Figures
Abstract
Childhood trauma and adverse childhood experiences have a strong relationship with health disparities across the lifespan. Despite experiencing approximately doubled rates of trauma, Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) are poorly characterized in deaf populations. We sought to characterize deaf-specific demographic factors and their association with multiple experiences of ACEs before the age of 18 years old. An analytical cross-sectional approach was used to ascertain associations of deaf-specific demographic factors and experiences with ACEs. The complete dataset included 520 participants for a total response rate of 56%. After adjusting for confounding effects, less severe hearing loss of 16–55 dB (2+ OR: 5.2, 4+ OR: 4.7), having a cochlear implant (2+ OR: 2.1, 4+ OR: 2.6), and not attending at least one school with signing access (2+ OR: 2.4, 4+ OR: 3.7) were significantly and independently associated with reported experiences of multiple ACEs. We conclude that factors associated with childhood hearing loss and language experiences increase risk of experiencing ACEs. Given the strong relationship between ACEs and poor social outcomes, early intervention clinical practice and health policies should consider interventions to support healthy home environments for deaf children.
Citation: Hall WC, Dye TDV, Siddiqi S (2023) Associations of childhood hearing loss and adverse childhood experiences in deaf adults. PLoS ONE 18(6): e0287024. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0287024
Editor: Ann-Sofie Sundqvist, Örebro University Faculty of Medicine and Health: Orebro universitet Fakulteten for medicin och halsa, SWEDEN
Received: September 13, 2022; Accepted: May 30, 2023; Published: June 21, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Hall et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: A consolidated dataset has been created for Open Science Framework (OSF). All data files are available from the Center for Open Science OSF data base (DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/K98PE).
Funding: This work was funded in part by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institute of Health (WCH, SS; K12GM106997), and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (WCH; UL1TR002001). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Over the past decades, childhood maltreatment and trauma have been linked to increased risk of adverse adult health outcomes as signified by the 1998 CDC-Kaiser Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) study [1]. Generally, higher incidence of ACEs is associated with increased risk in poor health behaviors, and worse mental and physical health in adulthood [2]. ACEs also have a dose effect where multiple experiences lead to worse outcomes [3]. ACE-associated consequences are experienced as early as adolescence [4] and, among others, includes increased risk of premature death [5] and allostatic changes [6], and impacts the healthy development of future generations [7]. Most recently, ACEs are described as a “major public health problem in the United States” worsening during the COVID-19 pandemic [8]. One population especially vulnerable to childhood trauma, and therefore ACEs, are children with pre-lingual hearing loss.
Rates of various trauma experiences are nearly doubled in the deaf population (see review in Johnson et al. [9]). A study of childhood abuse and neglect found 76% of deaf college students reported such experiences, and were significantly more likely than hearing college students to experience emotional and physical neglect and abuse, including sexual abuse [10]. Those who experience childhood sexual trauma are highly likely to be re-victimized in adulthood [11]. Additionally, there are unique deaf traumas such as poor communication with hearing and non-signing parents [12, 13], and “information deprivation trauma” where already-traumatic life events are intensified due to limited information access (such as natural disasters and family deaths) [14]. Unsurprisingly, the deaf population also experiences significant health disparities. Apart from well-documented language, cognitive, and education disparities, the deaf population experiences less access to language-concordant preventive services, decreased health literacy, increased use of the emergency room, and increased obesity, suicide, and interpersonal violence, among others [15–18].
The elevated prevalence of trauma and disparities occurs in a context of the majority (90–95%) of deaf children being born into hearing families that have previously never met a deaf person, and do not know a natural sign language such as American Sign Language (ASL) [19]. The mismatch between the typical spoken language of the home and the child’s inability to effortlessly access spoken language can create a neurodevelopmental emergency, given language acquisition’s foundational role in healthy brain and overall human development [20–22].
The population of children with hearing loss can be quite heterogeneous in terms of the type and severity of hearing loss, language exposure, and assistive technology use. For those with severe enough hearing loss, language development is immediately at risk if they do not receive effortless access and exposure to language in their daily environment, creating cognitive, socioemotional, and physical developmental consequences that are not fully ameliorated by hearing loss technology (such as cochlear implants) [23]. For some, this phenomenon is increasingly described as “language deprivation” [20, 24] in deaf communities, and can become severe enough to be its own mental health syndrome in adulthood [25]. Everyday communication barriers, social isolation in the home, and risk of delayed development make deaf children susceptible to both typical and unique forms of abuse and neglect. In fact, preliminary theorizing of explanatory factors underlying the general population’s adult health consequences of ACEs included language acquisition disruptions, and highlighted deaf children as a salient example where such a relationship may exist [26].
