Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Exploratory study evaluating the relationships between perinatal adversity, oxidative stress, and infant neurodevelopment across the first year of life

  • Kameelah Gateau ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    kgateau@chla.usc.edu

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Lisa Schlueter,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Developmental Neuroscience and Neurogenetics Program, The Saban Research Institute, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Lara J. Pierce,

    Roles Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation York University, Department of Psychology, Toronto, ON, Canada

  • Barbara Thompson,

    Roles Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Pediatrics and Human Development, Michigan State University, Grand Rapids, Michigan, United States of America

  • Alma Gharib,

    Roles Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America, Developmental Neuroscience and Neurogenetics Program, The Saban Research Institute, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Ramon A. Durazo-Arvizu,

    Roles Formal analysis, Validation

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Charles A. Nelson,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Pat Levitt

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, United States of America, York University, Department of Psychology, Toronto, ON, Canada

Abstract

Early childhood adversity increases risk for negative lifelong impacts on health and wellbeing. Identifying the risk factors and the associated biological adaptations early in life is critical to develop scalable early screening tools and interventions. Currently, there are limited, reliable early childhood adversity measures that can be deployed prospectively, at scale, to assess risk in pediatric settings. The goal of this two-site longitudinal study was to determine if the gold standard measure of oxidative stress, F2-Isoprostanes, is potentially a reliable measure of a physiological response to adversity of the infant and mother. The study evaluated the independent relationships between F2-Isoprostanes, perinatal adversity and infant neurocognitive development. The study included mother-infant dyads born >36 weeks’ gestation. Maternal demographic information and mental health assessments were utilized to generate a perinatal cumulative risk score. Infants’ development was assessed at 6 and 12 months and both mothers and infants were assayed for F2-isoprostane levels in blood and urine, respectively. Statistical analysis revealed that cumulative risk scores correlated with higher maternal (p = 0.01) and infant (p = 0.05) F2-isoprostane levels at 6 months. Infant F2-isoprostane measures at 2 months were negatively associated with Mullen Scales of Early Learning Composite scores at 12 months (p = 0.04). Lastly, higher cumulative risk scores predicted higher average maternal F2-isoprostane levels across the 1-year study time period (p = 0.04). The relationship between perinatal cumulative risk scores and higher maternal and infant F2-isoprostanes at 6 months may reflect an oxidative stress status that informs a sensitive period in which a biomarker can be utilized prospectively to reveal the physiological impact of early adversity.

Introduction

At a population level, experiencing adversity early in childhood has been associated with long term mental and physical health morbidities [1, 2]. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACES) study established a link between seven domains of adversity (psychological, physical or sexual abuse, parental violence, exposure to substance abuse, mentally ill or imprisoned household members) and adult health risk behaviors and chronic illness [1]. Since that study, there have been numerous reports that have validated the associations that are described at a population level. Nationally,15–20% of all individuals report having experienced 4 or more early adversity events that are associated with a 20-year reduction in total lifespan [24]. Chronic adult diseases that are associated with ACES generate the most healthcare costs [5]. Additionally, among the leading causes of death (cancer, heart disease and suicide), 15% of mortality is attributable to experiencing childhood adversity (CA) [2]. While the link between adversity and poorer health outcomes has been established, recent studies report that current ACES screening at the individual level are not predictive of later life disease risk [6, 7]. Additionally, the psychological challenges, including stress, anxiety and depression, and socioeconomic instability due to the COVID 19 pandemic has been reported to increase the risk of a child experiencing early life adversity and its downstream effects [810]. Though the COVID-19 pandemic is both an acute and pervasive potential source of CA, recent findings continue to rely on retrospective reporting. Thus, despite the heightened concern for CA in this current public health crisis, there are not reliable and prospective scalable measures of early adversity.

Picard et al established the prevailing hypothesis linking early CA to disruption of typical development, theorizing that chronic stress disrupts homeostasis that generates “allostatic load” on the brain and body physiological systems [11, 12]. Not all stressors are equivalent, and when they are brief or isolated and buffered by support systems the physiologic response is acute (positive or tolerable stress responses) and recovery to physiological homeostasis occurs [11, 13, 14]. However, a toxic stress response occurs when a child faces multiple, concomitant and severe stressors with limited socioemotional buffers [13]. For example, when some children experience multiple adverse environmental and psychosocial risk factors, there appears to be a higher likelihood of psychological harm and psychiatric disorders[15, 16]. Developmental literature too has demonstrated that multiple adverse risk factors experienced in conjunction with one another lead to poorer cognitive outcomes and educational achievement [17]. For example, the findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project highlight how the absence of crucial early social engagement and environmental inputs for institutionalized children were associated with impaired executive functioning and cognitive abilities, language deficits and higher prevalence of psychological disorders [1821].

Physiologically, the toxic stress response consists of a derangement of the normal neuro-endocrine-immune interactions, alterations of mitochondrial function and oxidative stress, with failure of the body to normalize these changes after the stressor is removed [2225]. Oxidative stress is hypothesized to be one of the major homeostatic dysregulations contributing to allostatic load and toxic stress response [25]. Oxidative stress occurs when there is an imbalance of reactive oxidative species (ROS) and the neutralizing capacity of antioxidant systems, leading to disruption of normal cellular function [12, 24]. The brain is particularly susceptible to ROS [26].

