this affects
our power to detect a true effect, but we hope that you can recognize the complexity
of our study design that made it challenging and costly to recruit additional participants.
We recruited 60 dyads, a total of 120 participants, for 2 separate in-lab sessions
that involved audio-visual recordings, neuroimaging (to collect pilot data for a future
study), and running separate but simultaneous protocols for the storyteller and listener.
Inevitably, we lost data points from this sample due to technological issues, failure
to follow study instructions, and scheduling issues, which we described on page 11:
“Participants were not invited to return to the lab for session 2 if the videos they
recorded at session 1 were unusable, either because of technological issues with the
recordings or because they did not follow the video prompt instructions. Ten couples
were not eligible for session 2 for this reason. Two additional couples dropped out
of the study after session 1 due to scheduling issues. One additional couple was removed
from analyses due to technological issues during session 2, leaving a total of 47
couples in the sample.”
As per request, post-hoc power analyses were conducted to gain an estimate of whether
our sample size was appropriate for the given statistical analyses. We used G*Power
for a repeated measures (fully within-subjects) model with 4 conditions (2 x 2 design
with valence and touch), a small-medium effect size of 0.25, power of 0.8, and alpha
of 0.05. Results indicate that the total sample size recommended is 24 individuals.
These analyses suggest that our final sample of 47 dyads for the in-lab study and
31 participants for the follow-up study are reasonable. Of course, future research
should aim to replicate these results with a larger sample, and thus we were careful
to frame our results as a first step (particularly the follow-up study results). Nonetheless,
we believe it is important to share these findings with the scientific community in
order to inform future research in this area (a point we elaborated on in Responses
2 and 4).
We have added some text on page 7 to remind our readers that we used a within-subjects
design, and thus are reasonably powered to detect an effect:
“The rationale for our sample size derives from recently published work on affective
touch [12,32,33]. Since these studies found effects of touch on pain with samples
of 16-43 dyads, we aimed to obtain a sample of 60 dyads for our within-subjects design.”
Comment 7: Continuing on with the follow-up analysis. It could be a pretty big confound
to ask people to retrospectively rate how they felt and then at the same time as asking
them how they felt now. This could greatly skew your baseline. Why not use the data
you already have from them for when they reported how they felt during the experiment
as the baseline rather than forcing them to recall how they felt and then imposing
that as a baseline?
Response 7: Thank you for this suggestion. As described in Response 2, we re-ran our
analyses for this follow-up study using the same baseline as we used in our main analyses
as you recommended. We found the same pattern of results when using our baseline measure
of emotional pain from the experimental study as we do when using the one item baseline
measure from the follow-up study. We have added a description of these analyses on
page 19:
“Furthermore, when using our measure of emotional intensity from the in-lab portion
of the study (i.e. emotional pain at first recall), b = 0.44, t(57.32) = 3.81, p <
.001, 95% CI =[0.21, 0.66], instead of participants’ self-reported emotional pain
at the time of the original event assessed at follow-up, we still found a significant
effect of touch on current emotional pain, b = -0.59, t(29.76) = -2.15, p = 0.04 95%
CI =[-1.12, -0.05].”
Comment 8: A major concern is the way the relationship quality measure is used (potentially
just the way you report it?). In the intro, measures, and in the results sections,
info is severely lacking. In the measures you only state two items. Was the full scale
not used? If not, your relationship quality analyses are greatly confounded; what
is the point of having the scale if you only used two items from it? If so, these
sections need to be clarified. What are your reliability stats, means, SD, etc. Additionally,
how did you use the scale in your analyses? From the figure it looks like you classified
couples into high, med, and low RQ… But HOW? How many of your participant-couples
fit into each category? Is that same classification used for all your analyses? Additionally,
the “Funk measure’s” name is the “Couple Satisfaction Index” or CSI.
Response 8: Thank you for flagging that we should be referring to Funk’s scale as
the Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI). As described in Response 3, we now refer to
the Funk Scale as the CSI in the paper. We have also included information about the
version and number of items that we used, and provided the full scale in our OSF repository,
described on page 13. As noted with bolded text below, the items we provide in the
paper are simply examples of the scale since the full scale is cited in our references
and freely retrievable online (which we now direct our readers to in our OSF repository).
“As a measure of relationship satisfaction, participants completed the 32-item version
of the Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI), which includes items such as “I have a warm
and comfortable relationship with my partner” and “I really feel like part of a team
with my partner” [38]. The full scale can be accessed through our OSF repository.”
We have additionally added more information about the models tested on page 15:
“Both of these models (assessing the effect of consoling touch on participants’ emotional
pain and comfort) were then re-run with participants’ relationship satisfaction (i.e.
CSI) as a possible moderator of the relationship between consoling touch and emotional
pain/comfort. These models included valence, touch, relationship satisfaction, and
interactions between them as predictors, and emotional pain at first recall as a covariate.”
Please note that couples were not classified into high, medium, and low relationship
satisfaction categories. Rather, we used estimated marginal means to approximate what
participants’ comfort ratings would be at high (mean +1 standard deviation), medium
(mean), and low (mean -1 standard deviation) values of relationship satisfaction,
after accounting for the other variables in the model. This is a way to break down
interactions with a continuous moderating variable without categorically binning dyads
into relationship satisfaction groups. We have included reference to the specific
package we used (please note that you can review our analyses by viewing our code
on OSF), as well as a brief description of this method with citation in our manuscript
on page 15:
“We followed up on significant interaction terms that included relationship satisfaction
by obtaining estimated marginal means for our model using the “emmeans” package. This
method uses the given model to approximate the outcome variable at different levels
of a continuous moderator, adjusting for other variables in a model [39].”