Only two known studies have explored ACEs in deaf communities. A study of 376 deaf Norwegians found an association between three ACEs–physical abuse by parent (36%), peer bullying (23%), and serious sexual abuse (30%)–and adult mental health problems (30%) [27]. Another study proposed “Adverse Childhood Communication Experiences” (ACCEs) which is poor direct and indirect childhood communication with parents and other family members. ACCEs were associated with increased risk of diabetes, hypertension, heart disease, lung disease, depression, and anxiety in a sample of 1,524 adults who reported becoming deaf before 13 years old [28].
To date, ACEs are an understudied topic in the deaf population (for instance, the largest prevalence estimate of ACEs to date used a telephone survey which excluded this population [3]). Since deaf children can experience entirely unique and complex childhood contexts (including hearing loss severity, sign language use, the influence of cochlear implants, and various educational environments, among others) relative to the general population, we sought to characterize what deaf-specific demographic factors, if any, may elucidate increased risk for experiencing ACEs before the age of 18 years old.
Methods
Study design
An analytical cross-sectional approach combining two similar studies (due to deaf communities generally being difficult to recruit large samples from) was used to ascertain associations of Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) in deaf populations. The study design included a comprehensive list of standard and deaf-specific demographic factors alongside 10 ACE questions over two phases of data collection.
Study 1 (“Adverse childhood experiences pilot project”).
The University of Rochester Research Subjects Review Board (UR RSRB) determined this study (UR RSRB STUDY00003417) met federal and university criteria for exemption with the need for written consent waived. Inclusion criteria included self-reported childhood deafness, age 18 years or older, and being able and willing to consent to participate in the study. Exclusion criteria included no hearing loss, under 18 years of age, and those who were not able to consent and participate in the study. Data collection began May 2019 and concluded January 2020. In total, 148 people identifying as d/Deaf and two as Deafblind participated.
Study 2 (“Developing a measure of deaf childhood experiences”).
The UR RSRB determined this study (UR RSRB STUDY00003417) met federal and university criteria for exemption with the need for written consent waived. Inclusion criteria included self-reported childhood deafness, born and/or raised in the USA (before age five), age 18 years or older, and being able and willing to consent to participate in the study. Exclusion criteria included no hearing loss, raised in the USA after five years old, under 18 years of age, and those who were not able to consent to participate in the study. Data collection began April 2020 and concluded July 2020. In total, 355 people identifying as d/Deaf, and 115 as Deafblind participated.
Recruitment
Community recruitment for both studies occurred on social media platforms commonly used by deaf people (Facebook and Twitter), person-to-person networking, and through community organizations. Two of the authors (WCH and SS) are deaf and have community networks that were used for recruitment as well. Participants were routed to online REDCap surveys for participation.
Study variables
General and deaf-specific demographic factors, and self-reported ACEs (recategorized as having experience 0 or 1, 2+, and 4+) were the study variables. General demographics included age, race and ethnicity, household income, education attainment, employment status, sexual orientation, and gender. Deaf-specific demographics included self-reported hearing loss status (slight, mild, moderate, moderately-severe, severe, profound), hearing loss onset (birth, before 3, 3–5, 5+), parent hearing status (at least one deaf parent, hearing), education setting (at least one oral-only setting, at least one setting with signing access), cochlear implant status (yes, no), cochlear implant surgery age (0-11m, 1-1y11m, 2-2y11m, 3-3y11m, 4-4y11m, 5+ years), current use of the cochlear implant (never, sometimes, most of the time, all the time), and signing exposure before five years old (yes, no). Survey questions sought to capture granular complexity of certain demographic factors for other project needs. For the purposes of these analyses, however, demographic variables were collapsed into simplified logical constructs where possible and appropriate. The study language was written English and both studies ascertained variables in the same manner. IRB approval was obtained to combine datasets and remove duplicates (i.e., participants who participated in both studies).
Data cleaning and quality.
Data quality was addressed through implementing required responses, skip patterns, and range checking in implementation of the REDCap surveys. As a result, question-specific missing data rates were minimal (generally <1%) and out-of-range values were prevented. Several demographic variables (sexual orientation and race, for example) were collected as open-ended responses to prioritize respondent self-classification and those responses were subsequently recoded into discrete categories. Cronbach’s alpha for the ACEs scale used in this study was 0.734, which met or exceeded Cronbach’s alpha levels for other ACEs studies [29].