Both animal and human studies have demonstrated the association between exposure to early life adversity and oxidative stress [2731]. In mice, it has been demonstrated that early life stress alters mitochondria and neurodevelopmental proteins, leading to disruption of mitochondrial energetics [24]. The relationship between oxidative stress and neurodevelopmental outcomes was hypothesized by Ross and colleagues. Ross postulated that oxidative stress and elevated ROS was the link between low levels of poly-unsaturated fatty acids and elevated phospholipase A2 levels (the enzyme that cleaves damaged fatty acids from the cell membrane) seen in patients with neurodevelopmental disorders [32]. In a recent study elevated maternal oxidative stress in the third trimester of pregnancy was associated with social impairments in their children at 4 years of age [31]. Another recent study demonstrated an association of CA, oxidative stress, and mental health in adolescent girls in the juvenile justice system [33]. This study found a significant association between elevated oxidative stress marker F2-isoprostane, internalizing disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, depressive disorder, bulimia and anorexia nervosa, dysthymic disorder) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Studies have demonstrated that F2-isoprostane (8-IsoF2a) serves as a sensitive, gold standard biomarker of oxidative stress [34, 35]. 8-IsoF2a metabolites are generated as the result of peroxidation of arachidonic acid by ROS and can be quantified in urine and blood serum samples [34, 36, 37]. At the time of birth, newborns exhibit a physiologic oxidative stress response as they transition from a low oxygen environment in utero to a relatively high oxygen environment. Freil and colleagues demonstrated that serum 8-IsoF2a levels in infants at birth are markedly elevated and decline precipitously over the first 6 months of life to stable, adult levels [38]. Importantly, the elevated 8-IsoF2a levels in infants at birth are not influenced by 8-IsoF2a levels during pregnancy, with no indication of transplacental transference [39]. With established normative values, being easily assessable, and no vertical transmissibility, 8-IsoF2a is an ideal potential candidate biomarker of early adversity.

Dimensional approaches to capture early adversity provide opportunities to both identify elements of experiences that are most potent in derailing homeostasis and characterize the interdependent nature of these factors. Approaching quantification and assessment of early adversity from a dimensional wholistic approach should also include consideration of the screening window. Studies both in animals and humans have demonstrated the association of maternal stress during pregnancy and altered neurocognitive development and stress responses in offspring, highlighting a crucial screening window existing in the perinatal (pregnancy and first postpartum year) time periods[4043]. A review done by Van den Bergh et al. reported on seventeen studies that evaluated the relationship between maternal perinatal stress and neurodevelopmental outcomes [41]. This review found that perinatal stress was associated with cognitive, behavioral and emotional problems in infants, preschool and adolescent children. Work done by Silvera and colleagues evaluated cumulative perinatal adversity’s association with developmental outcomes. This study found that higher perinatal adversity scores were significantly associated with lower neurodevelopmental and school readiness scores and increased behavioral problems [43]. These important findings not only infer a crucial perinatal screening period they also highlight the long-lasting effects experiencing early adversity has on neurodevelopmental trajectories.

When forecasting scalability of early adversity screening, an understanding of the mechanisms underlying the impact of early adversity on neurodevelopmental outcomes is also needed. Our mediation model, adapted from Horn et al, presented in Fig 1, hypothesizes a mechanism of how perinatal adversity may impact infant neurodevelopment [33]. To our knowledge no studies to date have prospectively evaluated the relationships between perinatal adversity, oxidative stress, and neurocognitive development in infants. Given the limited sample size we provide exploratory data analyses to evaluate the viability of the mediation model proposed in Fig 1. In this study, we aimed to evaluate the individual associations between perinatal adversity, oxidative stress as measured by 8-IsoF2a and infant cognitive development.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Adapted mediation model of early childhood adversity, oxidative stress and neurocognitive development [33].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgph.0001984.g001

Methods

Ethics statement

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from Institutional Review Boards (IRB) at both sites (Children’s Hospital Los Angeles Institutional Review Board and Boston Children’s Hospital Institutional Review Board). Informed written consent was obtained from the mother for all mother- infant dyads who participated in the study.

Participants

The study included 116 mother- infant dyads recruited between 2016–2019 from pediatric practices affiliated with Boston Children’s Hospital and Children’s Hospital Los Angeles. Participants were recruited if they met the following inclusion criteria: receiving early postnatal services at the designated primary care clinics, mothers aged 18 or older with an infant < 2 months, and birth weight greater than the 20th percentile (at least 2,500 grams and typically developing).

Exclusion criteria included infants who were born prematurely (< 36 weeks of gestation); infants who had congenital disorders (genetic, metabolic, malformations, or neurological disorder etc.); infants who experienced birth complications (fetal distress, requiring resuscitation at birth, meconium aspiration syndrome, etc). Study visits took place at 2, 6, 9, and 12 months from 2016 to 2019. All deidentified study data were entered into REDCap by the research coordinator. No author had access to information that could identify individual participants during or after data collection.

Measures

Family demographic factors.