Comment 9: Your research paradigm could use some bolstering in how imagining experiences
or memories is an appropriate research method. I am not saying it is not appropriate.
I am just recommending you address this and justify it with relevant literature. I
have listed some suggested references at the bottom that you may want to look into.
Response 9: Thank you for noting this area for clarification. Participants in our
study did not simply imagine negative experiences - they were recounting their own
past experiences. We did not ask them to imagine what it was like to experience something,
but rather to thoroughly describe how a past event from their own life made them feel,
and to reflect on how it makes them feel to think about it now. We have expanded on
this description on page 8:
“Participants were asked to focus their stories on how they felt at the time of the
event, how they dealt with their feelings, and how they feel about the event now.
To clarify, participants were not imagining emotional pain. Rather, they were being
asked to reflect on and relive their own personal emotionally painful experiences
by describing negative events from their past and the feelings those memories brought
up in detail [34,35]. This method for manipulating emotion is consistent with countless
studies that have used writing or talking about past emotional experiences to evoke
an emotional response [36,37], as opposed to using impersonal standardized stimuli
to induce negative affect.”
Comment 10: You address how “it is unclear whether touch reduces emotional pain the
same as physical pain”. But you never detail why emotional pain matters. You should
provide more background and justification on why emotional pain is important and a
relevant construct separate from and beyond physical pain. Why should readers care
about emotional pain? Addressing this will give the readers a clearer view of why
this study matters.
Response 10: Thank you for flagging this portion of the introduction where additional
information could be helpful to our readers. We have expanded our discussion of emotional
pain and why it matters on page 3:
“Three out of four people report that their most painful life experience was emotional
in nature, rather than physically painful [1]. Emotional pain, defined as an unpleasant
feeling (or suffering) associated with a psychological, non-physical origin, often
stemming from thwarted psychological needs [2], undergirds a range of psychiatric
issues, including depression, anxiety, borderline personality disorder, and suicidal
ideation [3,4]. Given the prevalence of emotional pain, and the negative outcomes
associated with such pain, it is crucial to examine how individuals effectively cope
with and process it.
When we experience negative events or hardships, support from others can mitigate
the harmful effects of those experiences and buffer us from trauma or prolonged distress
[5]. For example, talking through our problems or finding a welcome distraction during
a tough time can be valuable in helping us to regulate our emotions [6]. But there
are also powerful forms of social support that more implicitly communicate understanding
and concern, such as when a loved one holds our hand [7,8]. This type of physical
support, often referred to as consoling touch, is observed across species and across
cultures [9], and has been shown to reliably reduce the experience of physical pain
[10–12]. Notably, however, research has yet to experimentally assess whether touch
reduces the subjective experience of emotional pain in the same way that it reduces
the subjective experience of physical pain.”
Comment 11: Switch the order in presentation of method section. Logically before participants
tell about their stories, we should be told how they chose which stories to tell.
Response 11: Thank you for this suggestion. We have switched the order of presentation
of this information on page 7-8:
“Then, the person assigned to receive support recounted stories about past experiences.
For these stories, participants began by completing a form that allowed us to select
which negative stories they would recount based on: (a) whether the experience was
emotionally painful at the time of the event and (b) whether they were comfortable
discussing the experience on camera. This form is available on our OSF repository
(titled: “Story Selection”). After selecting which stories they would share, participants
recounted a total of 4-5 unrehearsed stories, each lasting about 3 minutes, as we
video recorded them.”
Comment 12: How was the prompt on the computer monitor programmed/controlled? Did
you use e-prime or some other software that ran automatically? Did you have RA’s running
it in the background?
Response 12: Thank you for noting this area for clarification. We have updated our
description of Session 1 on page 8 to include more information:
“Reminders for each prompt were presented via Qualtrics. Once the participant was
ready to tell their story, they would flip a 3-minute hourglass to help them keep
track of time and speak towards the camera. After each story, they responded to questions
on Qualtrics about how they felt while sharing the stories.”
We have additionally added information on page 9 about how data was presented at Session
9:
“Prior to each video, they were cued via PsychoPy to either hold hands or hold a squeeze
ball… After the rest, they heard a beep that cued them to turn their attention to
their laptops to answer questions via Qualtrics about their feelings...”
Comment 13: p.7 you describe how participants fill out a form which RA’s then selected
from to determine which story they would tell. More detail on this form should be
included. Possibly just including the form as supplementary material.
Response 13: We have added this form under “Study Materials” in our OSF repository,
and added a sentence on page 8 to alert the readers of this:
“This form is available on our OSF repository under Study Materials (titled: “Story
Selection”).”
Comment 14: p.8, first paragraph: What is meant by “After each story, they responded
to questions on the computer monitor about how they felt while sharing the stories.”?
Is this part of questions reported in your measures? If so, which measure are you
referring to? Or are these questions something different? If so, what does “how they
felt” mean or entail? How they felt is too vague.
Response 14: We have added a sentence on page 10 to clarify which questions we are
referring to here:
“After each video, participants underwent a minute of “rest” which involved closing
their eyes as they continued to hold their partner’s hand or hold the squeeze ball.