Analysis
The main study outcome was self-reports of experiencing two or more (2+) ACEs [27], and four or more (4+) ACEs [2]. Hosmer and Lameshow’s approach to logistic regression [30] was used by crosstabulating demographic variables with the main outcomes to generate bivariate associations. Any variables demonstrating at least marginal statistical association with the main outcome (p<0.10) were included in the subsequent multivariate model. All marginally-significant demographic variables were subsequently entered into a forward-stepwise logistic multivariate regression, with any significant variables (p<0.05) remaining in the final model after controlling for other variables. The Goodness-of-Fit statistic evaluated how well the data was fit. SPSS v28 (IBM Corporation) was used for all analyses.
Results
A total of 926 respondents across both studies initiated the survey on REDCap. 520 participants formed the complete dataset after filtering incomplete responses (190), duplicates (29, via email address), and those who did not self-identify as deaf, Deaf, or Deafblind (187) for a total response rate of 56%. In terms of standard demographics, the participant sample was primarily aged 25–34 years (58%), white (77.8%), straight (76.1%), female (53.2%), and earning $35,000 to $74,999 (combined 59.1%) with a college degree/some graduate school (33.1%) while being employed full-time (60.3%). For deaf-specific demographics, the participant sample reported hearing loss onset mainly at birth (34.3%), identifying as Deaf (62.2%), having hearing parent(s)/guardian(s) (71.5%), experiencing at least one oral-only setting (54.5%), one education placement (61.7%), being exposed to some type of signing before five years old (59.5%) and having a cochlear implant (50.9%). Of those who identified having a cochlear implant, most had the surgery at five years or older (43%), and currently use the implant either sometimes or most of the time (combined 82%).
Bivariate associations of demographic variables with the presence of 2+ and 4+ ACEs are presented in Table 1. For general demographic factors, only age under 35 years was significantly associated with both 2+ and 4+ ACEs, and race other than white was marginally-significantly associated. No other general demographic variables were significantly associated. In contrast, multiple deaf-related demographic factors were significantly associated with both 2+ and 4+ ACEs including: (1) slight-to-moderate hearing loss, (2) not attending at least one school with signing access, and (3) having a cochlear implant.
Table 2 presents the results of the multivariate model. Participants with slight-to-moderate hearing loss, who did not attend at least one school with signing access, and who had received a cochlear implant were all significantly more likely to report 2+ and 4+ ACEs after adjusting for confounding effects. Participants who self-identified as Deafblind were more likely to report 2+ ACEs while those with hearing loss onset after the age of five years old were more likely to report 4+ ACEs. The Pearson Goodness-of-Fit statistic was not significant (not shown), indicating the data fit the model well.
Discussion
This study is the first-ever characterization of deaf-specific demographic factors that may increase risk of reporting Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs). Previous work has indicated that experiencing two or more (2+) ACEs was significantly associated with reported mental health issues for deaf individuals [27], and four or more (4+) ACEs was considered high risk for toxic stress physiology in the general population [2]. Several factors remained significant predictors of reporting both 2+ and 4+ ACEs in our multivariate models, even after controlling for the confounding effects of other variables. These factors were (1) less severe hearing loss (16–55 dB), (2) having a cochlear implant, and (3) not having attended at least one school with signing access.
Detecting less severe hearing loss as a significant factor in our multivariate models was unexpected, as much of the literature for deaf trauma is centered around profoundly deaf individuals and signing communities without considering the circumstances around hearing ability. It is unclear what this association may mean for deaf vulnerability to trauma (other than the already-acknowleged heightened vulnerability that deaf children experience). For example, a study asking mental health providers who work with deaf communities about protective factors against trauma highlights access to information, language, and communication, but does not consider hearing loss severity and the use of technology [9]. The same is true for other highly-cited studies that have discussed trauma in deaf communities [10, 11, 14].
