Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Prediction of recidivism in a long-term follow-up of forensic psychiatric patients: Incremental effects of neuroimaging data

  • Carl Delfin ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    carl.delfin@gu.se

    Affiliations Centre for Ethics, Law and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, The Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden, Regional Forensic Psychiatric Clinic, Växjö, Sweden

  • Hedvig Krona,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Lund University, Faculty of Medicine, Department of Clinical Sciences Lund, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Lund, Sweden

  • Peter Andiné,

    Roles Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre for Ethics, Law and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, The Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden, Forensic Psychiatric Clinic, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden, Department of Forensic Psychiatry, National Board of Forensic Medicine, Gothenburg, Sweden

  • Erik Ryding,

    Roles Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Skåne University Hospital, Lund, Sweden

  • Märta Wallinius,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre for Ethics, Law and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, The Sahlgrenska Academy at University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden, Regional Forensic Psychiatric Clinic, Växjö, Sweden, Lund University, Faculty of Medicine, Department of Clinical Sciences Lund, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Lund, Sweden

  • Björn Hofvander

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Lund University, Faculty of Medicine, Department of Clinical Sciences Lund, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Lund, Sweden, Division of Forensic Psychiatry, Region Skåne, Trelleborg, Sweden

Abstract

One of the primary objectives in forensic psychiatry, distinguishing it from other psychiatric disciplines, is risk management. Assessments of the risk of criminal recidivism are performed on a routine basis, as a baseline for risk management for populations involved in the criminal justice system. However, the risk assessment tools available to clinical practice are limited in their ability to predict recidivism. Recently, the prospect of incorporating neuroimaging data to improve the prediction of criminal behavior has received increased attention. In this study we investigated the feasibility of including neuroimaging data in the prediction of recidivism by studying whether the inclusion of resting-state regional cerebral blood flow measurements leads to an incremental increase in predictive performance over traditional risk factors. A subsample (N = 44) from a cohort of forensic psychiatric patients who underwent single-photon emission computed tomography neuroimaging and clinical psychiatric assessment during their court-ordered forensic psychiatric investigation were included in a long-term (ten year average time at risk) follow-up. A Baseline model with eight empirically established risk factors, and an Extended model which also included resting-state regional cerebral blood flow measurements from eight brain regions were estimated using random forest classification and compared using several predictive performance metrics. Including neuroimaging data in the Extended model increased the area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (AUC) from .69 to .81, increased accuracy from .64 to .82 and increased the scaled Brier score from .08 to .25, supporting the feasibility of including neuroimaging data in the prediction of recidivism in forensic psychiatric patients. Although our results hint at potential benefits in the domain of risk assessment, several limitations and ethical challenges are discussed. Further studies with larger, carefully characterized clinical samples utilizing higher-resolution neuroimaging techniques are warranted.

Introduction

Crime in general and violent crime in particular is a significant public health concern. Decades of research have identified several risk factors for persistent criminality, and among the most widely reported are previous criminality, younger age, younger age at criminal onset, aggressive behavior, substance use and factors relating to dysfunction within family, school, and employment [19]. Furthermore, while persistent criminality is more common among males than females, risk factors appear to be similar between the sexes [10]. A comparable pattern emerges in individuals with mental disorders. Although previous research has found evidence of a substantially increased risk of violence in individuals with major mental disorders, even when adjusting for substance use and other known risk factors [1113], a recent meta-analysis concluded that major mental disorders by themselves appear to be unreliable predictors of both general and violent recidivism [14]. Instead, risk factors in mentally disordered offenders seem to mirror those found in the general population [15,16], with the most prominent risk factors being the presence of an antisocial personality disorder and/or a high degree of psychopathic traits [14,17,18].

One of the primary objectives in forensic psychiatry is risk management. To that end, risk assessments are performed to predict criminal recidivism and provide a baseline for risk management, but the currently most used risk assessment tools–which in essence are made up of various constellations of the traditional risk factors outlined above–are limited in their ability to do just that [16,19,20]. With the limits of risk assessments based solely on traditional risk factors in mind, coupled with advances in neuroimaging and an emerging hypothesis that life-course persistent antisocial behavior may be viewed as a neurodevelopmental disorder [21], the prospect of incorporating neuroimaging data to improve the prediction of criminal behavior has received increased attention [16,19,20,2224].

One of the most consistent neurobiological markers for antisocial behavior is reductions in frontal lobe structure and function, observed in several samples of criminal, violent and psychopathic individuals [2528]. In addition, both structural and functional temporal lobe reductions have been reported in several studies [2931], and reduced parietal glucose metabolism has been related to impulsive aggression, impulsive personality disorders, and violent offending [3235]. Frontal and temporal aberrations are also consistently found in aggressive patients suffering from schizophrenia [36], and reductions in both frontal, temporal, parietal and cerebellar gray matter volume have been observed after the onset of psychosis [37]. More recently, aberrations in smaller regions such as the hippocampus [38], nucleus accumbens [39] and amygdala [40] have been observed in offenders with psychopathy, altered connectivity between the amygdala and the cerebellum has been found in violent offenders [41], and it is possible that the cerebellum itself may be related to psychopathic traits, recidivism and violent criminality [30,42].

Certainly, our understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of criminal and violent behavior in mentally disordered individuals is far from conclusive, although the potential of utilizing neuroimaging data in the prediction of recidivism may hold some promise. To our knowledge only two studies, both using participants from the same sample of male prisoners, have been published where authors have incorporated neuroimaging data to predict recidivism [43,44]. In this retrospective and exploratory study we extend previous research into the domain of forensic psychiatry. We address the feasibility of including neuroimaging data in the prediction of recidivism by investigating if the prediction of recidivism using a Baseline model, with empirically well-established risk factors, could be improved by including resting-state regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) measurements in an Extended model, in a long-term follow-up of forensic psychiatric patients.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants (N = 44) were recruited from the Forensic psychiatric follow-up studies–the Malmö cohort (UPPRÄTT-Malmö study). The UPPRÄTT-Malmö study consists of 101 men and 24 women, aged 17–79 (median age = 38) at the time of inclusion. The UPPRÄTT-Malmö study is a nationally representative, total cohort of patients living in the Malmö University Hospital catchment area who, after committing a crime, underwent either a major forensic psychiatric investigation (FPI, N = 97) or a minor forensic psychiatric screening report (N = 28) between 1999 and 2005, and subsequently were sentenced to involuntary forensic psychiatric in-patient treatment. One previous study has investigated recidivism using traditional risk factors [45], and one previous study has investigated predictors of length of stay [46], both using the full UPPRÄTT-Malmö sample. When study inclusion commenced, FPI investigees were routinely being referred for a neuroimaging assessment, but due to changes in the local FPI procedures during study inclusion, only 50 participants underwent this assessment. Of those, six were omitted from the current study; two were missing data on educational attainment, three were missing data on age at first crime, and one was missing data on psychopathic traits due to lack of sufficient data for scoring. Thus, the final sample in this study was 44 participants, aged 20–79 (median age = 35).

Clinical assessment and characteristics

Psychiatric diagnoses were assessed at the end of FPI according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edition (DSM-IV) [47] using semi-structured interviews by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) [48] and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (SCID-II) [49]. The diagnoses reflect the participant’s psychiatric status at the time of the FPI, and by definition, all individuals presented symptoms consistent with one or more major mental disorders at the time of both the FPI and the committed crime(s). We clustered participants’ primary DSM-IV diagnoses into five categories: psychotic disorder, mood disorder, personality disorder, cognitive disorders, and neurodevelopmental disorders. Information about participant’s age at the time of FPI was obtained from the FPI protocols, while dates of admittance (i.e., start of forensic psychiatric in-patient treatment) and discharge were obtained from patient records. The number of days under forensic psychiatric care was defined as the number of days between patient’s intake date and either date of discharge, date of death, date of deportation, or the 31st of December, 2013 (i.e., end of follow-up) if patients were still under forensic psychiatric care when follow-up ended.