After the rest, they heard a beep that cued them to turn their attention to their
laptops to answer questions via Qualtrics about their feelings, including “how much
pain”, “how hurt”, “how sad”, “how angry”, “how much stress or anxiety”, “how emotional”,
and “how comforted by their partner” they felt on a Likert scale of 1 to 10. The first
six items were used to measure “emotional pain during the task”, whereas the last
item measured “comfort during the task” (see Measures for more detail).”
Comment 15: p.8 “two experimenters independently watched the set of videos… and selected
two negative videos for viewing in session two”. Were there specific coding by the
RA’s to determine if they were emotional? What were the criteria the RA’s used for
this decision? Was there any interrater reliability scores you can report? You should
have some sort of justification on how their subjective opinion on “emotional” be
validated or justified?
Response 15: Thank you for flagging this area of the methods that could use more detail.
We have updated the manuscript on page 9 to offer more details about this rating process,
and have shared our full protocol for this procedure in our OSF repository (SIC_Pre-Session2_Protocol.pdf).
“Before session 2, two experimenters independently watched the set of videos to ensure
participants appropriately followed the study instructions. As they watched each video,
they were asked to provide an overall rating on a scale of 1-10 (10 being the highest)
based on the following question: “To what extent did the participant experience emotional
pain in the video?” For videos to be considered similar enough for our experimental
manipulation in session 2, ratings had to be within at least two points of each other
(e.g. 9 and 7). Each rater selected the two videos that they rated as most similar
based on the above question. Videos additionally had to be approximately the same
length (within 18 s, about 10 seconds of the total video length). If the two raters
agreed on which two videos were most similar, those videos were prepared for use in
session 2. If the raters disagreed (approximately 22% of the time), a third rater
was asked to provide a rating. If no consensus was reached, or no two videos approximately
matched on emotionality and length, the couple was excluded from participating in
session 2 (see Exclusion Criteria for details).”
Comment 16: Measures section, p. 11 you use the term “’relationship’ between participants’
distress and comfort”. This term should be ‘relation’. Relationship means that thing
between real people. Inanimate objects have a ‘relation’ not a ‘relationship’. Especially
since you have “relationship quality” as a primary term in your study. That could
get confusing.
Response 16: We have rephrased the sentence, now on page 12:
“To test the association between participants’ emotional pain and comfort, we examined
the correlation between these variables in each condition.”
Comment 17: I am guessing that your Comfort scale is on the same 1-10 scale as your
distress scales was? This should be clarified in that section.
Response 17: We have updated the sentence on page 12:
“Meanwhile, “comfort during the task” was assessed with a single item asking participants
how comforted they felt by their romantic partners as they recalled each memory on
a scale of 1 to 10.”
Reviewer #2:
Comment 1: This paper, “The Comfort in Touch: Immediate and Lasting Effects of Handholding
on Emotional Pain” investigates whether handholding (from a romantic partner) alleviates
negative affect associated with the recall of negative life experiences. The authors
test for effects in static self-report measures collected both immediately after emotion
induction and a few months later. The authors find an effect of handholding on emotional
distress in the follow-up period, but not during the task. Handholding did, however,
increase feelings of comfort during the task.
The subject of this paper interesting,
the paradigm novel, and the results push research on the protective effects of touch
forward. The study is well powered and gender balanced (which is rare for touch-analgesia
research). I do, however, think it is problematic to rely entirely on subjective self-reports
especially when it is noted that NIRs data were collected. Self-report measures are
easily influenced by experimental demand characteristics and also do not give us much
insight into neuropsychological mechanisms. Furthermore, the stimuli are complex time
series data but psychological activity during stimulus presentation is entirely disregarded.
These interesting and dynamic stimuli are instead reduced to single self-report values
post trial. I think the paper requires some major revisions, and I expand upon this
in detail below.
Response 1: We thank you for your feedback and the suggestions for improving upon
our manuscript. We agree that neuroimaging data has the potential to inform the mechanisms
underlying these phenomena, which is why we collected this pilot neural data, and
why we aim to extend this line of research using fMRI. However, please keep in mind
that NIRS does not provide access to subcortical neural regions that are informative
with regards to the experience of emotional pain, and thus our best measure of emotional
pain in this study is from participants’ self-report. Additionally, even though self-report
measures are influenced by demand characteristics, we’d like to point out that participants
did not rate their pain as lower in the consoling touch condition than the pain control
condition (i.e. holding a squeeze ball during a negative video). This finding suggests
that participants are not simply telling us what we want to hear in this study, since
this result is actually counter to our hypothesis. And while neuroimaging data is
important in helping us unpack mechanisms, we do not believe that what participants
tell us they are feeling should be wholly disregarded in favor of what their brains
may tell us. Self-report and neuroimaging data can simply reveal different parts of
the bigger picture, and our study takes a first step towards painting that picture.
We also appreciate your point regarding the use of a single post-trial measure as
opposed to continuous self-report across stimuli presentation. We considered attaining
a continuous measure of emotionality across the task, but we find that this measurement
approach can be distracting from the task itself, especially given that participants
are simultaneously reliving negative personal experiences, and holding their partners
hand (depending on the condition). We wanted participants to be able to immerse themselves
in this experience that involves both pain from the task and comfort from their partner,
instead of having their attention pulled away by our questions. Thus, we opted to
only ask for ratings at the end of each trial.
Comment 2: This paper is conceptualized as a pain paper, but pain is not studied here.
This becomes increasingly problematic as the paper progresses. We are set up to think
this paradigm will be nearly identical to those in the pain-domain, however, the stimuli
here are high-dimensional personal narrative stories 2-3 min long, whereas pain stimuli
tend to be ~7-11 sec stimulations, repeated over many trials with controllable intensity.