Concurrently, the literature on cochlear implants and subsequent socioemotional development generally focuses on studies where adults report about a deaf child (e.g., parents, teachers, and other individuals) rather than asking the deaf child directly [31–34]. While understanding adult perspectives about deaf children with whom they interact is important–when primary reports do not derive from the deaf child personally, negative experiences could be underreported or overlooked. Additionally, 43% of cochlear implant recipients in our sample had the implant inserted after five years old–relatively late for neurolinguistic development. Now, children more commonly experience cochlear implant surgery as early as one year of age (or earlier). It is unclear if implantation age may have an impact on the relationship with ACEs (for instance if earlier implantation led to cochlear implant outcomes that better aligned with family expectations) and deserves further investigation. Determining whether or not cochlear implants are protective in deaf childhood trauma is unclear; our study suggests this is an important area to investigate further.
In contrast, the controversy of signed language use in early child development and education settings for deaf children is strongly documented and a long-running thread in deaf communities’ discourse and advocacy stretching back at least several centuries [21, 35]. This discouragement of signed language use by medical and education systems has led to a landscape where many deaf children are placed in public school settings (and specialized schools) that do not include signed languages and visual communication strategies. Our study reflects this experience since a majority of our participants have experienced an oral-only school environment. Importantly, “signing access” was defined very broadly within the analyses as long as some signing was present in their school environment. This included any kind of access such as signing communication systems (such as Signed Exact English), and a full range of settings from having just an interpreter in a public school setting to attending a fully immersive residential school using American Sign Language. That not attending a school with some sort of signing access was a risk factor suggests that visual languages and communication may have a protective role for reducing risk of ACEs.
Taken as a whole, these predictive factors related to hearing status and school settings may indicate pressure to “pass for hearing” [36–38] in deaf children who–either individually or through some combination–have some usable residual hearing, who acquired some usable hearing through the cochlear implant in the hearing family home, and/or who were not placed in a signing educational environment. The majority of medical and educational interventions for deaf children are typically centered around restoring as much hearing as possible, discouraging sign language use, and ensuring as much English fluency and speech clarity as possible [39]. Such scenarios can also reflect a complex picture that may include parent decision-making values, their attitudes about hearing loss and disability, the types of early intervention services families receive and types of local educational settings that are available, socioeconomic status, and much more. What this overall picture may mean exactly in terms of heightened vulnerability to ACEs requires and deserves more investigation in future work.
Because hearing loss can only ever be remediated to the functional limits of hearing loss technology (hearing aids and cochlear implants) [24], deaf and hard of hearing children still experience the impact of hearing loss and many may fail to “pass for hearing.” Indeed, in our sample, even with 50% having cochlear implants and 54.5% experiencing a school setting without sign language access, a majority (62.2%) still identified with a culturally Deaf label that signifies membership and participation in signing deaf communities. Over time, continual failure to “pass for hearing” as deaf children continue to develop throughout their childhood may lead to accumulating and ongoing strains in family and social dynamics.
There is also the well-known context of the extra family stress that can occur when a child has a disability, and specifically hearing loss [40]. Generally, parents with children who have cochlear implants experience more stress than those with non-disabled children [41]. The experience of a dual diagnosis for Deafblind children can create additional pressure for families beyond typical stress of a disability diagnosis where parents especially need support for their mental health, advocacy skills, and learning how to parent a Deafblind child [42]. Overall, individual and societial expectations from and for both the hearing family and the deaf child to be “normal” may lead to a home environment that is more conducive to stress and negative experiences, contributing to trauma experiences of deaf people.
Our study has limitations, and caution should be taken in interpretation and generalizability of these results. The online survey was in written English, a second language for many members of the deaf population; additionally, language fluency in ASL and English was not directly assessed. The retrospective nature of asking adults about ACEs may introduce recall bias. Online surveys carry risk of false responses, multiple responses by the same person, and skewed data based on recruitment channels. The sample was predominantly white, preventing analyses exploring additional racial and ethnic intersectionalities. Furthermore, those with unilateral hearing loss is a population of interest that should be included in future work. Our sample demographics are heavily educated, possibly a function of the research team being situated in Rochester, NY where there is a large, highly-educated culturally Deaf population.
More attention should be given toward deaf children with multiple disabilities and hard-of-hearing children who are frequently “stuck in the middle” as they have usable (albeit limited) hearing, often appearing to be “hearing” with their listening and speaking abilities. Likewise, those with other marginalized identities (such as sexual orientation) need support as well. For families that have a child experience hearing loss several years after birth who are already developing spoken language, there should be sufficient support and counseling to address what may be a sudden and drastic developmental change with their child. Additionally, more consideration is needed as to what role the cochlear implant plays in the family home and how it may alter family dynamics, expectations, and interactions with the deaf child. Overall, it is important that future investigations explore the intersectionalities of deaf individuals with multiple marginalized identities and how this may influence the risk for ACEs.