Neuroimaging data

The neuroimaging data used in this study was acquired using single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) and was collected as part of the FPI investigation. Thus, the choice of SPECT was clinically motivated rather than motivated by research. Measurements were carried out using 99mTc-exametazime (CeretecTM, Nycomed-Amersham/GE Healthcare) and a Ceraspect SPECT camera (Digital Scintigraphics Inc., Waltham, Massachusetts). Participants were administered 900 MBq of 99mTc-exametazime through a pre-set cannula in a cubital vein while resting comfortably supine, awake and silent in a muted room with eyes open focusing on a point in the ceiling.

99mTc-exametazime is lipophilic and passes through the blood-brain barrier and the cell membrane to reach intra-cellular space in proportion to rCBF. Intracellular 99mTc-exametazime rapidly transforms into a polar form that cannot leave the cell, and thus the 99mTc-exametazime distribution in the brain remains unchanged for several hours, providing a snapshot of rCBF a brief period after the injection. The imaging procedure began about 15 minutes after administration and participants were recorded for 30 minutes.

The radiation from the 99mTc-exametazime was recorded in 1800 to allow a 3-dimensional reconstruction of the activity, proportional to the rCBF, after scatter and attenuation corrections, with a resolution of 9 mm FWHM (full-width at half-maximum). The recorded three-dimensional activity was saved into a 128*128*64 voxel matrix and subdivided into 10 slices with 1 cm thickness, parallel to the orbitomeatal line. A region-of-interest (ROI) set [50] was scaled to fit the outer dimensions of the brain for three dimensional measurement of activity, proportional to rCBF. The measured value in each ROI was quantified, using Amersham ROI software (GE Healthcare, Buckinghamshire, UK), in percent of the mean 99mTc-exametazime concentration in the whole brain.

Pharmacological treatment at the time of SPECT acquisition

A majority of Swedish forensic psychiatric patients receive pharmacological treatment, often with multiple agents, although antipsychotics are the most prevalent [51]. Antipsychotics are known to affect rCBF primarily in frontal, temporal and striatal regions [52], while anticholinergics, often administered to reduce extrapyramidal symptoms of antipsychotics, appear to reduce rCBF in the whole brain [53]. We collected data on pharmacological treatment from medical records from the time of SPECT acquisition and structured the data according to five major pharmacological categories: antipsychotics, antidepressants, benzodiazepine sedatives/hypnotics, non-benzodiazepine sedatives/hypnotics, and anticholinergics, each coded as either ‘yes’ or ‘no’.

Follow-up data on criminality

To account for the fact that patients in some cases relapse in crime during their forensic psychiatric care [54], the time at risk was defined as beginning at each patient’s intake date and lasting until reconviction, death, deportation or until the end of follow-up at the 31st of December 2013. Recidivism was defined as a criminal conviction during the time at risk and is presented as general recidivism (i.e. all convictions, including violent) due to low base rates of specific crimes. The mean time at risk for the entire sample was 3623 days (SD = 1495), ranging from 166 to 5342 days. Nine patients in the sample died during the follow-up, and had an average time at risk of 1477 days (SD = 1165), ranging from 166 to 3410 days. In addition, one patient was deported during the follow-up, and had a time at risk of 219 days. Dates of new crimes and convictions, dates of legal force of new sentences and following periods of sanctions, as well as dates of deportations were provided by the National Council of Crime Prevention. Dates of deaths were obtained from the Cause of Death Register at the National Board of Health and Welfare.

Baseline model measures

Nine traditional risk factors were selected based on previous literature [16,14,45] to be included in the Baseline model: age at FPI, age at first crime, degree of psychopathic traits, sex, substance use disorder, cluster B personality disorder, educational attainment, mental disorder in first-degree relative, and previous criminality. Note that we opted to include both age at FPI and age at first crime as baseline risk factors not only because both are associated with recidivism, but also because prior research suggests that both global and regional CBF tends to decrease with age [5561].

Information about participant’s sex, previous criminality (defined as any previous conviction prior to the crime that lead to FPI and dichotomized as ‘yes’ or ‘no’), age at first crime, educational attainment (dichotomized as ‘yes’ or ‘no’, with ‘yes’ indicating primary school or higher attainment), and mental disorder in first-degree relative (dichotomized as ‘yes’ or ‘no’) was obtained from the FPIs using structured protocols. In most instances, psychopathic traits were scored during the FPI based on information from the clinical assessment as well as extensive file and register reviews, using the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL-SV) [62], consisting of 12 items scored on a 3-point scale (0, 1, 2). When an item of the PCL:SV was omitted, a score was assigned according to the PCL:SV manual. In cases where PCL:SV ratings were missing (N = 5 of the current study sample), these were performed retrospectively, based on file reviews [63].

Extended model measures

The Extended model consisted of the variables used in the Baseline model plus neuroimaging data in the form of resting-state rCBF measurements. Given the exploratory nature of this study, and with a parsimonious approach to the number of predictors included, eight ROIs were selected (the left and right frontal lobe, the left and right parietal lobe, the left and right temporal lobe, and the left and right cerebellum; see Fig 1) ensuring coverage of the brain’s major regions (the occipital lobe was excluded since visual stimulation was part of the procedure to ensure participant’s wakefulness, and smaller volume regions such as the thalamus and basal ganglia were excluded due to the procedure’s low spatial resolution).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Regions-of-interest.

An overview of the regions-of-interest (ROIs) used in the current study. Numbers refer to centimeters above/below the orbitomeatal line. C = cerebellum, T = temporal lobe, F = frontal lobe, P = parietal lobe.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217127.g001

Data analyses

Data preparation and statistical analysis was conducted using the R statistical programming language [64]. All R code is publicly available at the corresponding authors’ GitHub page (https://github.com/carldelfin/neuroprediction), adhering to the principles of reproducible research [65]. Statistical significance was pre-defined as p < .05.

Group comparisons and correlations.

Differences between recidivists and non-recidivists were examined using Barnard’s test [66] for dichotomous variables and Welch’s t-test for numerical variables. Barnard’s test is a more powerful alternative to Fisher’s exact test when sample sizes are small [67]. Welch’s t-test performs better than Student’s t-test in situations where sample size and variance is unequal between groups and equal to Student’s t-test in situations where sample size and variance is equal [68]. Correlations were examined using Spearman’s ρ.

Random forest classification.

Random forest classification (RF) [69], a so-called ensemble method which aggregates the results from a large collection of decision trees [70], was used to predict recidivism. Recognized for its high accuracy, the RF machine learning algorithm has no distributional assumptions, performs well with both small sample sizes and high-dimensional data [71], and has previously been successfully used to predict general recidivism in mentally disordered offenders [72]. The RF algorithm works by building each decision tree using a random bootstrap sample (with replacement) corresponding to roughly 66% of the data. The remaining so-called out-of-bag (OOB) data is used for estimating model error and assessing variable importance, allowing the RF algorithm to reduce overfitting; an otherwise common occurrence in predictive modeling [73]. In addition, only a random subset of predictors is selected at each node in the decision tree. Each predictor is split to optimize tree performance, and the predictor split that produces the highest tree performance is selected for that node. After constructing a tree, each observation in the OOB data is passed down the tree and is classified, in the case of the present study, as either “yes” or “no” for recidivism. The final (i.e., aggregated) classification of each observation is the majority “vote” based on all the trees where that observation was in the out-of-bag sample.

Each model was created using 10 000 trees, using the default of √p predictors at each node, with p being the total number of predictors available. Since the RF algorithm can be sensitive to class imbalance, the majority class (i.e., non-recidivists) was down-sampled to ensure that each bootstrap sample contained the same number of non-recidivists as recidivists [74].