Then when the results are reported and discussed terms like “emotional pain” and “distress”
are sometimes used interchangeably and sometimes used to mean different things. I
do appreciate that the authors situated this paper within the context of touch-analgesia
research, but I think the introduction does not do justice to the way this paper can
converse with that research. I think this happens because negative affect is equated
with pain, instead of interrogating both phenomena for what they really are in order
to understand the complexity of human experience. I am not suggesting that the connections
made with pain research be discarded, but instead, be discussed more critically. For
example, some analgesia papers do not just study “pain intensity” but also “pain unpleasantness”
which can be considered a more “affective” component of pain than the intensity measure.
I think this is a noteworthy bridge to your investigation which explores “unpleasantness”
so to speak, but not within any context of physical pain.
Response 2: Thank you for noting an area of the paper that is in need of further clarification.
We have more clearly laid out our research question regarding emotional pain in the
introduction on page 3, including a precise definition of emotional pain:
“Three out of four people report that their most painful life experience was emotional
in nature, rather than physically painful [1]. Emotional pain, defined as an unpleasant
feeling (or suffering) associated with a psychological, non-physical origin, often
stemming from thwarted psychological needs [2], undergirds a range of psychiatric
issues, including depression, anxiety, borderline personality disorder, and suicidal
ideation [3,4]. Given the prevalence of emotional pain, and the negative outcomes
associated with such pain, it is crucial to examine how individuals effectively cope
with and process it.
When we experience negative events or hardships, support from others can mitigate
the harmful effects of those experiences and buffer us from trauma or prolonged distress
[5]. For example, talking through our problems or finding a welcome distraction during
a tough time can be valuable in helping us to regulate our emotions [6]. But there
are also powerful forms of social support that more implicitly communicate understanding
and concern, such as when a loved one holds our hand [7,8]. This type of physical
support, often referred to as consoling touch, is observed across species and across
cultures [9], and has been shown to reliably reduce the experience of physical pain
[10–12]. Notably, however, research has yet to experimentally assess whether touch
reduces the subjective experience of emotional pain in the same way that it reduces
the subjective experience of physical pain.”
Additionally, we now include a brief discussion on page 4 of the sensory versus affective
components of physical pain (see bolded text), and describe where emotional pain fits
into this picture:
“A body of research suggests that physical pain and emotional pain share a common
neural system [22–24], although the extent of this overlap is still debated. Specifically,
while physical pain versus no physical pain, and emotional pain (i.e. social rejection)
versus no emotional pain are differentiated in the same regions of the brain, physical
and emotional pain are represented differently in those regions [25]. Nonetheless,
such neural overlap between physical and emotional pain suggests that touch may similarly
regulate physical and emotional pain. Additionally, while physical pain and emotional
pain differ insofar as physical pain has a sensory component (e.g. stinging, burning)
[26] and emotional pain stems from psychological events rather than physical stimulation
[2], they both involve an affective component (e.g. unpleasantness, distress). Prior
research suggests that consoling touch reduces the affective component of physical
pain [27], suggesting that consoling touch also has the potential to reduce the subjective
unpleasantness associated with emotional pain.”
Comment 3: Remove the term “emotional pain” from this study entirely. It is not useful
to call it “pain” here, it just confuses it with physical pain. I think “distress”
is a good term to use throughout. Please operationalize this at the start, and use
it consistently (this is particularly confusing in Figures 1 and 3 where one talks
about ‘distress’ and the other ‘emotional pain’ but these seems to be the same if
not highly correlated measures).
Response 3: We appreciate this reviewer’s perspective, but argue that it is important
to keep the term “emotional pain” here. We do not think that readers will confuse
emotional pain with physical pain. Indeed, “emotional pain” is a term that is used
in common English to refer to suffering or distress related to emotional situations.
Moreover, a Google Scholar search of the term “emotional pain” yields over 68,000
entries. Thus, we are not the first to use this term and would like to keep it here
in order to connect with other prior literature that has used this same term (e.g.,
Shneidman, 1996, The Suicidal Mind). However, to more fully clarify how we are using
this term, we have now included a definition of this term in our introduction, and
more clearly delineated the reasons why we are focusing on emotional pain for this
study, shown in Response 2. We also agree that streamlining our language would be
helpful for the reader, so we have chosen to use “emotional pain” throughout, instead
of more vaguely using the term “pain” or “distress”.
Comment 4: At the end of the introduction revisit this sentence: “Given that physical
pain is often temporally bound (i.e. restricted to a certain amount of time), whereas
emotional pain is often more enduring, it is possible that physical and emotional
pain differ in terms of how consoling touch shapes their immediate and lasting experience.”
I think this is a great point, but the way this is written it sounds like it is the
*only* difference between them. There are other important differences relevant to
this study that can help us to better understand the mechanism supporting touch analgesia.
For example, physical pain requires nociceptive input (except in the case of chronic
pain). Touch could mediate effects in the periphery or could instead mediate effects
at a psychological level. Your study excludes the periphery and therefore could be
a nice new piece of evidence to put into conversation with the existing literature.
Response 4: Thank you for noting that there are several interesting distinctions between
physical and emotional pain. We have softened our claim in the introduction on page
5 to note that we are simply describing one notable difference that seems relevant
to our findings:
“One (though not the only) notable difference between emotional and physical pain
is that physical pain is often temporally bound (i.e. restricted to a certain amount
of time), whereas emotional pain is often more enduring. Thus, it is possible that
physical and emotional pain differ in terms of how consoling touch shapes their immediate
and lasting experience.”