Public health implications
Overall, there is emerging attention on the relationship between childhood experiences and adult health outcomes in the deaf population. This interest includes increasing community and academic dialogue about language deprivation and its downstream effects across the lifespan [13, 20, 25], which this study appears to indicate also includes a heightened risk of ACEs. Such heightened risk aligns with calls for bimodal bilingual education as a public health measure [43] to improve deaf population health. Given the strong relationship between adverse childhood experiences and poor health and social outcomes across the lifespan, changes in clinical practice and health policies in early intervention, and education systems, and the medical home should be considered that better supports healthy and safe home environments for deaf children.
References
- 1. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;14(4):245–58. pmid:9635069
- 2. Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, Sethi D, Butchart A, Mikton C, et al. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2017;2(8):e356–e66. pmid:29253477
- 3. Merrick MT, Ford DC, Ports KA, Guinn AS. Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences from the 2011–2014 behavioral risk factor surveillance system in 23 states. JAMA Pediatrics. 2018;172(11):1038–44. pmid:30242348
- 4. Flaherty EG, Thompson R, Dubowitz H, Harvey EM, English DJ, Proctor LJ, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and child health in early adolescence. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(7):622–9. pmid:23645114
- 5. Brown DW, Anda RF, Tiemeier H, Felitti VJ, Edwards VJ, Croft JB, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of premature mortality. Am J Prev Med. 2009;37(5):389–96. pmid:19840693
- 6. Danese A, McEwen BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiol Behav. 2012;106(1):29–39. pmid:21888923
- 7. Sun J, Patel F, Rose-Jacobs R, Frank DA, Black MM, Chilton M. Mothers’ Adverse Childhood Experiences and Their Young Children’s Development. Am J Prev Med. 2017;53(6):882–91. pmid:28919342
- 8. Bryant DJ, Oo M, Damian AJ. The rise of adverse childhood experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol Trauma. 2020;12(S1):S193–S4. pmid:32551773
- 9. Johnson P, Cawthon S, Fink B, Wendel E, Schoffstall S. Trauma and Resilience Among Deaf Individuals. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2018;23(4):317–30. pmid:30053140
- 10. Schenkel LS, Rothman-Marshall G, Schlehofer DA, Towne TL, Burnash DL, Priddy BM. Child maltreatment and trauma exposure among deaf and hard of hearing young adults. Child Abuse Negl. 2014;38(10):1581–9. pmid:24862922
- 11. Schild S, Dalenberg CJ. Consequences of child and adult sexual and physical trauma among deaf adults. J Agg Mal Trau. 2015(24):237–56.
- 12. Anderson ML, Wolf Craig KS, Hall WC, Ziedonis DM. A pilot study of deaf trauma survivors’ experiences: Early traumas unique to being deaf in a hearing world. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma. 2016;9(4):353–8.
- 13. Hall WC, Li D, Dye TDV. Influence of hearing loss on child behavioral and home experiences. American Journal of Public Health. 2018;108(8):1079–81. pmid:29927649
- 14. Schild S, Dalenberg CJ. Trauma exposure and traumatic symptoms in deaf adults. Psychol Trauma. 2012;4(1):117–27.
- 15. Barnett S, Klein JD, Pollard RQ, Samar V, Schlehofer D, Starr M, et al. Community participatory research with deaf sign language users to identify health inequities. American Journal of Public Health. 2011;101(12):2235–8. pmid:22021296
- 16. McKee M, Barnett SL, Block RC, Pearson TA. Impact of communication on preventive services among deaf American Sign Language users. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2011;41(1):75–9. pmid:21665066
- 17. Smith SR, Samar VJ. Dimensions of deaf/hard-of-hearing and hearing adolescents’ health literacy and health knowledge. Journal of Health Communication. 2016;21:141–54. pmid:27548284
- 18. McKee MM, Winters PC, Sen A, Zazove P, Fiscella K. Emergency Department utilization among Deaf American Sign Language users. Disabil Health J. 2015;8(4):573–8. pmid:26166160
- 19. Mitchell RE, Karchmer MA. Parental hearing status and signing among deaf and hard of hearing students. Sign Language Studies. 2005;5(2):231–44.
