Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

Genetic association study of dyslexia and ADHD candidate genes in a Spanish cohort: Implications of comorbid samples

  • Mirian Sánchez-Morán,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations BCBL-Basque Center on Cognition Brain and Language, Donostia-San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain, CIC bioGUNE, Derio, Bizkaia, Spain

  • Juan Andrés Hernández,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology

    Affiliation Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Spain

  • Jon Andoni Duñabeitia,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation BCBL-Basque Center on Cognition Brain and Language, Donostia-San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain

  • Adelina Estévez,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Spain

  • Laura Bárcena,

    Roles Formal analysis

    Affiliation CIC bioGUNE, Derio, Bizkaia, Spain

  • Aintzane González-Lahera,

    Roles Formal analysis

    Affiliations CIC bioGUNE, Derio, Bizkaia, Spain, CIBERehd, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain

  • María Teresa Bajo,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Research Center for Brain, Mind & Behavior, Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain

  • Luis J. Fuentes,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain

  • Ana M. Aransay ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Ana M. Aransay, Manuel Carreiras

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors are co-senior authors on this work.

    Affiliations CIC bioGUNE, Derio, Bizkaia, Spain, CIBERehd, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain

  • Manuel Carreiras

    Contributed equally to this work with: Ana M. Aransay, Manuel Carreiras

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    m.carreiras@bcbl.eu

    ‡ These authors are co-senior authors on this work.

    Affiliations BCBL-Basque Center on Cognition Brain and Language, Donostia-San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain, Ikerbasque, Basque Foundation for Science, Bilbao, Spain, Universidad del Pais Vasco UPV/EHU, Leioa, Bizkaia, Spain

Correction

19 Dec 2018: Sánchez-Morán M, Hernández JA, Duñabeitia JA, Estévez A, Bárcena L, et al. (2018) Correction: Genetic association study of dyslexia and ADHD candidate genes in a Spanish cohort: Implications of comorbid samples. PLOS ONE 13(12): e0209718. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209718 View correction

Abstract

Dyslexia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are two complex neuro-behaviorally disorders that co-occur more often than expected, so that reading disability has been linked to inattention symptoms. We examined 4 SNPs located on genes previously associated to dyslexia (KIAA0319, DCDC2, DYX1C1 and FOXP2) and 3 SNPs within genes related to ADHD (COMT, MAOA and DBH) in a cohort of Spanish children (N = 2078) that met the criteria of having one, both or none of these disorders (dyslexia and ADHD). We used a case-control approach comparing different groups of samples based on each individual diagnosis. In addition, we also performed a quantitative trait analysis with psychometric measures on the general population (N = 3357). The results indicated that the significance values for some markers change depending on the phenotypic groups compared and/or when considering pair-wise marker interactions. Furthermore, our quantitative trait study showed significant genetic associations with specific cognitive processes. These outcomes advocate the importance of establishing rigorous and homogeneous criteria for the diagnosis of cognitive disorders, as well as the relevance of considering cognitive endophenotypes.

Introduction

Dyslexia [MIM: 127700] and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD [MIM:143465]) are two neurobehavioral disorders with high prevalence [1]. Both are considered complex disorders influenced by multiple genetic and environmental risk factors [1,2] and it is thought that many genes are implicated in their development, each one contributing with a small effect on the total phenotypic variance. Dyslexia is characterized by difficulties in learning to read despite normal intelligence, educational opportunities or physical abilities (correct vision and audition). On the other hand, ADHD is described with symptoms such as excessive motor activity, impulsiveness and inattention [3]. ADHD is frequently divided into three subtypes depending on the predominant symptoms: the inattentive type, the hyperactive-impulsive type or the combined type [3]. In this study, we have only focused on children with inattentive symptoms (i.e. attention deficits).