The family demographic factors were collected through self-report from mothers at the 2- month visit. Demographic factors of interest were marital status, highest level of education completed, range of annual family income, age, and racial identity (which we recognize as a social construct). Marital status was represented as single (single, widowed, separated, divorced) or partnered (married, cohabiting). Maternal education was assessed as seven categories ranging from 8th grade or less to M.D., Ph.D., J.D., or equivalent. The nine categories of annual family income ranged from <$5,000 to $100,000+. Maternal age was self-reported at the time of the 2-month visit. Participants were asked to select a racial category from a selection of 14 different identities which included the following options: White, Black or African American, American Indian or Alaska Native, Asian Indian, Chinese, Filipino, Japanese, Korean, Vietnamese, Other Asian, Native Hawaiian, Guamanian or Chomorro, Samoan, and Other Pacific Islander. Racial information collected about the study participants in Boston only included the infants, thus the race information analyzed in this study are of the race designations of the infants as reported by their mothers.

Maternal mental health.

Two assessments of maternal mental health were utilized in this study. The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) is a validated 10-item screening tool frequently used in the perinatal and postnatal setting to identify mothers with depression [44]. Depressive symptoms are assessed using a four-tiered severity scale for the prior 7 days from the research visit. Scores range from 0 to 30, with a score of 13 or greater indicating risk for major depression [44, 45]. The EPDS was collected at 2 months postpartum. The Patient Health Questionnaire 9 (PHQ9) is a 9-item assessment used to identify and monitor over time major depressive symptoms in the general population and in the postpartum period [46, 47]. This assessment utilizes a four-tiered scale assessing the frequency of depressive symptoms over the past 2 weeks. The score ranges from 0 to 27, with a score of 10 or greater indicating elevated depressive symptoms. The PHQ9 was completed at 2 months postpartum. Both measures were used in this study to assess both anxiety and depressive symptoms (EPDS) and the constitutional symptoms of depression (PHQ9) [47].

Cumulative risk score

The cumulative risk score (CRS) assessing perinatal adversity was calculated using the cumulative risk modeling described by Evans et al [48]. Demographic factors including age, highest education attained, marital status, family income and race were standardized into risk categories and calculated as being present (1) or absent (0). Risk delineations for each demographic factor were calculated based on a participant occupying the lowest standard deviation of the sample mean (age< = 25 years, income <$25,000) or occupying a demographic category previously described as having higher risk for infant morbidity and mortality (Black race, < = High School education, single marital status) [4951]. Approximately 30% of participants did not provide income data. Rates of missing data did not differ across study sites. For those participants with missing income data, risk stratifications were assigned using neighborhood poverty levels and the Internal Revenue Services’ census tract definition of low-income communities (LIC) [5254]. Mental health risk scaled scores were generated from previously described risk stratified scoring guidelines for PHQ9 and EPDS. Scores were standardized as low/no risk (0), medium risk (1) or high risk (2) [4447]. Total cumulative risk score (CRS) for each participant was calculated by summation of all demographic and mental health risk categories occupied to create an overall perinatal cumulative risk score, with a range of cumulative risk scores from 0–9.

Cognitive scores

Infant cognition was measured using The Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). The MSEL is a play-based developmental assessment that evaluates gross motor, visual reception, fine motor, expressive language, and receptive language [55]. An early learning composite score (ELC) is generated from the assessments excluding gross motor (excluded per standard practice). This composite score is then standardized to compare cognitive developmental status from birth through sixty-eight months of age and has a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. [55]. Assessments were done at 6 and 12 at both study sites by trained and assessment-validated members of the research team in either English or Spanish, based on the primary language spoken at home.

Oxidative markers

The lipid oxidation biomarker F2 -isoprostane (8-IsoF2a) was the oxidative marker of interest. 8-IsoF2a is one of several isoprostane products generated by reactive oxygen species peroxidation of arachidonic acid [32, 33]. Infant urine samples were collected via bag specimen at all 4 study visits, aliquoted and stored frozen at –80°C within 1 hour of collection. For urine, adjustments were made for potential concentration differences by measuring creatinine in each sample. No other exogenous or endogenous factors have been consistently shown to effect urinary 8-IsoF2a levels thus no other adjustments were made [56]. Concentrations were expressed as ng/mg creatinine. Maternal blood samples were collected by a trained phlebotomist at all 4 study visits. Urine and blood samples were shipped in batches, deidentified with a subject code, to the Eicosanoid Core Laboratory at Vanderbilt University for 8-IsoF2a analyses. 8-IsoF2a was measured using gas chromatography/negative ion chemical ionization mass spectrometry, following previously published methods [34, 36, 57]

Statistical analysis

Demographic characteristics of both mothers and infants, infant MSEL scores and maternal and infant 8-IsoF2α levels were compared by site using chi-squared analysis for discrete categorical variables and independent student t-test for continuous variables. Due to skewing in the data, EPDS and PHQ9 scores were compared using a Mann-Whitney U test. CRS between both sites were evaluated by independent student t-test. A priori α to distinguish differences between sites was set as 0.05.

A sub-analysis by study location was done to evaluate the participants with missing income data. Chi-squared analysis was utilized to evaluate site differences in missing income data. Demographic, EPDS and PHQ9 variables for participants with missing income data were evaluated in comparison to individuals with income data as described above. Pearson Correlation Coefficients were calculated to evaluate the association between LIC census tract data and participants with reported income.