Assessing model performance

Predictive performance was assessed using several metrics. We report the area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (AUC), representing the probability (from 0 to 1) that a randomly selected recidivist will have been predicted by the model as having a higher probability of recidivism than a randomly selected non-recidivist. The AUC has been put forward as the recommended measure of predictive performance in forensic psychiatry [75], although opinions differ about its predictive interpretation. For instance, an AUC of .71 corresponds to Cohen’s d = .80, which is a large effect size [75]. Others have suggested that AUCs in risk assessments should be more conservatively interpreted, with AUCs between .60 and .70 having modest accuracy and AUCs between .80 and .90 having moderate accuracy [76]. We also report accuracy (the overall proportion of correct classifications), sensitivity (the proportion of recidivists correctly classified as such), specificity (the proportion of non-recidivists correctly classified as such), positive predictive value (PPV; the proportion of predicted recidivists that actually are recidivists), and negative predictive value (NPV; the proportion of predicted non-recidivists that actually are non-recidivists). In addition, we report scaled Brier scores, as recommended by recent research [77], calculated using the DescTools R package. The (unscaled) Brier score is defined as the squared difference between the actual binary outcome Y (0 or 1) and the predicted probability p (ranging from 0 to 1). By scaling the Brier score so that it no longer depends on the prevalence of Y, the resulting scaled Brier score ranges between 0 and 1. Similar to Pearson’s R2, a higher scaled Brier scores indicates better calibration of the predictive model [78]. The advantage of (whether scaled or unscaled) Brier scores is that the best score will be attained by the model that is able to predict as close to the true probabilities as possible.

Assessing variable importance.

Individual variable importance is estimated by the RF algorithm during the OOB phase by randomly permuting each variable and recording how it affects classification accuracy. If a variable is important for classification then permutation should result in a large decrease in classification accuracy, whereas for unimportant variables, permutation should have little to no effect on classification accuracy. We report the scaled (mean divided by SD) mean decrease in accuracy for each variable, which is an estimate of the decrease in model accuracy, should that variable be omitted. In addition, partial dependence plots [79] visualize both the direction and size of effect (a wider range in the y-axis implies a larger effect) of each variable, after averaging out the effect of all other variables.

Ethics

The study was register-based and retrospective; all clinical data (including SPECT measurements) was routinely collected as part of the FPI during the time study inclusion commenced. Thus, informed consent was not considered necessary, as it would not be possible to contact most participants due to the length of time that had passed after finishing treatment and because contact could pose a risk to vulnerable subjects with mental health and/or legal problems. All procedures used in this study were approved by the regional ethics review board in Lund (2007/64 and 2014/911).

Results

Recidivism

Sixteen patients (36% of the sample) were convicted of a crime during their time at risk. Most crimes were non-violent, such as theft, fraud, falsification of documents, driving under the influence of alcohol, and drug offences, although seven patients (16% of the sample) were convicted of violent crimes, including assault and battery, unlawful threat, and robbery. There was no difference in time at risk between recidivists and non-recidivists (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Detailed overview of sample clinical characteristics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217127.t001

Clinical characteristics

Slightly less than one third of the sample were still under forensic psychiatric care at the end of the follow-up, with an average length of stay of almost 4.8 years, and no significant difference between recidivists and non-recidivists. The primary diagnosis of the majority of patients was a psychotic disorder, and no significant difference was found between recidivists and non-recidivists (Table 1). Comorbidity was relatively common, with 59% of patients diagnosed with two or more DSM-IV Axis I disorders (median = 2, range = 1 to 8). The median number of DSM-IV Axis II disorders was 0, ranging from 0 to 3. Most patients received antipsychotics at the time of SPECT acquisition, with no significant differences regarding pharmacological treatment between recidivist and non-recidivists. Anticholinergic treatment did appear less common among recidivists, although the difference did not reach statistical significance at the pre-defined level (Table 1).

rCBF measurements

Recidivists had significantly lower bilateral parietal lobe and right cerebellar rCBF compared to non-recidivists. They also exhibited slightly higher temporal lobe rCBF than the non-recidivist group, but the difference was not statistically significant at the pre-defined level (Table 1).

Baseline risk factors

Recidivists were significantly younger at the time of FPI, and also had a significantly younger age at their first crime. There was also a significantly higher frequency of cluster B personality disorders among the recidivists. There were no significant differences regarding PCL:SV total score, sex, substance use disorder, educational attainment, mental disorder in first-degree relative, or previous criminality (Table 1).

Associations between age at FPI and rCBF

Age at FPI was significantly and positively associated with right parietal lobe rCBF (ρ = .31, p = .039) and significantly and negatively associated with left frontal lobe rCBF (ρ = -.35, p = .02). Associations between age at FPI and right cerebellar (ρ = .28, p = .065), left cerebellar (ρ = .27, p = .074), right frontal lobe (ρ = -.09, p = .555), right temporal lobe (ρ = .12, p = .456), left temporal lobe (ρ = -.23, p = .134), and left parietal lobe rCBF (ρ = .18, p = .237) did not reach the predetermined level of significance.

Baseline model

Predictive performance of the Baseline model was modest, all measures considered. The AUC was .69, with a scaled Brier score of .08 and an accuracy of .64 (95% CI [.48, .78]), and sensitivity = .63, specificity = .64, PPV = .50, and NPV = .75. The most important variables in terms of mean decrease in accuracy were cluster B personality disorders, age at first crime, age, and substance use disorders (Fig 2, left panel). Partial dependence plots revealed that a cluster B personality disorder, lower age at first crime, younger age at the time of FPI, and substance use disorders increased the probability of being classified as a recidivist (Fig 3, top panel).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Variable importance.

Variable importance measured as the scaled mean decrease in accuracy of each variable in the Baseline and Extended model. A higher value confers a higher decrease in the accuracy of the model, should that variable be omitted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217127.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Partial dependence plots.

Partial dependence plots for the eight most important (in terms of scaled mean decrease in accuracy) variables in each model. A higher value on the y-axis confers a higher probability of being predicted as a recidivist for the corresponding value on the x-axis for that variable.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217127.g003

Extended model

Predictive performance increased across all metrics in the Extended model. The AUC was .81 with a scaled Brier score of .25 and an accuracy of .82 (95% CI [.67, .92]), and sensitivity = .75, specificity = .86, PPV = .73, and NPV = .86. The most important variables were right parietal rCBF, cluster B personality disorder, age at first crime, and left parietal rCBF (Fig 2, right panel). In terms of actual predictions, the Extended model correctly classified two additional recidivists and six additional non-recidivists compared to the Baseline model, resulting in 12 out of 16 recidivists and 24 out of 28 non-recidivists being correctly classified by the Extended model. The probability of being classified as a recidivist increased with lower right parietal lobe rCBF, a cluster B personality disorder, lower age at first crime, and lower left parietal lobe rCBF (Fig 3, bottom panel). A similar effect was visible for lower right cerebellar rCBF and lower age, although less pronounced. Conversely, higher (bilateral) temporal lobe rCBF modestly increased the probability of being classified as a recidivist (Fig 3, bottom panel).

Supplementary analysis of pharmacological data

Despite no statistically significant differences in pharmacological treatment between recidivists and non-recidivists, we conducted a supplementary analysis before ruling out any potential effects of pharmacological treatment at the time of SPECT acquisition. Adding pharmacological data to the Extended model did not result in a notable increase in predictive performance. The AUC increased slightly from .81 to .82, while all other measures remained identical.