Comment 5: On page 4 “A body of research suggests that physical pain and emotional
pain share a common neural system (Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004, 2005; Eisenberger,
Lieberman, & Williams, 2003).” The degree to which these two neural processes are
the same this is hotly debated, and it is necessary to acknowledge this debate with
appropriate citations for the opposing camp (i.e., Wager et al and all the letters
involved in the “Pain in the ACC?” debate) if you do want to discuss this work.
Response 5: Thank you for noting that mention of both sides of this argument would
be helpful here. We now speak to this debate on page 4, with additional information
about the similarities and differences between physical and emotional pain:
“A body of research suggests that physical pain and emotional pain share a common
neural system [22–24], although the extent of this overlap is still debated. Specifically,
while physical pain versus no physical pain, and emotional pain (i.e. social rejection)
versus no emotional pain are differentiated in the same regions of the brain, physical
and emotional pain are represented differently in those regions [25]. Nonetheless,
such neural overlap between physical and emotional pain suggests that touch may similarly
regulate physical and emotional pain. Additionally, while physical pain and emotional
pain differ insofar as physical pain has a sensory component (e.g. stinging, burning)
[26] and emotional pain stems from psychological events rather than physical stimulation
[2], they both involve an affective component (e.g. unpleasantness, distress). Prior
research suggests that consoling touch reduces the affective component of physical
pain [27], suggesting that consoling touch also has the potential to reduce the subjective
unpleasantness associated with emotional pain.”
Comment 6: Refrain from the term “consoling touch” when describing this investigation
— call it what it is — handholding, but link this to the wider concept of consoling
touch as appropriate.
Response 6: In order to connect this work with a larger literature on consoling touch
across both humans and animals, we think it is important to continue to use the term
“consoling touch” to describe the concept that we are examining here. In other words,
we think that our study is part of a broader discussion on how different kinds of
consoling touch can affect negative emotional experiences. Handholding is simply one
way to operationalize consoling touch, as opposed to other forms of touch such as
hugging or gentle stroking. We have clarified this point in the introduction on page
5 with the bolded text below:
“This study applied a novel approach to understanding the emotional benefits of touch
by examining how handholding with a romantic partner, one form of consoling touch,
shapes experiences of emotional pain and comfort during emotional recollection, as
well as how it shapes lasting emotional pain associated with emotional experiences.”
Comment 7: “This work suggests that handholding, especially with a romantic partner,
attenuates subjective distress associated with physical pain, as well as activation
in neural regions associated with threat responses (Coan et al., 2013; Coan et al.,
2006; Johnson et al., 2013), with some work suggesting that handholding is more effective
than other forms of touch, such as gentle stroking, in reducing subjective pain (Reddan,
Young, Falkner, López-Solà, & Wager, 2020).” I would be a little careful with the
last claim because that Reddan (2020) paper did not find significant differences between
the touch conditions on self-reported pain intensity.
Response 7: Thank you for noting this point. We have removed the latter claim on page
4, and included additional references:
“While research suggests that touch can increase positive feelings like security,
and decrease negative feelings like stress [19], the majority of research on the pain-relieving
effects of touch have focused on how consoling touch affects individuals experiencing
physical pain, such as treatment-related pain, or painful shocks administered in experimental
settings. This work suggests that handholding, especially with a romantic partner,
attenuates subjective distress associated with physical pain, as well as activation
in neural regions associated with threat responses [10–12,20,21].”
Comment 8: Consider adding evidence from other neuroimaging papers in addition to
the Kraus (2019) to give a more complete survey of what we know about handholding
analgesia. The Reddan (2020) paper in particular agrees with your hypothesis about
safety conceptualization you put forth in the discussion. There is also:
López-Solà, M., Geuter, S., Koban, L., Coan, J.A., Wager, T.D. (2019). Brain mechanisms
of social touch-induced analgesia. Pain. 160(9), 2072–2085.
Response 8: We have added reference to all three of these papers in our introduction
(Reference 20, 21, 28).
Comment 9: Personal narratives have a lot of variation both within a single story
and then also across participants. Emotional content and intensity will fluctuate
and sometimes people add a “silver lining” or what they learned from a trying event
at the end of their recollection which then can make the story somewhat “positive.”
In short, these stimuli are complex! But this complexity is not controlled for or
even assessed in any meaningful way. I appreciate that the authors attempted to control
for “emotional intensity” in their stimuli via a “memory-related distress” score collected
during visit 1, however, I am not convinced this control is valid.
Response 9: We appreciate the points you are making, but a natural consequence of
using naturalistic stimuli is that they will be more variable and complex than standardized
impersonal stimuli. We wanted to use personal stimuli for this study to more closely
approximate what it is like to receive emotional support in the form of touch in everyday
life. We often share our personal experiences with close others, and this may create
opportunities for them to offer support through consoling touch. We agree that emotional
content and intensity fluctuate between individuals, and between emotional events,
but this is why we used a within-subjects design. Since our measure of emotional pain
at first recall (previously called “memory-related distress”) captures how painful
each story was for participants without a touch manipulation, we believe this score
is the best way to control for variation in the emotional intensity of each video
(from participants’ own perspective).