Perinatal adversity and oxidative stress Pearson Correlation Coefficients were used to evaluate the bivariate associations between infant and maternal 8-IsoF2α and CRS. The effect of perinatal adversity on oxidative stress was also assessed by fitting a linear mixed-effects model (LME) with 8-IsoF2α over the first year of life (measured at 2, 6, 9, and 12 months) as the dependent variable and CRS as the independent variable. LME models take advantage of the within-individual correlation and make it possible to include research participants with incomplete observations under robust statistical assumptions.

Oxidative stress and infant neurocognitive development The bivariate associations between infant and maternal 8-IsoF2α and MSEL scores at 6 and 12 months were assessed via Pearson Correlation Coefficients and Linear regression model that measured the change of MSEL scores between 6 and 12 months, and the change of 8-IsoF2α during the same period.

Perinatal adversity and infant neurocognitive development The bivariate associations between MSEL and CRS were assessed via Pearson Correlation Coefficients. The change in the MSEL score from month 6 to month 12 was regressed on the CRS score to test the association between perinatal adversity and infant cognitive development. All analyses were performed using SPSS Version 27.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA) and STATA version 17.1 (College Station, TX).

Results

There were 116 mother-infant dyads recruited across both study sites. Six mother-infant dyads who did not meet inclusion criteria following enrollment were excluded. Developmental outcomes on infants with MSEL scores at 6 (n = 96) or 12 months (n = 86) were included in the analysis. Demographic characteristics and developmental scores for infants are presented in Table 1 and demographic characteristics and mental health scores seen in the mothers are presented in Table 2. Notably, there was a statistically significant difference (P<0.001) in race across the two study sites with 78% of infants identified as White (which includes Hispanic and Non-Hispanic ethnicities) in Los Angeles and 56% identified as Black or African American in Boston (Table 1). As previously published (Valdes et al. 2020), there were site differences in MSEL scores at both 6 and 12 months with higher scores for the Boston population for both time points. Additionally, maternal demographics differed between the 2 study sites, including family income (p = 0.01), marital status (p = 0.03), and primary language spoken at home (p<0.001) (Table 2) [52]. The median scores for mental health assessments were 4 (IQR 1–7) for EPDS and 2 (IQR 0–4) for PHQ9, with no differences in scores between the two sites for either measure (Table 2).

A maximum CRS of 8 was seen (out a maximum achievable score of 9) and there were no significant differences in mean scores between sites; with Boston having a mean CRS of 2.8 (SD 1.9) and Los Angeles having a mean CRS of 2.7 (SD 1.3) (p = 0.65). Analysis of reported income and LIC census tract data demonstrated a significant correlation between living in a LIC and a reported income of less than $25,000 (r = 0.36, p<0.001). Sub-analysis of the 32 participants with missing income data demonstrated 14% were from Boston and 15% were from Los Angeles. For participants with missing income data, there were no statistically significant differences in demographic, EPDS or PHQ 9 variables, except infant race (p = 0.001), which is consistent with results of the aggregate analysis.

The bivariate correlation coefficients depicted in Table 3 represent the individual associations of maternal and infant 8-IsoF2α, cumulative risk score and infant MSEL scores at 6 and 12 months. Elevated 8-IsoF2a levels were significantly correlated with elevated cumulative risk scores at 6 months in both infants (p = 0.05) and mothers (p = 0.01). Additionally, higher infant 8-IsoF2a levels at 2 months were significantly associated with lower MSEL scores at 12 months (p = 0.04). Cumulative risk scores were not statistically significantly correlated with infant MSEL scores at 6 or 12 months.

thumbnail
Table 3. Pearson correlations of maternal and infant 8-IsoF2a, cumulative risk scores, and infant MSEL scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgph.0001984.t003

To date, there is a lack of data on changes of 8-IsoF2a levels over the first postnatal year for infants and their mothers serving as primary caregivers. The longitudinal data collected in the present study provided an opportunity to perform analyses using a mixed effects model strategy. First, Fig 2 reports significant differences of 8-IsoF2a levels over time for both infants and mothers (p<0.001). At 2 months, higher infants 8 -IsoF2α levels were observed for infants located in Los Angeles (7.1 ng/mg Cr) compared to Boston (4.1 ng/mg Cr; p< 0.01) (Fig 2A). There were no differences in infant 8-IsoF2a levels between sites at the other time points (6, 9 or 12 month visit, p > 0.05 for each comparison). At 9 months, higher maternal 8-IsoF2α levels were observed for mothers located in Los Angeles (0.03 ng/dl) compared to Boston (0.02 ng/dl; p<0.001) (Fig 2B). No differences for maternal 8-IsoF2a levels were observed between sites at the other time points (2, 6, or 12 month visit).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Mean 8-IsoF2a levels across the first year of life.