Discussion

Main findings

We have demonstrated the feasibility of incorporating neuroimaging data, in the form of resting-state rCBF measurements, in the prediction of recidivism in a long-term follow-up of forensic psychiatric patients. The Extended model, which included traditional risk factors as well as resting-state rCBF measurements, saw a 17% increase in AUC, over 200% increase in scaled Brier score, and a 28% increase in accuracy from the Baseline model, which included traditional risk factors only. Recidivists did not significantly differ from non-recidivists in number of patients still under forensic psychiatric care at the end of follow-up, average length of stay, primary diagnosis or time at risk, suggesting that the increased performance of the Extended model was not attributable to any of these variables. Furthermore, supplementary analysis showed that the increased performance in the Extended model was most likely not a result of differences in pharmacological treatment between recidivists and non-recidivists. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to include neuroimaging data in the prediction of recidivism in a forensic psychiatric sample, and our results call for continued studies that use neuroimaging methods that potentially could be beneficial also in current clinical forensic practice.

Baseline model: Traditional risk factors

A cluster B personality disorder, age at first crime and age at FPI emerged as important in both models, similar to previously published findings from the full UPPRÄTT-Malmö cohort [45]. Partial dependence plots revealed that younger age at first crime as well as younger age at FPI increased the probability of being classified as a recidivist in both the Baseline and the Extended model. Furthermore, the effect of age at first crime was larger than the effect of age at FPI. These results are in line with the long-standing observation that early-onset aggressive and antisocial behavior increases the risk of life-course persistent criminality [1,10,80]. Likewise, a cluster B personality disorder increased the probability of being classified as a recidivist, in agreement with previous literature showing an increased risk of criminal and violent behavior in individuals who have received diagnoses within this cluster of personality disorders [81,82]. We, as many others before us [83], therefore recommend sustained efforts to identify and support individuals exhibiting antisocial tendencies at a young age.

Substance use disorder was a moderately important predictor of increased probability of recidivism in the Baseline model, although the effect was diminished in the Extended model. Recent research has demonstrated several unfavourable long-term outcomes of substance use in adolescents and young adults with mental disorders [84], and the combination of substance use and mental disorders seems to confer a higher risk of recidivism than substance use or mental disorder alone [85]. Unfortunately, treatment of substance use disorders in mentally disordered offenders appears rare [86,87], even though it may have a positive impact on reducing recidivism.

Several of the traditional risk factors showed no appreciable effect on the prediction of recidivism. Psychopathy, for instance, was not an important predictor in terms of mean decrease in accuracy, although the partial dependence plot revealed that higher PCL:SV scores tended to confer a higher probability of recidivism in the Baseline model. In our study, recidivists scored an average 12 points on the PCL:SV, which is relatively low when contrasted with suggested cutoffs at ≥ 18 for psychopathy and 13–17 for possible psychopathy [88]. It is possible that the low scores and low variability between recidivists and non-recidivists were not sufficient for psychopathic traits to be considered an important variable. It is also possible that using the separate Part 1 (i.e., interpersonal and affective features) and Part 2 (i.e., unstable and antisocial lifestyle) of the PCL:SV would have led to a greater predictive effect of psychopathic traits. Still, research has suggested that both the PCL:SV total score, Part 1 score, and Part 2 score are similar in their ability to predict recidivism [89] as well as violence and aggression [90,91]. In addition, five PCL:SV ratings were performed retrospectively based on file reviews only, which also may have influenced our results, although research has shown that retrospective, file-only PCL:SV ratings can be used reliably for research purposes in Swedish forensic psychiatric populations [63]. Previous criminality was not associated with an increased probability of recidivism, which was a surprising find. However, since previous criminality was operationalized as any convictions prior to the crime that lead to FPI and subsequent forensic psychiatric care, all participants, by definition, had already committed a crime when the follow-up started. In other words, a “criminal history” already existed for each patient in our study, which may have diluted the effects of the previous criminality variable (i.e., a possible ceiling effect). We found no effect of mental disorder in a first-degree relative, even though early psychosocial adversities such as parental abuse, parental absence and parental mental disorder have been associated with several negative outcomes, including criminality [7,92]. However, there is evidence of a dose-response relationship between childhood adversities and negative adult outcomes [9395], and it may be that mental disorder in a first-degree relative alone was not sufficient to yield a predictive effect. Furthermore, at least one study found that while criminal conviction was linked to parental mental disorder, multiple (three or more) convictions was not related to parental mental disorder alone, but to rather to parental criminality, or a combination of both [96], again hinting at a possible ceiling effect. Since only five females were included in the study, the lack of effect of sex is difficult to evaluate. It is worth noting, still, that of the two females that did recidivate, neither committed violent crimes. Finally, no effect of education was found. Meta-analytic results have shown that when separated from employment, education is no longer predictive of general recidivism in mentally disordered offenders, in line with our results [14].

Extended model: Neuroimaging risk factors

Right parietal lobe rCBF emerged as the most important variable, while left parietal lobe rCBF was the fourth most important variable in predicting recidivism in the Extended model. Recidivists had lower rCBF in both the right and left parietal lobe compared to non-recidivists, and partial dependence plots further revealed that lower bilateral parietal rCBF was associated with an increased probability of being classified as a recidivist. Lower right cerebellar rCBF was a modest predictor of recidivism, as was increased bilateral temporal lobe rCBF.

In agreement with prior studies reporting either reduced parietal lobe glucose metabolism or reduced parietal lobe rCBF in violent, impulsive and aggressive samples [3235], our results suggest that reduced bilateral parietal rCBF may be an important predictor of general recidivism in forensic psychiatric patients. We theorize that parietal lobe contributions to inhibitory control may function as a pathway to criminal behavior. Specifically, although both frontal and parietal regions are involved in response inhibition [97100], research has suggested that age-related changes in neural circuitry from childhood to early adulthood results in increased recruitment of parietal and occipital regions in response inhibition, while prefrontal recruitment decreases [101]. Poor response inhibition is a consistent marker of externalizing psychopathology, including alcohol and substance abuse [102104], ADHD [105], aggression [106], as well as violent [107] and non-violent [108] criminality, and appears to be primarily genetic in origin [109]. Thus, an interesting albeit speculative interpretation of our results, given previous evidence of parietal involvement in response inhibition, is that reduced parietal rCBF may lead to poorer inhibitory control, subsequently increasing the risk of recidivism. Speaking against our proposal of parietal lobe contributions to reduced inhibitory control as a pathway to criminal behavior in the current study, however, is the fact that no apparent predictive effect of the frontal lobes was found, despite that frontal regions are robustly activated during response inhibition [100]. In addition, since no test-based measure of behavioral inhibitory control was available, our proposal remains speculative. Future research should further explore possible relationships between parietal lobe function, inhibitory control and criminal behavior.

The role of the cerebellum in criminal and antisocial behavior is relatively unexplored. The cerebellum is known to be recruited during a wide range of cognitive tasks [110], and previous research has demonstrated that cerebellar lesions can lead to a range of psychopathologies, including disinhibited behaviors such as impulsivity and poor attention [111]. Thus, it is possible that reduced cerebellar blood flow leads to an increased risk of disinhibited behavior, similar to what is observed in cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome [112,113]. Recent studies have found that cerebellar gray and white matter volume may be related to criminality, anger and psychopathic traits [30,42], and while our study adds to previous research suggesting cerebellar involvement in criminal behavior, reconciling our results with studies of cerebellar gray and white matter volume it not straightforward, and more research is clearly needed. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, for instance, it should be possible to study the association between rCBF, GMV and criminal, antisocial or disinhibited behavior.

Our results seem to be at odds with previous reports of frontal and temporal lobe aberrations related to antisocial behavior. Several meta-analytic or review studies have concluded that reduced frontal lobe structure and function appears to be a consistent neurobiological marker of antisocial behavior [2528]. Still, authors have noted that observed effects are modest [28], not sufficient to cause physical aggression or violence on their own [26], and localized to smaller subregions [25]. Since rCBF was averaged across the left and right frontal lobe in our study, possible effects of rCBF in smaller subregions may have been diffused, a problem that may be resolved using neuroimaging techniques with higher spatial resolution. It is also possible that the entire sample exhibited reduced frontal rCBF, although with little variability between recidivists and non-recidivists. Unfortunately, the lack of a suitable comparison group makes it impossible to investigate this theory. Finally, the negative association between age at FPI and left frontal rCBF is in line with previous research, although it remains unclear why a significant association was not found for the right frontal lobe, as previous findings suggest bilateral reductions [59,61].