Nine loci (DYX1-DYX9) [4] have been identified as candidates for susceptibility to dyslexia, which contain a number of genes potentially related to this disorder: KIAA0319 (6p22.3) [MIM: 609269] [5,6], DCDC2 (6p22.1) [MIM: 605755] [68], DYX1C1 (15q21.3) [MIM: 608706] [9,10], ROBO1(3p12)[MIM: 602430] [11], FOXP2 (7q31) [MIM: 605317] [12]. For ADHD, molecular studies have concentrated on genes encoding proteins involved in the dopaminergic pathway, with special interest in the dopamine receptor DRD4 (11p15.5) [MIM: 126452] [13] and dopamine transporter DAT1 (or SLC6A3, 5p15.3) [MIM: 126455] [14]. Other genes such as DBH (9q34) [MIM: 609312] [15], COMT (22q11.21) [MIM: 116790] [16] and MAOA (Xp11.3) [MIM: 309850] [17] have also been examined because of their roles in the dopaminergic system, but have not been studied extensively. Importantly, despite de fact that there have been many genetic studies of dyslexia and ADHD, conclusive results linking the two conditions/disorders is lacking.

As reported, dyslexia and ADHD appear together more often than expected [18] and some studies have shown a stronger relationship between dyslexia and symptoms of inattention [1922] rather than hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms. Twin and family studies suggest that this overlap is, partly, due to shared genetic background [21,23,24]. Indeed, several studies have demonstrated intersecting genetic regions for these syndromes [25,26]. The search for dyslexia or ADHD-specific susceptibility genes is difficult not only because of the complexity and heterogeneity of these conditions, but also due to their co-occurrence (also known as comorbidity) with other cognitive disorders, which makes it even more difficult to obtain conclusive results, particularly if the other disorders are not diagnosed. When comorbidity is ignored, it can be erroneously concluded that a particular variable is associated with a given disorder, when in fact it is interacting with the comorbid condition [27]. Importantly, the results of genetic analyses can only be as good as the symptomatology criteria of the studied phenotype. Therefore, it is of vital importance to correctly establish the diagnostic criteria applied to the cohorts in order to find meaningful associations [28]. In the study of dyslexia candidate genes in cases of ADHD (or the other way around, ADHD candidate genes in cases of dyslexia), these are fundamental considerations.

The aim of this study was to evaluate the reproducibility of association between reported SNPs in dyslexia and ADHD in our Spanish cohort, examining the relationship when comorbid individuals are included or excluded in the case population. In addition to each single marker approach, we have investigated the effect of composite genotypes using pairs of SNPs, to assess the epistatic relation of those markers with these cognition disorders.

Nonetheless, the diagnostic of all-or-none, when assessing whether the disorder is present or not, may not be optimal for many genetic studies, as this type of characterization does not capture the complete essence of the phenotype [1]. Both dyslexia and ADHD might arise from many different cognitive processes and, consequently, it has been suggested that identifying susceptibility genes for endophenotypes may prove a very fruitful strategy [2,29]. Many of these cognitive functions seem to be continuously distributed in the general population. Therefore, in addition to searching for genetic differences between cases and controls, we also investigated the correlation of the output genotypes and phenotypes of all participants (including the extreme ones), since direct analysis of continuous indices of severity or cognitive traits may allow discovery of genes related to the specific cognitive processes underlying each disorder [30,31].

Materials and methods

1.-Sample collection and DNA extraction

A total of 4678 saliva samples were collected from children at schools all over Spain from different regions (Andalucía, Basque Country, Castilla-León, Murcia, and Canary Islands), after informed consent was obtained from their tutors or parents. La-Laguna University Ethical Committee approved consent by tutors or parents. Parents were informed and provided the opportunity to opt-out in cases in which consent was obtained from tutors. A saliva sample was collected from each participant with Oragene saliva kits (OG-500, DNA Genotek Inc., Canada) and corresponding DNA was extracted following manufacturer’s instructions, quantified and qualified on 0.8%Agarose-1xTAE-gels. Only participants with Spanish origins were used for subsequent analyses. The Spanish origin was assessed by principal component analysis (PCA) using samples from another research study diagnosed with the same criteria (a total of 1500 samples that overlapped with this study samples). A genome-wide genotyping was performed (638592 SNPs along the genome) and population stratification was analyzed by PCA. The results showed no population stratification. See S1 Fig. for further details.