We would also like to point out that we had independent raters view and rate the stories
to ensure they were not too dissimilar in terms of perceived emotional intensity before
inviting participants to return for participation in session 2. We have updated the
manuscript on page 9 to offer more details about this rating process, and have shared
our full protocol for this procedure in our OSF repository (SIC_Pre-Session2_Protocol.pdf).
“Before session 2, two experimenters independently watched the set of videos to ensure
participants appropriately followed the study instructions. As they watched each video,
they were asked to provide an overall rating on a scale of 1-10 (10 being the highest)
based on the following question: “To what extent did the participant experience emotional
pain in the video?” For videos to be considered similar enough for our experimental
manipulation in session 2, ratings had to be within at least two points of each other
(e.g. 9 and 7). Each rater selected the two videos that they rated as most similar
based on the above question. Videos additionally had to be approximately the same
length (within 18 s, about 10 seconds of the total video length). If the two raters
agreed on which two videos were most similar, those videos were prepared for use in
session 2. If the raters disagreed (approximately 22% of the time), a third rater
was asked to provide a rating. If no consensus was reached, or no two videos approximately
matched on emotionality and length, the couple was excluded from participating in
session 2 (see Exclusion Criteria for details).”
Comment 10: Being that the “memory-related distress” score and the “task-related distress”
score are the same questions asked of the same people about the same stimuli, I expect
these to be highly correlated. Are they? Please test for multicollinearity in all
the models which you use the “memory-related distress” score as a nuisance regressor
(or the measure where they “recalled how they felt at the time of the event”). If
it is correlated with your regressor of interest then it is possible your reported
tests are invalid (and this might not be a bad thing — maybe your null findings are
actually detectable).
Response 10: Thank you for this feedback. Yes, memory-related distress (now referred
to as “emotional pain at first recall”) and task-based distress (now referred to as
“emotional pain during the task”) were correlated (r = 0.73 in the consoling touch
condition and r = 0.55 in the emotional pain only condition). This makes sense since
baseline covariates tend to be correlated with outcome variables (e.g. pre and post-intervention
math scores should be correlated). However, multicollinearity is an issue that characterizes
redundancy in predictor variables (James et al. 2014). Task-related distress (i.e.
emotional pain during the task) is our outcome variable, not a predictor variable.
Thus, correlation between task-related distress and memory-related distress does not
cause an issue of multicollinearity. For thoroughness, we went ahead and tested for
multicollinearity in all of our models (using the “car” package in R), and found no
evidence of multicollinearity (all VIF scores were < 4).
Comment 11: Can you attempt to provide some kind of validity check across the stimuli
alone? You can, for example, get independent raters to rate the videos moment-by-moment.
They you can see if these stimuli are balanced across participants. You can look for
the amount of negativity vs like “silver lining” in the negative stories and test
if there are outlier stories, etc. This might require you collecting more data on
these videos, and I know that can be a big and annoying ask, but I think it is important
and won’t be wasted. You can use those “normative” ratings in future studies, etc.
Also, if you know these stimuli are balanced you can eliminate the use of that “memory-related
distress” nuisance regressor which might be watering down your effects.
Response 11: Thank you for flagging this area of the methods that could use clarification.
As described in Response 9, we had our research team rate these videos on overall
emotional intensity to help with balancing the conditions. As you brought up in Comment
9, it is very difficult to perfectly match stimuli across conditions when using personal
stimuli, but we have argued that there are benefits to using personalized stimuli
that justify our use of them in this study. We know our stimuli are not perfectly
matched, but this is why we controlled for memory-related distress (i.e. emotional
pain at first recall) in our analyses. We believe this measure was the best way to
control for variation in the emotional intensity of each video, from participants’
own perspective.
Comment 12: Was the follow up survey a post hoc addition or part of the original study
design? Please specify both at the time when it is first introduced and when it is
described in the methods. Address the length of time/gap as well, earlier on.
Response 12: We have clarified this point on page 6 and 11:
“Finally, participants completed an exploratory follow-up survey to assess whether
there were any lasting effects of handholding on the experience of emotional pain.
In other words, we aimed to test whether emotional memories paired with handholding
in the lab would later be recalled with less emotional pain than those that were paired
with holding a squeeze-ball. Given the exploratory nature of this follow-up study,
this data is considered preliminary in elucidating how consoling touch potentially
shapes the lasting experience of emotional pain.”
“To examine potential lasting effects of consoling touch on emotional pain, participants
who received support completed a brief exploratory follow-up survey. These surveys
were sent out electronically after we completed in-lab data collection for all of
our dyads as an exploratory addition to our investigation. Thus, participants completed
the survey between 1.28 and 7.82 months after session 2 (M = 4.01 months). ”
Comment 13: Can you clarify whether the supportive partner could hear or see the story/stimulus
beyond the curtain and importantly whether the support receiver was aware of this
(could the support receiver possibly think “We are watching this/experiencing this
together?”)
Response 13: Thank you for noting this area for clarification. Yes, the support giver
could hear and see the stimuli, as the stimuli presentation screen was situated in
the center of the room across from both participants. Thus, the support receiver was
aware that they were experiencing the stimuli with their partner. However, the stimuli
was specifically personal to the support receiver (e.g. a time they felt rejected
or alone), so the support giver would have had a very different experience of the
stimuli than the support receiver. Participants knew which one of them was the “storyteller”
and the “listener”, so there would have been no confusion about who was in a position
to provide support. We have added some information about this on page 10 (see bolded
text):
“Throughout the task, participants sat on opposite sides of a curtain from each other
to prevent them from communicating verbally or through other non-verbal cues (e.g.
body language, facial expressions). Both participants could hear and see the videos
on a single screen on the wall across from them, such that the support receiver and
support giver experienced the stimuli simultaneously. During the two hand-holding
conditions, they held hands through the curtain. Thus, participants were aware of
the presence of their partner in all four conditions, but their physical contact was
limited to the two conditions that included touch.”