(A) Infant 8-IsoF2a. (B) Maternal 8-IsoF2a. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval. *p<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgph.0001984.g002

Second, the linear mixed effects model (MLE) evaluated the association of CRS with maternal and infant 8-IsoF2a (Table 4). Notably, higher CRS predicted higher average maternal 8-IsoF2a across the 1-year study time period. When adjusting the model for each individual time point higher CRS still predicted significantly higher maternal 8-IsoF2a regardless of the study time point. CRS, however, did not predict infant 8-IsoF2a levels (Table 4). Regression models evaluating the relationship between maternal and infant 8-IsoF2a & infant MSEL and CRS & MSEL were not statistically significant.

thumbnail
Table 4. Linear mixed effects models of CRS as a predictor of maternal and infant 8-IsoF2a across the first year of life.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgph.0001984.t004

Discussion

The primary goal of the present exploratory study was to evaluate the potential individual associations between perinatal adversity, maternal and infant oxidative stress and infant cognitive development as measured by MSEL composite scores. For this study, we generated a cumulative risk score (CRS), using a previously published strategy to capture perinatal adversity through assessment of maternal mental health scores and demographic factors [48]. Statistical analyses revealed that higher CRS were associated with higher infant and maternal 8-IsoF2a levels at 6 months. Additionally, higher infant 8-IsoF2a levels at 2 months were associated with lower MSEL scores at 12 months. Lastly, higher CRS significantly predicted higher average maternal 8-IsoF2a levels over the first year of life. No association was observed between CRS and infant cognitive scores. We note that there are limitations in interpreting CRS, due to the lack of inclusion of structural and socio-economic factors, as well as measures of resilience.

Despite these challenges, the current study introduced for the first time a measure that is more proximate to hypothesized adaptive physiological processes that may occur due to the experience of early adversity. Oxidative stress, mediated through ROS has been hypothesized to be involved in perturbations of the stress response that may subsequently yield a toxic stress response [22, 23, 25, 3537]. There have been numerous studies that have linked elevated levels of 8-IsoF2a to adult morbidities including heart disease and depression [58, 59]. Pediatric studies have found an association between elevated 8-IsoF2a with growth restriction and poor respiratory and neurodevelopmental outcomes [31, 33, 60, 61]. There have been no prospective longitudinal studies in the perinatal and postpartum periods, however, that have evaluated the association between early experiences of adversity, oxidative stress and infant neurodevelopment. Measures of potential oxidative stress in primary caregivers also is a novel dimension of the current study.

The findings in the present study showing that infant 8-IsoF2a levels decrease by 6 months and then subsequently increase, together with the correlation of elevated cumulative risk and elevated 6-month infant 8-IsoF2a levels are different from previously established temporal changes of oxidative stress. Infants at birth have a physiologically elevated ROS burden that is 4-fold higher than normal adult values, due to transitioning from a relatively hypoxic environment to a hyperoxic environment [38]. The levels of ROS during the first 6 months normally drop precipitously to adult levels [38]. In this study, however, levels between 2 and 6 months decrease but then increase. Additionally, the finding that higher CRS predicted higher maternal 8-IsoF2a across first year postnatal may indicate the utility of 8-IsoF2a as a longitudinal marker of a toxic stress response. The data captured in the present exploratory study suggest that future studies should include biomarkers of metabolic disruption early in the postnatal period to validate the sensitivity and specificity of this measure.

Interestingly, while higher 2 month infant 8-IsoF2a were significantly correlated with lower MSEL scores at 12 months, higher 6 month infant 8-IsoF2a were significantly correlated with higher MSEL scores at 12 months. This lone finding may due be in part to the 6-month time point being the physiologic nadir of oxidative stress and given most infants had lower levels of 8-IsoF2a this finding may be driven by a few individuals. Ongoing studies by this group with larger more diverse sample sizes are likely to provide more information about these correlations. Additionally, there was not a statistically significant association between the CRS and MSEL scores at 6 and 12 months. This may be due in part to the limited number of high cumulative risk dyads in this study population. The mean CRS was approximately 2.7 out of a possible 9, indicating that a broader distribution in future studies is warranted. Such a study is underway, including a larger, more diverse sample. It is noteworthy that previous studies reporting associations between cumulative risk and developmental outcomes performed their earliest developmental evaluation at 24 months [43]. Further analysis of the developmental assessments and 8-IsoF2a levels of current dyads at later ages may reveal more robust relations with demographic and maternal mental health risk scores.

The exploration of a physiologic biomarker of a toxic stress response lends to the possibility of identifying a more informative measure of the homeostatic disruption that adversity confers. The self-report survey methods that anchor current adversity screening pose many challenges with forecasting individual health states, particularly in the pediatric period [6]. Many of the most widely used screening tools assess the most severe forms of adversity, limiting the detection of more subtle forms or variable responses to adversity [62]. Additionally the current screening surveys were developed from retrospective adult studies that focus on familial and household dysfunction with little to no assessment of other forms of stress and adversity (i.e. bullying, discrimination, economic adversity etc.) [7]. In the present study, adversity was assessed through a cumulative risk score that included economic, psychosocial, and severity graded mental health factors. These factors are important in understanding the structural and subjective context in which the mother-infant dyad exist, however, these assessments do not provide insight into the individual’s physiological response to these contexts. The multi-finality within the risk and adversity literature is often discussed in terms of buffering and resilience. More specifically, a toxic stress response occurs when a child faces multiple, concomitant, and severe stressors with limited socioemotional buffers available. Self-reported screening surveys, especially those that focus solely on adversity items and fail to include measures of buffering and resilience, alone are limited in their ability to reliably predict later individual negative outcomes. In contrast, biomarkers offer critical insight into the individual’s physiological responses to a myriad of risk and resilience factors. Additionally biomarkers have been shown to better predict health outcomes when compared to self-reported health status [63]. Thus, we suggest that the goal to reliably capture early risk due to childhood adversity should include both sensitive and specific objective biological measures.