Several studies have reported decreased temporal lobe function in aggressive and antisocial samples [35,114,115], although at least one study found no reductions in a sample of violent offenders [32]. In the present study, increased temporal lobe rCBF provided a modest increase in the probability of being classified as a recidivist. Recent studies have revealed a positive association between threat-related amygdala response and impulsive aggression [116118], and there is prior evidence of perceived threat mediating the relationship between psychosis proneness and aggressive behavior [119] suggesting that increased activity in at least one small temporal lobe subregion may be related to some forms of antisocial behavior. Since our study used a resting-state paradigm, however, no threat-related responses were expected. In addition, the observed effect was modest and thus should be interpreted with caution.

A final consideration is that a majority of the patients (68%) in the current study had a primary diagnosis of psychotic disorder. Psychotic disorders are characterized both by functional aberrations [120] and progressive gray matter reductions [121123], with some reductions even attributable to pharmacological treatment [124]. The complex nature of brain changes in the various stages of psychotic disorders makes it difficult to compare our results with, for instance, studies of neurobiological contributions to recidivism in non-psychotic populations.

Strengths and limitations

This study has some notable strengths and several limitations. The average time at risk was ten years, which is rare in forensic psychiatric samples and even rarer when combined with neuroimaging. We included data on pharmacological treatment, which is often overlooked in psychiatric samples, and provided a detailed clinical description of the sample. We employed modern statistical techniques, and all analysis code is publicly available.

As for limitations, a general problem when interpreting results from forensic investigations is that very heterogeneous samples have been studied. For instance, the participants in the current study varied in age, sex, diagnoses, and type of crimes committed. Also, even though we had access to the psychiatric diagnoses that were assessed concurrently with the SPECT investigation, we did not have any data on specific psychiatric symptoms, such as level of psychotic symptoms, at the time of SPECT. In addition, the small sample size means that our results must be carefully interpreted, and readers should refrain from drawing firm conclusions regarding the neurobiological contributions to recidivism before our results are replicated in independent, larger samples. While we included data on pharmacological treatment, we have no information about actual adherence, although adherence is generally believed to be high at forensic psychiatric units due to the possibilities of control of medication intake. Another limitation is seen in our subjective choice of baseline risk factors, which although guided by previous research may have affected predictive accuracy and thus the incremental effect of neuroimaging data. For instance, we did not assess the effect of IQ, which has been associated with CBF in both children and adolescents [125] and in adults [126]. The clinical status of many of the included patients made it difficult to perform reliable and valid IQ assessments at the time of FPI, and these assessments were thus omitted from the protocol. Furthermore, predicting measures other than recidivism could have rendered different results. Future research may compare models predicting recidivism with models predicting other outcome measures, such as the number of adverse incidents during in-patient care and including measures of self-reported criminality. Future studies may also benefit from the complementary information gained from survival models that estimate how the probability of recidivism increases over. We opted not to include such models in the current study due to the limited sample size and relatively large number of predictors used, which limits the interpretability of our results. Finally, the low spatial resolution of the SPECT methodology used prevented detailed study of smaller neural subregions of interest, such as the anterior cingulate cortex [43,44] or the angular gyrus [32,35]. Since it is possible that only smaller subregions of the ROIs included in the current study may be linked to recidivism, using large ROIs may lead to oversimplification and reduced power [127].

Implications for clinical practice and directions for future research

We have demonstrated that improvements in recidivism prediction in forensic psychiatry may be achievable if neuroimaging data is incorporated into risk assessment models. We reiterate, however, that the study is exploratory in nature, that the results should be carefully interpreted, and that further studies are needed before generalizations can be made. Importantly, the number needed to detain (NND) [128], based on the observed recidivism rate of 36% during our follow-up was 2 in the Baseline model and 1.4 in the Extended model, which is of doubtful clinical relevance. However, the NND depends on the rate of recidivism. Thus, if we assume a recidivism rate of 10% (a plausible rate for a shorter time span, such as one year) the Extended model would reduce the NND by 50%, from 6 in the Baseline model to 3. In addition, the large increase in scaled Brier score in the Extended model suggests an improvement in predictive performance that is not obvious by looking solely at measures based on confusion matrices. Since discriminatory performance metrics such as AUC, accuracy, and NND require predicted probabilities to be binary, valuable information is lost. For instance, if a correct prediction is 0, using a threshold of 0.5, a prediction of 0.1 and a prediction of 0.49 are weighted equally; both will be regarded as 0, even though the former is obviously closer to the truth. Brier scores, on the other hand, utilize all information available in the predicted probabilities. The large increase in scaled Brier score in the Extended model indicates that while the difference in NND may not be clinically relevant, the improved predictive performance of the Extended model may still be useful. For instance, a possible clinical application of improved prediction models would be as decision support systems, aiding physicians in directing resources and interventions to patients at the highest risk of recidivism [129]. A patient with a predicted probability of 0.49 thus should have higher priority in risk management than a patient with a predicted probability of 0.1, even though both fall under the (arbitrary) threshold of 0.5.

Finally, since SPECT measurements are unlikely to be available in most risk assessment situations, this further limits clinical application of our results. However, the primary purpose of this study was to examine if incorporating neuroimaging data leads to incremental increase in predictive performance, and we have shown that using measures of rCBF, an incremental increase in predictive performance is possible. We urge researchers to further assess incremental increases in predictive performance using other neuroimaging techniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging. In addition, electroencephalography, while not strictly a neuroimaging method, may be more feasible in clinical settings.

In conclusion, forensic psychiatry is uniquely positioned at the intersection between neuroscience and law. Recently, the emerging field of neurolaw has brought to light the many ethical challenges and questions that unfold in this intersection [19]. Notable examples include reductionism and stigmatization of individuals based on brain function, which are–and have been historically–important ethical challenges that must be addressed with care [24]. Neuroscientific data must be applied responsibly in any context, and if employed in real life legal scenarios, multiple measurement techniques and multiple cognitive tasks should be used [23,24]. Thus, future research should assess the validity of our results in larger, carefully characterized samples, utilizing different and preferably multiple neuroimaging techniques along with assessments of psychiatric symptoms and pharmacological treatment at the time of imaging, and finally test prediction models in held-out samples. Furthermore, our findings and subsequent discussion regarding rCBF measurements and the prediction of recidivism should be interpreted as preliminary, indicating possible avenues for further research rather than definite neural correlates of criminality. The nature of antisocial and criminal behavior is complex and dynamic, and measurements of blood flow in a single particular region of the brain likely only accounts for a small amount of variance in criminal behavior.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Henrik Anckarsäter and Thomas Nilsson for their excellent scientific advice. Finally, we would like to express our gratitude to all included patients for their participation.

References

  1. 1. Farrington DP, Ttofi MM, Coid JW. Development of adolescence-limited, late-onset, and persistent offenders from age 8 to age 48. Aggress Behav. 2009;35: 150–163. pmid:19172660
  2. 2. Assink M, van der Put CE, Hoeve M, de Vries SLA, Stams GJJM, Oort FJ. Risk factors for persistent delinquent behavior among juveniles: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;42: 47–61. pmid:26301752
  3. 3. Murray J, Farrington DP. Risk factors for conduct disorder and delinquency: key findings from longitudinal studies. Can J Psychiatry. 2010;55: 633–642. pmid:20964942
  4. 4. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Harrington H, Milne BJ. Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: follow-up at age 26 years. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14: 179–207. pmid:11893092
  5. 5. Hodgins S. Mental disorder, intellectual deficiency, and crime. Evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49: 476–483. pmid:1599373
  6. 6. Hodgins S. Schizophrenia and violence: Are new mental health policies needed? J Forens Psychiatry Psychol. Routledge; 1994;5: 473–477.
  7. 7. af Klinteberg B, Almquist Y, Beijer U, Rydelius P-A. Family psychosocial characteristics influencing criminal behaviour and mortality–possible mediating factors: a longitudinal study of male and female subjects in the Stockholm Birth Cohort. BMC Public Health. 2011;11: 756. pmid:21962152
  8. 8. Hirschi T, Gottfredson M. Age and the Explanation of Crime. Am J Sociol. University of Chicago Press; 1983;89: 552–584.
  9. 9. Sweeten G, Piquero AR, Steinberg L. Age and the explanation of crime, revisited. J Youth Adolesc. 2013;42: 921–938. pmid:23412690
  10. 10. Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Dev Psychopathol. 2001;13: 355–375. pmid:11393651
  11. 11. Arseneault L, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor PJ, Silva PA. Mental disorders and violence in a total birth cohort: results from the Dunedin Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57: 979–986. pmid:11015816
  12. 12. Bonta J, Law M, Hanson K. The prediction of criminal and violent recidivism among mentally disordered offenders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 1998;123: 123–142. pmid:9522681
  13. 13. Chang Z, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, Fazel S. Psychiatric disorders and violent reoffending: a national cohort study of convicted prisoners in Sweden. Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2: 891–900. pmid:26342957
  14. 14. Bonta J, Blais J, Wilson HA. A theoretically informed meta-analysis of the risk for general and violent recidivism for mentally disordered offenders. Aggress Violent Behav. 2014;19: 278–287.
  15. 15. Wolf A, Fanshawe TR, Sariaslan A, Cornish R, Larsson H, Fazel S. Prediction of violent crime on discharge from secure psychiatric hospitals: A clinical prediction rule (FoVOx). Eur Psychiatry. 2017;47: 88–93. pmid:29161680
  16. 16. Sedgwick O, Young S, Das M, Kumari V. Objective predictors of outcome in forensic mental health services-a systematic review. CNS Spectr. 2016;21: 430–444. pmid:26797162
  17. 17. Phillips HK, Gray NS, MacCulloch SI, Taylor J, Moore SC, Huckle P, et al. Risk Assessment in Offenders With Mental Disorders: Relative Efficacy of Personal Demographic, Criminal History, and Clinical Variables. J Interpers Violence. SAGE Publications Inc; 2005;20: 833–847. pmid:15914704
  18. 18. Lund C, Hofvander B, Forsman A, Anckarsäter H, Nilsson T. Violent criminal recidivism in mentally disordered offenders: a follow-up study of 13–20 years through different sanctions. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2013;36: 250–257. pmid:23672945
  19. 19. Meynen G. Neurolaw: recognizing opportunities and challenges for psychiatry. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2016;41: 3–5. pmid:26674511
  20. 20. Nadelhoffer T, Bibas S, Grafton S, Kiehl KA, Mansfield A, Sinnott-Armstrong W, et al. Neuroprediction, Violence, and the Law: Setting the Stage. Neuroethics. 2012;5: 67–99. pmid:25083168
  21. 21. Fairchild G, van Goozen SHM, Calder AJ, Goodyer IM. Research review: evaluating and reformulating the developmental taxonomic theory of antisocial behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54: 924–940. pmid:23826820
  22. 22. Glenn AL, Raine A. Neurocriminology: implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15: 54–63. pmid:24326688
  23. 23. Poldrack RA, Monahan J, Imrey PB, Reyna V, Raichle ME, Faigman D, et al. Predicting Violent Behavior: What Can Neuroscience Add? Trends Cogn Sci. 2017; pmid:29183655
  24. 24. Gkotsi GM, Gasser J. Neuroscience in forensic psychiatry: From responsibility to dangerousness. Ethical and legal implications of using neuroscience for dangerousness assessments. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2016;46: 58–67. pmid:27209602
  25. 25. Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2009;174: 81–88. pmid:19833485
  26. 26. Séguin JR. The frontal lobe and aggression. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2009;6: 100–119. pmid:24976846
  27. 27. Brower MC, Price BH. Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: a critical review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;71: 720–726. pmid:11723190
  28. 28. Wahlund K, Kristiansson M. Aggression, psychopathy and brain imaging—Review and future recommendations. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2009;32: 266–271. pmid:19409616
  29. 29. Raine A, Yang Y. Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1: 203–213. pmid:18985107
  30. 30. Tiihonen J, Rossi R, Laakso MP, Hodgins S, Testa C, Perez J, et al. Brain anatomy of persistent violent offenders: more rather than less. Psychiatry Res. 2008;163: 201–212. pmid:18662866
  31. 31. Gregory S, Ffytche D, Simmons A, Kumari V, Howard M, Hodgins S, et al. The antisocial brain: psychopathy matters. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69: 962–972. pmid:22566562
  32. 32. Raine A, Buchsbaum M, LaCasse L. Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42: 495–508. pmid:9285085
  33. 33. Siever LJ, Buchsbaum MS, New AS, Spiegel-Cohen J, Wei T, Hazlett EA, et al. d,l-fenfluramine response in impulsive personality disorder assessed with [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20: 413–423. pmid:10192822
  34. 34. Hirono N, Mega MS, Dinov ID, Mishkin F, Cummings JL. Left frontotemporal hypoperfusion is associated with aggression in patients with dementia. Arch Neurol. 2000;57: 861–866. pmid:10867784
  35. 35. Söderström H, Tullberg M, Wikkelsö C, Ekholm S, Forsman A. Reduced regional cerebral blood flow in non-psychotic violent offenders. Psychiatry Res. 2000;98: 29–41. pmid:10708924
  36. 36. Soyka M. Neurobiology of aggression and violence in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37: 913–920. pmid:21860037
  37. 37. Borgwardt SJ, McGuire PK, Aston J, Gschwandtner U, Pflüger MO, Stieglitz R-D, et al. Reductions in frontal, temporal and parietal volume associated with the onset of psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2008;106: 108–114. pmid:18789654
  38. 38. Boccardi M, Ganzola R, Rossi R, Sabattoli F, Laakso MP, Repo-Tiihonen E, et al. Abnormal hippocampal shape in offenders with psychopathy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31: 438–447. pmid:19718651
  39. 39. Boccardi M, Bocchetta M, Aronen HJ, Repo-Tiihonen E, Vaurio O, Thompson PM, et al. Atypical nucleus accumbens morphology in psychopathy: another limbic piece in the puzzle. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2013;36: 157–167. pmid:23399314
  40. 40. Boccardi M, Frisoni GB, Hare RD, Cavedo E, Najt P, Pievani M, et al. Cortex and amygdala morphology in psychopathy. Psychiatry Res. 2011;193: 85–92. pmid:21676597
  41. 41. Leutgeb V, Wabnegger A, Leitner M, Zussner T, Scharmüller W, Klug D, et al. Altered cerebellar-amygdala connectivity in violent offenders: A resting-state fMRI study. Neurosci Lett. 2016;610: 160–164. pmid:26523791
  42. 42. Leutgeb V, Leitner M, Wabnegger A, Klug D, Scharmüller W, Zussner T, et al. Brain abnormalities in high-risk violent offenders and their association with psychopathic traits and criminal recidivism. Neuroscience. 2015;308: 194–201. pmid:26362887