Comment 14: “The final sample included predominantly Caucasian (39%) and Asian/Asian
American (34%) participants (mean age = 21.75 years). “ Please give the complete
racial breakdown.
Response 14: We have added detailed reporting of the ethnicity breakdown in the manuscript
on page 6. Please note that these percentages changed a bit when creating a separate
category for multiracial participants.
“The final sample included approximately 30% White, 32% Asian/Asian American, 11%
Latino/a, 2% Filipino/a, 2% Black, and 11% multiracial participants. The remaining
participants chose another identity or preferred not to answer.”
Comment 15: Can you clarify if you measured gender identity or biological sex?
Response 15: We measured gender identity through the following question: “What is
your gender?” Thus, we are careful to use the term gender rather than sex throughout
the manuscript.
Comment 16: make clear if the scales were Likert or VAS (It seems like Likert).
Response 16: We have specified the use of Likert scales on page 10:
“...including “how much pain”, “how hurt”, “how sad”, “how angry”, “how much stress
or anxiety”, “how emotional”, and “how comforted by their partner” they felt on a
Likert scale of 1 to 10.”
Comment 17: The term “memory-related distress” is confusing and not ideal.
Response 17: We agree and have replaced this term throughout with “emotional pain
at first recall.” Please see our note (“Note to Editor and all Reviewers”) in the
beginning of this document for details.
Comment 18: For clarity, please use a different shorthand for your conditions - not
“touch-pain condition” etc., to indicate this paradigm is about emotional distress
and not physical pain.
Response 18: We agree and have replaced these condition names throughout to minimize
confusion (see below text from page 9-10 for example):
“Prior to each video, they were cued via PsychoPy to either hold hands or hold a squeeze
ball such that participants underwent four conditions in a randomized order: (a) hand-holding
during a negative video (i.e. consoling touch condition); (b) hand-holding during
a neutral video (i.e. touch only control condition); (c) holding a squeeze ball during
a negative video (emotional pain only control condition); and (d) holding a squeeze
ball during a neutral video (full control condition).”
Comment 19: Please include a results table for the non-significant GLMM gender test
in your supplementary
Response 19: We have added these results to our Supplementary Materials and referenced
these materials on page 14.
Comment 20: Please show the data distribution/individual data points in you bar plots
Response 20: We have revised figures 1 and 3 to use box plots instead of bar plots
to give readers a better sense of the distribution. See below (Figures 1A, 1B, and
3):
Comment 21: Report complete stats for the following two post hoc tests that only report
p-values “when additionally controlling for the amount of time that passed between
the in-lab manipulation and the completion of the follow-up survey, this effect of
touch on current emotional pain did not change (p = 0.03). Relationship satisfaction
was not a significant moderator of this effect (p = 0.67).”
Response 21: We have expanded the description of these results on page 18-19:
“When including relationship satisfaction as a moderator in the model, we did not
find a significant interaction between touch and relationship satisfaction, b = -0.02,
t(28.06) = -0.75, p = 0.46, 95% CI = [-0.06, 0.02].
In addition, when controlling for emotional pain at the time of the original emotional
event, b = 0.50, t(53.38) = 5.33, p < .001, 95% CI =[0.32, 0.69], and the amount of
time that passed between the in-lab manipulation and the completion of the follow-up
survey, b = -0.00, t(28.35) = -0.78, p = 0.44, 95% CI =[-0.01, 0.00], we still found
a significant effect of touch on current emotional pain, b = -0.58, t(29.05) = -2.24,
p = 0.03, 95% CI =[-1.09, -0.07]. Furthermore, when using our measure of emotional
intensity from the in-lab portion of the study (i.e. emotional pain at first recall),
b = 0.44, t(57.32) = 3.81, p < .001, 95% CI =[0.21, 0.66], instead of participants’
self-reported emotional pain at the time of the original event assessed at follow-up,
we still found a significant effect of touch on current emotional pain, b = -0.59,
t(29.76) = -2.15, p = 0.04 95% CI =[-1.12, -0.05].”
Comment 22: There is a history of gender effects in the touch-pain studies you mention
(i.e., Coan and Lopez-Sola studies use ONLY female samples, Reddan found gender effects,
etc). You found no effects of gender. Please consider discussing this (can be brief).
Response 22: We have added a brief description about this point on page 20 of the
discussion:
“We found no effect of gender on our outcome variables. Given that the majority of
touch studies have only examined female participants [10,12,21], with some work finding
gender differences during the experience of physical pain [20], further research is
needed to clarify how gender shapes the outcome of consoling touch.”
Comment 23: Please reword the following sentence, it is confusing: “Indeed, research
suggests that individuals are often motivated to experience certain emotional states
even if they feel hedonically unpleasant, because they are in some way instrumental
to feel.”
Response 23: Thank you for this note. We have edited this sentence on page 21:
“Indeed, research suggests that individuals often feel motivated to experience negative
emotional states because they are helpful in navigating certain experiences (e.g.
anger when preparing for a conflict, sadness in coping with a loss), even if those
emotional states feel unpleasant [40–42].”