There are many limitations to this study. First, we interpret the results with caution due to the limited sample size and relatively low prevalence of reported or endorsed cumulative risk in the study population. Second, the developmental window of 2–12 months is early, and does not provide evidence for stability of these findings. Thus, the impact of perinatal adversity on developmental trajectories at later ages is needed. This will be addressed in future studies as data from later ages are collected and analyzed. Third, income data for thirty percent of participants were not available and the risk designations for that variable were gathered from neighborhood poverty data, which cautions interpretability of the measured associations. Additionally, the cumulative risk score income and age cut off values were based on the observed distribution within the study population, which limits the generalizability of the score. Fifth, the maternal-infant relationship was also not assessed or included in the analysis constraining the ability to directly assess a toxic stress response in this sample. There was incomplete data on self-reported maternal race. Infant race therefore was used but may result in incomplete data capture when maternal and infant race is not concordant. Lastly, race is a social construct and a poor surrogate for capturing the true risk factors that inform the negative health outcomes that are experienced by historically marginalized individuals [64]. We recognize that more nuanced and wholistic measures are needed to capture the environment and experiences of individuals assigned or self-identifying in marginalized racial categories.

With these limitations in mind, the study did identify novel and possibly crucial associations between perinatal adversity and oxidative stress in both mothers and infants at 6 months. This may indicate a sensitive period during which physiological impact of early environmental risk can be assessed effectively using prospective measures. Future studies will fully evaluate how long-term developmental and oxidative stress status are informed by early maternal infant environment, particularly among mothers experiencing moderate to severe adversity.

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families for their participation in the studies, as well as their feedback to enhance the study visit experience. We thank research staff members Kathryn Arneaud and Alma Martinez for their efforts on this study.