  43. 43. Aharoni E, Vincent GM, Harenski CL, Calhoun VD, Sinnott-Armstrong W, Gazzaniga MS, et al. Neuroprediction of future rearrest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110: 6223–6228. pmid:23536303
  44. 44. Steele VR, Claus ED, Aharoni E, Vincent GM, Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA. Multimodal imaging measures predict rearrest. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9: 425. pmid:26283947
  45. 45. Krona H, Nyman M, Andreasson H, Vicencio N, Anckarsäter H, Wallinius M, et al. Mentally disordered offenders in Sweden: differentiating recidivists from non-recidivists in a 10-year follow-up study. Nord J Psychiatry. 2017;71: 102–109. pmid:27701993
  46. 46. Andreasson H, Nyman M, Krona H, Meyer L, Anckarsäter H, Nilsson T, et al. Predictors of length of stay in forensic psychiatry: the influence of perceived risk of violence. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2014;37: 635–642. pmid:24631525
  47. 47. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
  48. 48. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.; 1996.
  49. 49. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.; 1997.
  50. 50. Kretschmann HJ, Weinrich W. Neuroanatomy of cranial computed tomography. New York, NY: Thieme-Stratton, Inc.; 1985.
  51. 51. Degl’ Innocenti A, Hassing LB, Lindqvist A-S, Andersson H, Eriksson L, Hanson FH, et al. First report from the Swedish National Forensic Psychiatric Register (SNFPR). Int J Law Psychiatry. 2014;37: 231–237. pmid:24295538
  52. 52. Goozée R, Handley R, Kempton MJ, Dazzan P. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of antipsychotic medications on regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in schizophrenia: association with response to treatment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;43: 118–136. pmid:24690578
  53. 53. Mori K, Teramoto K, Nagao M, Horiguchi J, Yamawaki S. Regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia using stable xenon-enhanced computed tomography. Neuropsychobiology. 1999;39: 117–124. pmid:10087455
  54. 54. Nilsson T, Wallinius M, Gustavson C, Anckarsäter H, Kerekes N. Violent recidivism: a long-time follow-up study of mentally disordered offenders. PLoS One. 2011;6: e25768. pmid:22022445
  55. 55. Lu H, Xu F, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Cheng Y, Flicker B, et al. Alterations in cerebral metabolic rate and blood supply across the adult lifespan. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21: 1426–1434. pmid:21051551
  56. 56. Chen JJ, Rosas HD, Salat DH. Age-associated reductions in cerebral blood flow are independent from regional atrophy. Neuroimage. 2011;55: 468–478. pmid:21167947
  57. 57. Buijs PC, Krabbe-Hartkamp MJ, Bakker CJ, de Lange EE, Ramos LM, Breteler MM, et al. Effect of age on cerebral blood flow: measurement with ungated two-dimensional phase-contrast MR angiography in 250 adults. Radiology. 1998;209: 667–674. pmid:9844657
  58. 58. Hagstadius S, Risberg J. Regional cerebral blood flow characteristics and variations with age in resting normal subjects. Brain Cogn. 1989;10: 28–43. pmid:2713143
  59. 59. Aanerud J, Borghammer P, Chakravarty MM, Vang K, Rodell AB, Jónsdottir KY, et al. Brain energy metabolism and blood flow differences in healthy aging. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2012;32: 1177–1187. pmid:22373642
  60. 60. Martin AJ, Friston KJ, Colebatch JG, Frackowiak RS. Decreases in regional cerebral blood flow with normal aging. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1991;11: 684–689. pmid:2050757
  61. 61. Schultz SK, O’Leary DS, Boles Ponto LL, Arndt S, Magnotta V, Watkins GL, et al. Age and regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia: age effects in anterior cingulate, frontal, and parietal cortex. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;14: 19–24. pmid:11884650
  62. 62. Hart SD, Cox DN, Hare RD. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL: SV). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems; 1995.
  63. 63. Grann M, Långström N, Tengström A, Stålenheim EG. Reliability of file-based retrospective ratings of psychopathy with the PCL-R. J Pers Assess. 1998;70: 416–426. pmid:9760735
  64. 64. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria; 2017.
  65. 65. Gandrud C. Reproducible Research with R and R Studio, Second Edition. Taylor & Francis; 2015.
  66. 66. Barnard GA. A New test for 2 x 2 Tables. Nature. 1945;3974: 783–784.
  67. 67. Lydersen S, Fagerland MW, Laake P. Recommended tests for association in 2 x 2 tables. Stat Med. 2009;28: 1159–1175. pmid:19170020
  68. 68. Delacre M, Lakens D, Leys C. Why Psychologists Should by Default Use Welch’s t-test Instead of Student's t-test. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. rips.ubiquitypress.com; 2017; Available: http://rips.ubiquitypress.com/articles/10.5334/irsp.82/
  69. 69. Breiman L. Random Forests. Mach Learn. Kluwer Academic Publishers; 2001;45: 5–32.
  70. 70. Breiman L, Friedman J, Stone CJ, Olshen RA. Classification and regression trees. New York, NY: Chapman & Hall; 1984.
  71. 71. Scornet E, Biau G, Vert J-P. Consistency of random forests. Ann Stat. Institute of Mathematical Statistics; 2015;43: 1716–1741.
  72. 72. Pflueger MO, Franke I, Graf M, Hachtel H. Predicting general criminal recidivism in mentally disordered offenders using a random forest approach. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15: 62. pmid:25885691
  73. 73. Yarkoni T, Westfall J. Choosing Prediction Over Explanation in Psychology: Lessons From Machine Learning. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12: 1100–1122. pmid:28841086
  74. 74. Kuhn M, Johnson K. Applied predictive modeling. New York, NY: Springer; 2013.
  75. 75. Rice ME, Harris GT. Comparing effect sizes in follow-up studies: ROC Area, Cohen’s d, and r. Law Hum Behav. 2005;29: 615–620. pmid:16254746
  76. 76. Sjöstedt G, Grann M. Risk Assessment: What is Being Predicted by Actuarial Prediction Instruments? Int J Forensic Ment Health. Routledge; 2002;1: 179–183.
  77. 77. Wu Y-C, Lee W-C. Alternative performance measures for prediction models. PLoS One. 2014;9: e91249. pmid:24608868
  78. 78. Steyerberg EW, Vickers AJ, Cook NR, Gerds T, Gonen M, Obuchowski N, et al. Assessing the performance of prediction models: a framework for traditional and novel measures. Epidemiology. 2010;21: 128–138. pmid:20010215
  79. 79. Friedman JH. Greedy Function Approximation: A Gradient Boosting Machine. Ann Stat. Institute of Mathematical Statistics; 2001;29: 1189–1232.
  80. 80. Farrington DP. Cross-national comparative research on criminal careers, risk factors, crime and punishment. European Journal of Criminology. SAGE Publications; 2015;12: 386–399.
  81. 81. Coid J, Yang M, Tyrer P, Roberts A, Ullrich S. Prevalence and correlates of personality disorder in Great Britain. Br J Psychiatry. RCP; 2006;188: 423–431.
  82. 82. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Smailes E, Kasen S, Oldham JM, Skodol AE, et al. Adolescent personality disorders associated with violence and criminal behavior during adolescence and early adulthood. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157: 1406–1412. pmid:10964855
  83. 83. Pitzer M, Esser G, Schmidt MH, Laucht M. Early predictors of antisocial developmental pathways among boys and girls. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;121: 52–64. pmid:19489749
  84. 84. Asselmann E, Wittchen H-U, Lieb R, Beesdo-Baum K. Sociodemographic, clinical, and functional long-term outcomes in adolescents and young adults with mental disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017; pmid:28861892
  85. 85. Balyakina E, Mann C, Ellison M, Sivernell R, Fulda KG, Sarai SK, et al. Risk of future offense among probationers with co-occurring substance use and mental health disorders. Community Ment Health J. 2014;50: 288–295. pmid:23765181
  86. 86. Morgan RD, Flora DB, Kroner DG, Mills JF, Varghese F, Steffan JS. Treating offenders with mental illness: a research synthesis. Law Hum Behav. 2012;36: 37–50. pmid:22471384
  87. 87. Martin MS, Dorken SK, Wamboldt AD, Wootten SE. Stopping the revolving door: a meta-analysis on the effectiveness of interventions for criminally involved individuals with major mental disorders. Law Hum Behav. 2012;36: 1–12. pmid:22471380