- 20. Hall WC. What you don’t know can hurt you: The risk of language deprivation by impairing sign language development in deaf children. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2017;21(5):961–5. pmid:28185206
- 21. Cheng Q, Roth A, Halgren E, Mayberry RI. Effects of Early Language Deprivation on Brain Connectivity: Language Pathways in Deaf Native and Late First-Language Learners of American Sign Language. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:320. pmid:31607879
- 22. Penicaud S, Klein D, Zatorre RJ, Chen JK, Witcher P, Hyde K, et al. Structural brain changes linked to delayed first language acquisition in congenitally deaf individuals. Neuroimage. 2013;66:42–9. pmid:23063844
- 23. Geers AE, Mitchell CM, Warner-Czyz A, Wang NY, Eisenberg LS, CDACI Investigative Team. Early sign language exposure and cochlear implantation benefits. Pediatrics. 2017;140(1):e20163489.
- 24. Hall ML, Hall WC, Caselli NK. Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Language. 2019.
- 25. Hall WC, Levin LL, Anderson ML. Language deprivation syndrome: A possible neurodevelopmental disorder with sociocultural origins. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2017;52(6):761–76. pmid:28204923
- 26. Salomon MJS, Wagner SH. What explains the negative consequences of adverse childhood experiences on adult health? Insights from cognitive and neuroscience research. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):356–60. pmid:9635084
- 27. Kvam MH, Loeb M. The relation between adverse childhood experiences and later mental health among deaf adults. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research. 2010;12(4):233–44.
- 28. Kushalnagar P, Ryan C, Paludneviciene R, Spellun A, Gulati S. Adverse Childhood Communication Experiences Associated With an Increased Risk of Chronic Diseases in Adults Who Are Deaf. Am J Prev Med. 2020;59(4):548–54. pmid:32636047
- 29.
Holden G, Gower T, Chmielewski M. Methodological considerations in ACEs research. Adverse Childhood Experiences: Academic Press; 2020. p. 161–82.
- 30.
Hosmer DW Jr, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression: John Wiley & Sons; 2004.
- 31. Huttunen K, Valimaa T. Communication and linguistic and socioemotional development after cochlear implantation. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2010;15(4):383–404.
- 32. Wiefferink CH, Rieffe C, Ketelaar L, Frijns JHM. Predicting social functioning in children with a cochlear implant and in normal-hearing children: The role of emotion regulation. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2012;76(6):883–9. pmid:22459035
- 33. Percy-Smith L, Caye-Thomasen P, Gudman M, Jensen J, Thomsen J. Self-esteem and social well-being of children with cochlear implant compared to normal-hearing children. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2008;72(7):1113–20. pmid:18501436
- 34. Bat-Chava Y, Martin D, Kosciw JG. Longitudinal improvements in communication and socialization with cochlear implants and hearing aids: Evidence from parental reports. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46(12):1287–96.
- 35. Robinson OE, Henner J. The personal is political in the The Deaf Mute Howls: Deaf epistemology seeks disability justice. Disability & Society. 2017;32(9):1416–36.
- 36.
Harmon K. Compulsory hearing and ability trouble. In: Emens EF, editor. Disability and Equality Law: Taylor & Francis; 2017. p. 273–92.
- 37. Davis LJ. Deafness and the riddle of identity. The Chronicle of Higher Education. 2007;1(12):1–7.
- 38.
McAdams D. Beyond category: The complexities of deaf-hearing identity. In: Leigh I, editor. A Lens on Deaf Identities: Oxford University Press; 2009. p. 43–63.
- 39. Humphries T, Kushalnagar P, Mathur G, Napoli DJ, Padden C, Rathmann C, et al. Language acquisition for deaf children: Reducing the harms of zero tolerance to the use of alternative approaches. Harm Reduct J. 2012;9:16. pmid:22472091
- 40. Hintermair M. Parental resources, parental stress, and socioemotional development of deaf and hard of hearing children. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2006;11(4):493–513. pmid:16809431
- 41. Sarant J, Garrard P. Parenting stress in parents of children with cochlear implants: Relationships among parent stress, child language, and unilateral versus bilateral implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2014;19(1):85–106. pmid:23813672
- 42. Correa-Torres SM, Bowen SK. Recognizing the needs of families of children and youth who are deafblind. American Annals of the Deaf. 2016;161(4):454–61. pmid:27818401
- 43. Wilkinson E, Morford JP. How bilingualism contributes to healthy development in deaf children: A public health perspective. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2020;24:1330–8. pmid:32632844