References

  1. 1. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58. pmid:9635069
  2. 2. Grummitt LR, Kreski NT, Kim SG, Platt J, Keyes KM, McLaughlin KA. Association of Childhood Adversity With Morbidity and Mortality in US Adults: A Systematic Review. JAMA Pediatr. 2021. pmid:34605870
  3. 3. Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Health CoPAoCaF, Committee on Early Childhood Ao, and Dependent Care, Pediatrics SoDaB. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e232–46.
  4. 4. Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, Ramos Rodriguez G, Sethi D, Passmore J. Life course health consequences and associated annual costs of adverse childhood experiences across Europe and North America: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2019;4(10):e517–e28. pmid:31492648
  5. 5. Waters H, Graf M. The costs of chronic disease in the US. Santa Monica, CA: The Milken Institute. 2018.
  6. 6. Baldwin JR, Caspi A, Meehan AJ, Ambler A, Arseneault L, Fisher HL, et al. Population vs Individual Prediction of Poor Health From Results of Adverse Childhood Experiences Screening. JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175(4):385–93. pmid:33492366
  7. 7. SmithBattle L, Loman DG, Yoo JH, Cibulka N, Rariden C. Evidence for Revising the Adverse Childhood Experiences Screening Tool: a Scoping Review. J Child Adolesc Trauma. 2022;15(1):89–103. pmid:35222777
  8. 8. Spinelli M, Lionetti F, Pastore M, Fasolo M. Parents’ Stress and Children’s Psychological Problems in Families Facing the COVID-19 Outbreak in Italy. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1713. pmid:32719646
  9. 9. Trumello C, Bramanti SM, Lombardi L, Ricciardi P, Morelli M, Candelori C, et al. COVID-19 and home confinement: A study on fathers, father-child relationships and child adjustment. Child Care Health Dev. 2021.
  10. 10. Babore A, Trumello C, Lombardi L, Candelori C, Chirumbolo A, Cattelino E, et al. Mothers’ and Children’s Mental Health During the COVID-19 Pandemic Lockdown: The Mediating Role of Parenting Stress. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2021. pmid:34417927
  11. 11. Boyce WT, Levitt P, Martinez FD, McEwen BS, Shonkoff JP. Genes, Environments, and Time: The Biology of Adversity and Resilience. Pediatrics. 2021;147(2). pmid:33495368
  12. 12. Picard M, McEwen BS. Psychological Stress and Mitochondria: A Conceptual Framework. Psychosom Med. 2018;80(2):126–40. pmid:29389735
  13. 13. Shonkoff JP. Building a new biodevelopmental framework to guide the future of early childhood policy. Child Dev. 2010;81(1):357–67. pmid:20331672
  14. 14. Garner AS, Shonkoff JP, Health CoPAoCaF, Committee on Early Childhood Ao, and Dependent Care, Pediatrics SoDaB. Early childhood adversity, toxic stress, and the role of the pediatrician: translating developmental science into lifelong health. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e224–31.
  15. 15. Stress Rutter M., coping and development: some issues and some questions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1981;22(4):323–56.
  16. 16. Kraemer HC, Stice E, Kazdin A, Offord D, Kupfer D. How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(6):848–56. pmid:11384888
  17. 17. DelCarmen-Wiggins R, Carter A, ebrary I. Handbook of infant, toddler, and preschool mental health assessment. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press; 2004.
  18. 18. Wade M, Carroll D, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Associations between Early Psychosocial Deprivation, Cognitive and Psychiatric Morbidity, and Risk-taking Behavior in Adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2021:1–14. pmid:33629920
  19. 19. Wade M, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. Social communication deficits following early-life deprivation and relation to psychopathology: a randomized clinical trial of foster care. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020;61(12):1360–9. pmid:32222079
  20. 20. Wade M, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. Global deficits in executive functioning are transdiagnostic mediators between severe childhood neglect and psychopathology in adolescence. Psychol Med. 2020;50(10):1687–94. pmid:31391139
  21. 21. Humphreys KL, Guyon-Harris KL, Tibu F, Wade M, Nelson CA, Fox NA, et al. Psychiatric outcomes following severe deprivation in early childhood: Follow-up of a randomized controlled trial at age 16. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2020;88(12):1079–90. pmid:33370132
  22. 22. Council N. Excessive stress disrupts the development of brain architecture. Journal of Children’s Services. 2014;9:143–53.
  23. 23. McEwen BS. Allostasis and the Epigenetics of Brain and Body Health Over the Life Course: The Brain on Stress. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(6):551–2. pmid:28445556
  24. 24. Eagleson KL, Villaneuva M, Southern RM, Levitt P. Proteomic and mitochondrial adaptations to early-life stress are distinct in juveniles and adults. Neurobiol Stress. 2020;13:100251. pmid:33344706
  25. 25. Schiavone S, Jaquet V, Trabace L, Krause KH. Severe life stress and oxidative stress in the brain: from animal models to human pathology. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2013;18(12):1475–90. pmid:22746161
  26. 26. Gu Y, Dee CM, Shen J. Interaction of free radicals, matrix metalloproteinases and caveolin-1 impacts blood-brain barrier permeability. Front Biosci (Schol Ed). 2011;3(4):1216–31. pmid:21622267
  27. 27. Schnider M, Jenni R, Ramain J, Camporesi S, Golay P, Alameda L, et al. Time of exposure to social defeat stress during childhood and adolescence and redox dysregulation on long-lasting behavioral changes, a translational study. Translational Psychiatry. 2022;12(1). pmid:36163247
  28. 28. Malcon LMC, Wearick-Silva LE, Zaparte A, Orso R, Luft C, Tractenberg SG, et al. Maternal separation induces long-term oxidative stress alterations and increases anxiety-like behavior of male Balb/cJ mice. Exp Brain Res. 2020;238(9):2097–107. pmid:32656651
  29. 29. Mansur RB, Cunha GR, Asevedo E, Zugman A, Rios AC, Salum GA, et al. Perinatal complications, lipid peroxidation, and mental health problems in a large community pediatric sample. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(5):521–9. pmid:27785581
  30. 30. Boeck C, Koenig AM, Schury K, Geiger ML, Karabatsiakis A, Wilker S, et al. Inflammation in adult women with a history of child maltreatment: The involvement of mitochondrial alterations and oxidative stress. Mitochondrion. 2016;30:197–207. pmid:27530300