Comment 24: Please reword “In other words, we may need to feel certain emotions in
order to process and overcome them. “Maybe make this sentence more concrete. I am
not sure emotions are something to be “overcome” but maybe bad beliefs or persistent
memories underlying the emotions are?
Response 24: Thank you for this note. We have edited this sentence on page 21:
“In other words, we may need to feel certain emotions in order to process and learn
from them, allowing us to heal and regulate over time.”
Comment 25: Does your IRB require that you restrict data sharing (as described on
pg 5 of the compiled manuscript in the "where they data may be found" section)? If
not, consider changing this so you can share the data for this or future projects.
Response 25: Our IRB requires that we restrict data sharing as we have, but we have
already deposited our data (which are time-stamped) on OSF so that they can be easily
shared upon request with other researchers using a private link: https://osf.io/9wbua/?view_only=de32be8c75a94f1e9d3bdb9c7c21f19f
Comment 26: Please considering including the data that was used to make the composite
scores in the uploaded/shareable data.
Response 26: We have updated the datafile on our OSF repository to include the individual
items used to create composite scores.
Comment 27: When you used Google Hangouts to monitor your participants, were you able
to do this in a way that prevented data-sharing with Google? I haven’t seen Hangouts
used like this before, and this is just one of those things that makes me think about
how our research practices have to evolve alongside tech/data-mining companies.
Response 27: We did not take any steps to prevent data-sharing with Google, as I’m
not sure there is a way to do this while using Google’s services, or that there is
a similar free service that would escape this issue. Our participants were fully aware
of us using Google Hangouts to monitor them, and agreed to this use of Google Hangouts
during the study session. It’s a great point that you make though regarding how our
research practices going forward will have to attend to the way that these companies
mine data, and we thank you for noting this.
Comment 28: The mention of the NIRs data being saved for a different study creates
concern about dual publication.
Response 28: Thank you for raising this concern. Our NIRs data were used for exploratory
analyses in preparation for a future fMRI study, and we do not have plans to publish
this pilot data. To clarify this, we have rephrased the sentence on page 6:
“These neural data were designed to serve as exploratory pilot data for a future neuroimaging
study.”
Reviewer #3:
Comment 1: The manuscript is well-written and provides novel insights on the effect
of touch on distress and comfort and lasting pain during emotional recollection. I
have several issues that should be addressed.
Response 1: We thank you for your feedback and the suggestions for improving upon
our manuscript. We have taken steps to address your concerns, detailed below.
Comment 2: Generally, the introduction mostly sounds reasonable. However, the manuscript
deals with memory-related pain/distress but this issue was not connected to the proposed
rational.
Response 2: Thank you for noting this area for clarification. Participants in our
study were asked to thoroughly describe how a past event from their own life made
them feel, and to reflect on how it makes them feel to think about it now. Thus, our
aim in using their own personal memories was to manipulate their current emotional
state. We have expanded on this description on page 7 to more clearly describe why
we used their memories to induce emotional pain:
“Participants were asked to focus their stories on how they felt at the time of the
event, how they dealt with their feelings, and how they feel about the event now.
To clarify, participants were not imagining emotional pain. Rather, they were being
asked to reflect on and relive their own personal emotionally painful experiences
by describing negative events from their past and the feelings those memories brought
up in detail [34,35]. This method for manipulating emotion is consistent with countless
studies that have used writing or talking about past emotional experiences to evoke
an emotional response [36,37], as opposed to using impersonal standardized stimuli
to induce negative affect.”
We have also added additional motivation for our investigation of emotional pain and
connected it with the existing literature on page 3:
“Three out of four people report that their most painful life experience was emotional
in nature, rather than physically painful [1]. Emotional pain, defined as an unpleasant
feeling (or suffering) associated with a psychological, non-physical origin, often
stemming from thwarted psychological needs [2], undergirds a range of psychiatric
issues, including depression, anxiety, borderline personality disorder, and suicidal
ideation [3,4]. Given the prevalence of emotional pain, and the negative outcomes
associated with such pain, it is crucial to examine how individuals effectively cope
with and process it.
When we experience negative events or hardships, support from others can mitigate
the harmful effects of those experiences and buffer us from trauma or prolonged distress
[5]. For example, talking through our problems or finding a welcome distraction during
a tough time can be valuable in helping us to regulate our emotions [6]. But there
are also powerful forms of social support that more implicitly communicate understanding
and concern, such as when a loved one holds our hand [7,8]. This type of physical
support, often referred to as consoling touch, is observed across species and across
cultures [9], and has been shown to reliably reduce the experience of physical pain
[10–12]. Notably, however, research has yet to experimentally assess whether touch
reduces the subjective experience of emotional pain in the same way that it reduces
the subjective experience of physical pain.”
Comment 3: The description of the analytical approach is very general and not specific
to the current study. For example, what residual distribution was assumed? What degrees
of freedom were used? What random effects/covariance structures were assumed?
Response 3: We have added more information about our models on page 14:
“For our analyses, we used the statistical package R (Version 1.2.1335) to create
linear mixed models (LMMs, i.e. multilevel regression) with participant ID as the
group level variable, fixed effects, and random intercepts. We used the “lmer” package
in R, which by default uses the Satterthwaite degrees of freedom method and bases
confidence intervals and p-values on the t-distribution.”
Comment 4: Please, provide a detailed description of the analysis regarding the gender
effect.
Response 4: We have added a full report of these analyses and results to our Supplementary
Materials and referenced these materials on page 14 of the manuscript.
- Attachments
- Attachment
Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx