Peer Review History

Original SubmissionJuly 3, 2020
Decision Letter - Zezhi Li, Editor

PONE-D-20-13201

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Turgeon,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 06 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.
  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.
  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Zezhi Li, Ph.D., M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for including a copy of your related published article and discussing it in your manuscript. Please remove blinded references to this other study and give the full reference for ref. 49 in your manuscript since review is not blinded in this journal.

3. We note that Figure 1 includes an image of a participant in the study. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual.

4. We note that Figure 1 in your submission contain copyrighted images. All PLOS content is published under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which means that the manuscript, images, and Supporting Information files will be freely available online, and any third party is permitted to access, download, copy, distribute, and use these materials in any way, even commercially, with proper attribution. For more information, see our copyright guidelines: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright.

We require you to either (1) present written permission from the copyright holder to publish these figures specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license, or (2) remove the figures from your submission:

4.1.         You may seek permission from the original copyright holder of Figure(s) [#] to publish the content specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license.

We recommend that you contact the original copyright holder with the Content Permission Form (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=7c09/content-permission-form.pdf) and the following text:

“I request permission for the open-access journal PLOS ONE to publish XXX under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CCAL) CC BY 4.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Please be aware that this license allows unrestricted use and distribution, even commercially, by third parties. Please reply and provide explicit written permission to publish XXX under a CC BY license and complete the attached form.”

Please upload the completed Content Permission Form or other proof of granted permissions as an "Other" file with your submission. 

In the figure caption of the copyrighted figure, please include the following text: “Reprinted from [ref] under a CC BY license, with permission from [name of publisher], original copyright [original copyright year].”

4.2.    If you are unable to obtain permission from the original copyright holder to publish these figures under the CC BY 4.0 license or if the copyright holder’s requirements are incompatible with the CC BY 4.0 license, please either i) remove the figure or ii) supply a replacement figure that complies with the CC BY 4.0 license. Please check copyright information on all replacement figures and update the figure caption with source information. If applicable, please specify in the figure caption text when a figure is similar but not identical to the original image and is therefore for illustrative purposes only.

5. We noted in your submission details that a portion of your manuscript may have been presented or published elsewhere.

"Our manuscript is part of a larger research project that allowed us to publish an article in Child Abuse & Neglect, which we uploaded in the Attach Files. The results related to the manuscript we are submitting to PLOS ONE are original. They have not been published and are not under consideration for publication elsewhere. Please find the details of its specific contributions in our cover letter, in which we explain why our manuscript does not constitute dual publication. "

Please clarify whether thispublication was peer-reviewed and formally published. If this work was previously peer-reviewed and published, in the cover letter please provide the reason that this work does not constitute dual publication and should be included in the current manuscript.

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for allowing me to review this interesting manuscript.

Overall, I found it be well written and topical. There is clear attention to detail.

I only have minor comments:

- The topic is important and will be of interest to the readership.

- The manuscript could use some greater detail regarding the implications of this research. What could these results mean for identifying and treating mothers who have experienced trauma and have difficulty with emotion recognition? Are there specific treatments for this deficit?

- The analysis and methodology generally seems appropriate, however, I defer to the other reviewers and editors on this point. Highlight that a path analysis where all variables are collected at a single time-point has limitations.

- It may be worth mentioning the “happy face advantage” (references below) in interpreting some of these results and biases. This is a common bias in facial recognition:

Leppanen, J., Tenhunen, M., & Hietanen, J. (2003). Faster choice reaction times to positive than negative facial expressions: The role of cognitive and motor processes. Emotion, 3, 315–326.

Lipp, O., Craig, B., & Dat, M. (2015). A happy face advantage with male caucasian faces: It depends on the company you keep. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 109–115.

Minor points:

- The first paragraph could possibly be stronger and longer.

- Consider deleting second last sentence of page six: “This paper aims…”

Reviewer #2: The objective of the research entitled " Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment" is to examine the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition during parenthood. The study is quite interesting and offered new understanding of the differential long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment. I only have a few questions. Below are several suggestions for a revision.

Points to address:

1. Regarding the results of the Paired t-test section, a full list of pair-tested comparisons was presented in Table 1. However, the results state that “mean accuracy was significantly lower for disgust and fear compared with all other emotions” and “anger was the second emotion with the highest accuracy scores, followed by sadness and surprise, respectively”. The t-test table tells which pair comparison is significant, from the t-values and Cohen’s d the reader can infer which emotion have the relative higher or lower accuracy, but it is not straightforward. It would be great if the authors can show the mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion in a table or bar plot.

2. Is there a reason why only univariate correlations with p-values smaller than .10 were integrated into the first tested model?

3. For controlling for multiple tests, p-value = .003 was used, I wonder what happens if a different threshold? Are the main results still holds?

4. In line 123, abbreviation CTQ used before first define it.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Revision 1

Enclosed please find our response to reviewers concerning our manuscript entitled “Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment”, which we are resubmitting to PLOS ONE with great hopes of achieving publication as an original contribution.

We first would like to thank the reviewers for their suggestions that have helped clarify and improve the paper. We would also like to thank the editor for giving us the opportunity to resubmit what we believe are important results, as they highlight how childhood maltreatment interferes with mothers’ ability to discriminate child facial expressions of emotion. For clarity, we first state the comments verbatim (in italics), followed by our response. The sections in red indicate changes that were made in the manuscript.

Reviews from the Edition Team

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.

Authors’ reply: We have made the necessary changes to ensure that our manuscript meets PLOS ONE’s style requirements.

2. Thank you for including a copy of your related published article and discussing it in your manuscript. Please remove blinded references to this other study and give the full reference for ref. 49 in your manuscript since review is not blinded in this journal.

Authors’ reply: We have removed blinded references and have added the full reference for ref. 49 in our manuscript (which is now ref. 51, after revisions).

3. We note that Figure 1 includes an image of a participant in the study and 4. We note that Figure 1 in your submission contain copyrighted images.

Authors’ reply: We have removed the Figure from our submission as it is not essential to the reader’s understanding of our paper. The figure provided an illustration of the images to which participants were exposed, but the description in text provides sufficient information and is usually found in other similar papers (Gibb et al., 2009).

5. We noted in your submission details that a portion of your manuscript may have been presented or published elsewhere.

Please clarify whether this publication was peer-reviewed and formally published. If this work was previously peer-reviewed and published, in the cover letter please provide the reason that this work does not constitute dual publication and should be included in the current manuscript.

Authors’ reply: Our publication in Child Abuse & Neglect was peer-reviewed and formally published [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104432]. Our cover letter addresses how our current manuscript does not constitute dual publication, as it extends our previous findings. We have added a sentence in our manuscript to clarify the difference between both manuscripts, see lines 125-128:

In a previous study, we found that mothers who had higher scores on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) scales had lower performance scores on the emotion recognition task [51]. Overall scores were created and used in analyses, for both childhood maltreatment experiences and emotion recognition performances. The current study seeks to further explore these results, by drawing conclusions about the differential effects of five maltreatment subtypes on mothers’ ability to recognize six emotions expressed on child faces. To our knowledge, no studies have examined associations between the processing of the six basic facial emotional expressions and the five most documented forms of maltreatment (physical, emotional and sexual abuse, physical and emotional neglect).

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly.

Authors’ reply: We have included captions for our Supporting Information files at the end of our manuscript.

Reviewer #1:

Thank you for allowing me to review this interesting manuscript.

Overall, I found it be well written and topical. There is clear attention to detail.

I only have minor comments:

- The topic is important and will be of interest to the readership.

Authors’ reply: Thank you, we appreciate this comment.

- The manuscript could use some greater detail regarding the implications of this research. What could these results mean for identifying and treating mothers who have experienced trauma and have difficulty with emotion recognition? Are there specific treatments for this deficit?

Authors’ reply: In response to this comment, we have added sentences in the Discussion section to clarify the implications of our research. The last paragraph now reads:

Our current findings expand these results by helping us recognize how each form of maltreatment interacts with the way mothers perceive and interpret their child’s signals. These findings may lead to the development of specific interventions that address the consequences of each form of maltreatment on emotion recognition. The results of our previous and current studies highlight the specific needs of parents with a history of maltreatment. General emotion recognition interventions may not be appropriate for this population. Rather, our results encourage a more specific approach that focuses on individual difficulties in recognizing and responding to emotions. For instance, Kolijn and colleagues [73] showed that video-feedback, an individualised attachment-based intervention aimed at enhancing the recognition of child emotional expressions and sensitive parenting, may improve mothers’ ability to process children’s emotions, by reducing the neural effort associated with this task. These results are encouraging as they suggest that the detrimental, long-lasting effects of maltreatment on emotion recognition can be reversed.

- The analysis and methodology generally seems appropriate, however, I defer to the other reviewers and editors on this point. Highlight that a path analysis where all variables are collected at a single time-point has limitations.

Authors’ reply: A path analysis on cross-sectional data has limits, particularly regarding data interpretation. Historically, a path analysis was used to test causal effects and therefore a longitudinal data estimate was required. However, this type of analysis is now widely used with cross-sectional data, which makes it possible to discuss correlations between variables in the model. One limitation would be that our conclusions are "correlational" rather than "causal". We have added the sentence below to highlight this limit, see lines 360-362.