  31. 31. Rommel AS, Milne GL, Barrett ES, Bush NR, Nguyen R, Sathyanarayana S, et al. Associations between urinary biomarkers of oxidative stress in the third trimester of pregnancy and behavioral outcomes in the child at 4 years of age. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;90:272–8.
  32. 32. Ross MA. Could oxidative stress be a factor in neurodevelopmental disorders? Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2000;63(1–2):61–3. pmid:10970714
  33. 33. Horn SR, Leve LD, Levitt P, Fisher PA. Childhood adversity, mental health, and oxidative stress: A pilot study. PLoS One. 2019;14(4):e0215085. pmid:31026258
  34. 34. Milne GL, Yin H, Hardy KD, Davies SS, Roberts LJ. Isoprostane generation and function. Chem Rev. 2011;111(10):5973–96. pmid:21848345
  35. 35. Morrow JD, Harris TM, Roberts LJ. Noncyclooxygenase oxidative formation of a series of novel prostaglandins: analytical ramifications for measurement of eicosanoids. Anal Biochem. 1990;184(1):1–10. pmid:2321745
  36. 36. Milne GL, Yin H, Brooks JD, Sanchez S, Jackson Roberts L, Morrow JD. Quantification of F2-isoprostanes in biological fluids and tissues as a measure of oxidant stress. Methods Enzymol. 2007;433:113–26. pmid:17954231
  37. 37. Casetta B, Longini M, Proietti F, Perrone S, Buonocore G. Development of a fast and simple LC-MS/MS method for measuring the F2-isoprostanes in newborns. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012;25 Suppl 1:114–8. pmid:22348383
  38. 38. Friel JK, Friesen RW, Harding SV, Roberts LJ. Evidence of oxidative stress in full-term healthy infants. Pediatr Res. 2004;56(6):878–82. pmid:15470194
  39. 39. Comporti M, Signorini C, Leoncini S, Buonocore G, Rossi V, Ciccoli L. Plasma F2-isoprostanes are elevated in newborns and inversely correlated to gestational age. Free Radic Biol Med. 2004;37(5):724–32. pmid:15288129
  40. 40. Mulder EJ, Robles de Medina PG, Huizink AC, Van den Bergh BR, Buitelaar JK, Visser GH. Prenatal maternal stress: effects on pregnancy and the (unborn) child. Early Hum Dev. 2002;70(1–2):3–14.
  41. 41. Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M, Glover V. Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29(2):237–58. pmid:15811496
  42. 42. Weinstock M. Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain morphology and behaviour of the offspring. Prog Neurobiol. 2001;65(5):427–51. pmid:11689280
  43. 43. Silveira PP, Pokhvisneva I, Parent C, Cai S, Rema ASS, Broekman BFP, et al. Cumulative prenatal exposure to adversity reveals associations with a broad range of neurodevelopmental outcomes that are moderated by a novel, biologically informed polygenetic score based on the serotonin transporter solute carrier family C6, member 4 (SLC6A4) gene expression. Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(5):1601–17. pmid:29162172
  44. 44. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–6. pmid:3651732
  45. 45. Kozinszky Z, Dudas RB. Validation studies of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale for the antenatal period. J Affect Disord. 2015;176:95–105. pmid:25704562
  46. 46. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(9):606–13. pmid:11556941
  47. 47. Wang L, Kroenke K, Stump TE, Monahan PO. Screening for perinatal depression with the Patient Health Questionnaire depression scale (PHQ-9): A systematic review and meta-analysis. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2021;68:74–82. pmid:33360526
  48. 48. Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(6):1342–96. pmid:23566018
  49. 49. Ely DM, Driscoll AK. Infant Mortality in the United States, 2018: Data From the Period Linked Birth/Infant Death File. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2020;69(7):1–18. pmid:32730740
  50. 50. Gage TB, Fang F, O’Neill E, Dirienzo G. Maternal education, birth weight, and infant mortality in the United States. Demography. 2013;50(2):615–35. pmid:23073749
  51. 51. Balayla J, Azoulay L, Abenhaim HA. Maternal marital status and the risk of stillbirth and infant death: a population-based cohort study on 40 million births in the United States. Womens Health Issues. 2011;21(5):361–5. pmid:21689945
  52. 52. Valdes V, Pierce LJ, Lane CJ, Reilly EB, Jensen SKG, Gharib A, et al. An exploratory study of predictors of cognition in two low-income samples of infants across the first year of life. PLoS One. 2020;15(9):e0238507. pmid:32881915
  53. 53. Poverty Threshold, Preliminary Estimate of Weighted Average Poverty Thresholds for 2013–2018 (Internet). 2018. Available from: https://www.census.gov/data/tables/time-series/demo/income-poverty/historical-poverty-thresholds.html.
  54. 54. Targeted Populations Under Section 45D(e)(2). Federal Register; 2011. p. 75774–81.
  55. 55. Mullen EM. Mullen scales of early learning. AGS ed. Bloomington MN: Pearson Clinical Assessment; 1995. 1 canvas bag p.
  56. 56. Milne GL, Dai Q, Roberts LJ. The isoprostanes—25 years later. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2015;1851(4):433–45. pmid:25449649
  57. 57. Milne GL, Gao B, Terry ES, Zackert WE, Sanchez SC. Measurement of F2- isoprostanes and isofurans using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry. Free Radic Biol Med. 2013;59:36–44. pmid:23044261
  58. 58. Karlis G, Kotanidou A, Georgiopoulos G, Masi S, Magkas N, Xanthos T. Usefulness of F2-isoprostanes in early prognostication after cardiac arrest: a topical review of the literature and meta-analysis of preclinical data. Biomarkers. 2020;25(4):315–21. pmid:32274952
  59. 59. Black CN, Bot M, Scheffer PG, Cuijpers P, Penninx BW. Is depression associated with increased oxidative stress? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;51:164–75. pmid:25462890
  60. 60. Matthews MA, Aschner JL, Stark AR, Moore PE, Slaughter JC, Steele S, et al. Increasing F2-isoprostanes in the first month after birth predicts poor respiratory and neurodevelopmental outcomes in very preterm infants. J Perinatol. 2016;36(9):779–83. pmid:27171764
  61. 61. Franco MC, Kawamoto EM, Gorjão R, Rastelli VM, Curi R, Scavone C, et al. Biomarkers of oxidative stress and antioxidant status in children born small for gestational age: evidence of lipid peroxidation. Pediatr Res. 2007;62(2):204–8. pmid:17597662
  62. 62. Saini SM, Hoffmann CR, Pantelis C, Everall IP, Bousman CA. Systematic review and critical appraisal of child abuse measurement instruments. Psychiatry Res. 2019;272:106–13. pmid:30580133
  63. 63. Glei DA, Goldman N, Rodríguez G, Weinstein M. Beyond Self-Reports: Changes in Biomarkers as Predictors of Mortality. Popul Dev Rev. 2014;40(2):331–60. pmid:25089065
  64. 64. Del Pino S, Sánchez-Montoya SB, Guzmán JM, Mújica OJ, Gómez-Salgado J, Ruiz-Frutos C. Health Inequalities amongst People of African Descent in the Americas, 2005–2017: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(18).