  88. 88. Brazil KJ, Forth AE. Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL:SV). Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. 2016. pp. 1–4.
  89. 89. Pedersen L, Kunz C, Rasmussen K, Elsass P. Psychopathy as a Risk Factor for Violent Recidivism: Investigating the Psychopathy Checklist Screening Version (PCL:SV) and the Comprehensive Assessment of Psychopathic Personality (CAPP) in a Forensic Psychiatric Setting. Int J Forensic Ment Health. Routledge; 2010;9: 308–315.
  90. 90. Nicholls TL, Ogloff JRP, Douglas KS. Assessing risk for violence among male and female civil psychiatric patients: the HCR-20, PCL:SV, and VSC. Behav Sci Law. 2004;22: 127–158. pmid:14963884
  91. 91. Doyle M, Dolan M, McGovern J. The validity of North American risk assessment tools in predicting in-patient violent behaviour in England. Legal and Criminological Psychology. 2002;7: 141–154.
  92. 92. Dean K, Mortensen PB, Stevens H, Murray RM, Walsh E, Agerbo E. Criminal conviction among offspring with parental history of mental disorder. Psychol Med. 2012;42: 571–581. pmid:21846422
  93. 93. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication II: associations with persistence of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67: 124–132. pmid:20124112
  94. 94. Maas C, Herrenkohl TI, Sousa C. Review of research on child maltreatment and violence in youth. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2008;9: 56–67. pmid:18182631
  95. 95. Currie J, Tekin E. Understanding the cycle: childhood maltreatment and future crime. J Hum Resour. 2012;47: 509–549. pmid:24204082
  96. 96. Moffitt TE. Parental Mental Disorder and Offspring Criminal Behavior. Psychiatry. 1987;50: 346–360.
  97. 97. Rubia K, Russell T, Overmeyer S, Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Sharma T, et al. Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks. Neuroimage. 2001;13: 250–261. pmid:11162266
  98. 98. Menon V, Adleman NE, White CD, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;12: 131–143. pmid:11170305
  99. 99. Watanabe J, Sugiura M, Sato K, Sato Y, Maeda Y, Matsue Y, et al. The human prefrontal and parietal association cortices are involved in NO-GO performances: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2002;17: 1207–1216. pmid:12414261
  100. 100. Zhang R, Geng X, Lee TMC. Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222: 3973–3990. pmid:28551777
  101. 101. Velanova K, Wheeler ME, Luna B. Maturational changes in anterior cingulate and frontoparietal recruitment support the development of error processing and inhibitory control. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18: 2505–2522. pmid:18281300
  102. 102. Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM, et al. Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45: 468–475. pmid:16601652
  103. 103. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L. Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology. 2009;207: 163–172. pmid:19727677
  104. 104. Kamarajan C, Porjesz B, Jones KA, Chorlian DB, Padmanabhapillai A, Rangaswamy M, et al. Spatial-anatomical mapping of NoGo-P3 in the offspring of alcoholics: evidence of cognitive and neural disinhibition as a risk for alcoholism. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116: 1049–1061. pmid:15826845
  105. 105. Sjöwall D, Roth L, Lindqvist S, Thorell LB. Multiple deficits in ADHD: executive dysfunction, delay aversion, reaction time variability, and emotional deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54: 619–627. pmid:23061803
  106. 106. Pawliczek CM, Derntl B, Kellermann T, Kohn N, Gur RC, Habel U. Inhibitory control and trait aggression: neural and behavioral insights using the emotional stop signal task. Neuroimage. 2013;79: 264–274. pmid:23660028
  107. 107. Hancock M, Tapscott JL, Hoaken PNS. Role of executive dysfunction in predicting frequency and severity of violence. Aggress Behav. 2010;36: 338–349. pmid:20593426
  108. 108. Meijers J, Harte JM, Jonker FA, Meynen G. Prison brain? Executive dysfunction in prisoners. Front Psychol. 2015;6: 43. pmid:25688221
  109. 109. Friedman NP, Miyake A, Young SE, Defries JC, Corley RP, Hewitt JK. Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2008;137: 201–225. pmid:18473654
  110. 110. Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Paquier PF, De Deyn PP, Mariën P. Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110: 763–773. pmid:18602745
  111. 111. Gordon N. The cerebellum and cognition. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2007;11: 232–234. pmid:17400009
  112. 112. Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain. 1998;121 (Pt 4): 561–579.
  113. 113. Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20: 236–260. pmid:20821056
  114. 114. Bufkin JL, Luttrell VR. Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2005;6: 176–191. pmid:15753199
  115. 115. Volkow ND, Tancredi L. Neural substrates of violent behaviour. A preliminary study with positron emission tomography. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;151: 668–673. pmid:3502251
  116. 116. Dotterer HL, Hyde LW, Swartz JR, Hariri AR, Williamson DE. Amygdala reactivity predicts adolescent antisocial behavior but not callous-unemotional traits. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;24: 84–92. pmid:28279916
  117. 117. da Cunha-Bang S, Fisher PM, Hjordt LV, Perfalk E, Persson Skibsted A, Bock C, et al. Violent offenders respond to provocations with high amygdala and striatal reactivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12: 802–810. pmid:28338916
  118. 118. da Cunha-Bang S, Fisher PM, Hjordt LV, Holst K, Knudsen GM. Amygdala reactivity to fearful faces correlates positively with impulsive aggression. Soc Neurosci. 2018; 1–11.
  119. 119. Fanning JR, Berman ME, Mohn RS, McCloskey MS. Perceived threat mediates the relationship between psychosis proneness and aggressive behavior. Psychiatry Res. 2011;186: 210–218. pmid:20965573
  120. 120. Birur B, Kraguljac NV, Shelton RC, Lahti AC. Brain structure, function, and neurochemistry in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder-a systematic review of the magnetic resonance neuroimaging literature. NPJ Schizophr. 2017;3: 15. pmid:28560261
  121. 121. Liberg B, Rahm C, Panayiotou A, Pantelis C. Brain change trajectories that differentiate the major psychoses. Eur J Clin Invest. 2016;46: 658–674. pmid:27208657
  122. 122. Bartholomeusz CF, Cropley VL, Wannan C, Di Biase M, McGorry PD, Pantelis C. Structural neuroimaging across early-stage psychosis: Aberrations in neurobiological trajectories and implications for the staging model. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2017;51: 455–476. pmid:27733710
  123. 123. Vita A, De Peri L, Deste G, Sacchetti E. Progressive loss of cortical gray matter in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis and meta-regression of longitudinal MRI studies. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2: e190. pmid:23168990
  124. 124. Fusar-Poli P, Smieskova R, Kempton MJ, Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Borgwardt S. Progressive brain changes in schizophrenia related to antipsychotic treatment? A meta-analysis of longitudinal MRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37: 1680–1691. pmid:23769814
  125. 125. Kilroy E, Liu CY, Yan L, Kim YC, Dapretto M, Mendez MF, et al. Relationships between Cerebral Blood Flow and IQ in Typically Developing Children and Adolescents. J Cogn Sci. 2011;12: 151–170.
  126. 126. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Hashizume H, Sassa Y, Nagase T, Nouchi R, et al. Cerebral blood flow during rest associates with general intelligence and creativity. PLoS One. 2011;6: e25532. pmid:21980485
  127. 127. van der Gronde T, Kempes M, van El C, Rinne T, Pieters T. Neurobiological correlates in forensic assessment: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2014;9: e110672. pmid:25330208
  128. 128. Buchanan A. Risk of violence by psychiatric patients: beyond the “actuarial versus clinical” assessment debate. Psychiatr Serv. 2008;59: 184–190. pmid:18245161
  129. 129. Constantinou AC, Freestone M, Marsh W, Coid J. Causal inference for violence risk management and decision support in forensic psychiatry. Decis Support Syst. 2015;80: 42–55.