Third, we conducted a path analysis on cross-sectional data using variables collected at a single time-point. Our results must be interpreted as correlations and do not imply causation.

- It may be worth mentioning the “happy face advantage” (references below) in interpreting some of these results and biases. This is a common bias in facial recognition:

Leppanen, J., Tenhunen, M., & Hietanen, J. (2003). Faster choice reaction times to positive than negative facial expressions: The role of cognitive and motor processes. Emotion, 3, 315–326.

Lipp, O., Craig, B., & Dat, M. (2015). A happy face advantage with male caucasian faces: It depends on the company you keep. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 109–115.

Authors’ reply: Thank you for this advice. We have mentioned the “happy face advantage” in our manuscript and have added both references to facilitate the interpretation of our results, see lines 288-291.

Minor points:

- The first paragraph could possibly be stronger and longer.

Authors’ reply: Thank you for this advice. We have made changes to the paragraph to make it stronger. The paragraph now reads:

Parental sensitivity refers to a parent’s ability to interpret child signals correctly and to offer an appropriate response [1]. This allows the establishment of a secure attachment relationship [1,2] and promotes the healthy development of young children [3,4]. Among other things, parental sensitivity is associated with positive socioemotional functioning and academic outcomes [5-6]. Conversely, a misinterpretation of the child’s needs or avoidance of caregiving responsibilities leads to important developmental consequences, as the child is unable to use his caregiver as a secure base to explore his environment and seek contact in case of distress [1,7,8].

- Consider deleting second last sentence of page six: “This paper aims…”

Authors’ reply: This sentence was removed.

Reviewer #2:

The objective of the research entitled " Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment" is to examine the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition during parenthood. The study is quite interesting and offered new understanding of the differential long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment. I only have a few questions. Below are several suggestions for a revision.

Points to address:

1. Regarding the results of the Paired t-test section, a full list of pair-tested comparisons was presented in Table 1. However, the results state that “mean accuracy was significantly lower for disgust and fear compared with all other emotions” and “anger was the second emotion with the highest accuracy scores, followed by sadness and surprise, respectively”. The t-test table tells which pair comparison is significant, from the t-values and Cohen’s d the reader can infer which emotion have the relative higher or lower accuracy, but it is not straightforward. It would be great if the authors can show the mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion in a table or bar plot.

Authors’ reply: In line with this comment, we have added a bar plot with the mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion (see fig. 1).

Fig 1. Mean accuracy and standard deviation for each emotion.

2. Is there a reason why only univariate correlations with p-values smaller than .10 were integrated into the first tested model?

Authors’ reply: This criterion is based on the work of Bendel & Afifi (1977) and Mickey & Greenland (1989) who show that a p-value cut-off of .05 often fails to identify important variables (see references below). We have added this sentence in our manuscript, see lines 215-216.

Bendel, R. B., & Afifi, A. A. (1977). Comparison of stopping rules in forward “stepwise” regression. Journal of the American Statistical association, 72(357), 46-53. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1977.10479905

Mickey, R. M., & Greenland, S. (1989). The impact of confounder selection criteria on effect estimation. American journal of epidemiology, 129(1), 125-137. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115101

3. For controlling for multiple tests, p-value = .003 was used, I wonder what happens if a different threshold? Are the main results still holds?

Authors’ reply: The p-value = .003 corresponds to the Bonferroni correction we applied, as mentioned on p.10. Only one paired comparison test has a p-value > 0.003 [Disgust-Fear p=.028]. All other results reach the conservative p-value < 0.003, which we used, and would therefore be retained in the absence of a Bonferroni correction.

4. In line 123, abbreviation CTQ used before first define it.

Authors’ reply: We have defined the abbreviation before using it.

We would like to thank both reviewers for their time in reading and reviewing our manuscript.

Attachments
Attachment
Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx
Decision Letter - Zezhi Li, Editor

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

PONE-D-20-13201R1

Dear Dr. Turgeon,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Zezhi Li, Ph.D., M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper is well written. The authors have addressed my queries adequately. I believe the paper makes a worthwhile contribution to the literature.

Reviewer #2: The authors have satisfactorily responded to all my questions and made the necessary changes to the manuscript. The revised version of the manuscript appears to be good. I do not have any other questions.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Formally Accepted
Acceptance Letter - Zezhi Li, Editor

PONE-D-20-13201R1

Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment

Dear Dr. Turgeon:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Zezhi Li

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Open letter on the publication of peer review reports

PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.

We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.

Learn more at ASAPbio .