2.-Diagnostic criteria

Dyslexics and controls were selected by using a discriminant function created with an a priori group diagnosed with dyslexia (n = 43) and another a priori group of controls taking into account their performance (one standard deviation above the mean) in a text-comprehension test (n = 470). We used a text-comprehension test to define the control group because it involves decoding and reading to avoid circularity when applying the discriminant function to word and pseudoword reading. The discriminant function successful separated the dyslexics and the controls: Wilks’ (lambda) = 0.60, F (1,497) = 329.9, p<0.0001. The variables of the discriminant function that classified dyslexics and controls with a sensibility of 91% and specificity of 94% were: age, efficiency in reading words and pseudowods, rapid naming of pictures and colors (RAN), reaction time in phoneme picture matching for phonological awareness (PA), accuracy in letter position identification, reaction time in syllable identification (see S1 File for a description of the tasks and S8 Table for mean and SD values). Table 1 shows the standardized and the structure coefficients corresponding to these variables. We selected as dyslexics the participants with an IQ (intelligence quotient) above 80 that fell in the deciles 1 or 2 of the discriminant function, and as controls the participants that fell in the decile 5 or higher.

thumbnail
Table 1. Standardized and structure coefficients for the variables that entered the discriminant function.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.t001

Participants were classified as ADHD taking into account their error rates and reaction times in three tasks (Verbal-Stroop, Numerical-Stroop and Attentional Network Task (ANT)) (see S1 File for a description of the tasks and S9 Table for mean and SD values). Specifically, they were classified as ADHD if they had an IQ above 80 and fell in the quartile 4 on reaction times (slow responses) or in the quartile 1 in error rates (high error rates) in the three tasks. They were classified as controls when they fell in quartiles 1, 2 or 3 on reaction times. Finally, the comorbids were defined as those individuals with developmental dyslexia and ADHD (i.e. also tested and falling into both of the criteria-based categories as described above) (see S10 Table for mean and SD values).

3.-SNP genotyping

The DNA extracted from the collected saliva samples was characterized for seven SNP markers by Taqman SNP Genotyping assays resolved in a ViiA7 Real-Time-PCR System (Thermo-Fisher-Scientific Inc., Massachusetts, USA). The studied SNP-IDs are: rs57809907-DYX1C1 (custom design), rs6323-MAOA (custom design), rs1611115-DBH (C_2535786_10), rs2274305-DCDC2 (C_9344981_1_), rs4504469-KIAA0319 (C_390135_10), rs12533005-FOXP2 (C_220195_10) and rs4680-COMT1 (C_25746809_50). Reactions were performed according to manufacturer’s instructions.

4.-Statistical analyses

Resulting genotypes were tested for Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium with a χ2 goodness-of-fit test. Single-nucleotide-polymorphisms association analyses for genotypic, allelic, dominant and recessive models and pair-wise SNPs epistasis scrutiny in case-control strategy were carried out with PLINK (http://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/plink/) [32] against the null hypothesis of “no association”. The samples were classified in 6 groups (Fig 1) based on their phenotype, and 7 different contrasts were performed. All the analyses were implemented in the whole cohort as well as separated by gender.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Total number of samples diagnosed for each cognitive disorder studied.

a) Dys, dyslexia; ADHD, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; Ctr_Dys, dyslexia controls; Ctr_ADHD, ADHD controls; Com, Comorbids; Ctr_Com, comorbid controls. The number of females (F) and males (M) are detailed in parentheses (FFF/MMM) for each category.b) Total number of individuals in our population and number of individuals used for the study. *Note that for the Dyslexia Controls and ADHD controls we have only used samples without any disorder diagnosed. The 40 samples with ADHD have been excluded from the Ctr_Dys group and the 78 dyslexic samples have been excluded from the Ctr_adhd group for the analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.g001

For the quantitative trait loci (QTL) approach, the genotypes of the candidate markers were analyzed for reading and attention measures by evaluating the variance with age as covariate (ANCOVA) and Hochberg false discovery rate correction [33]. Children with IQ lower than 80 were excluded from the analyses. Related to reading and attention, the variables examined were word reading (high and low frequency words), pseudoword reading, PA, RAN, syllable discrimination, Verbal-Stroop, Numerical-Stroop, and ANT (S1 File).

Results

Parameters of the population studied

A total of 4678 saliva samples were collected from children at schools all over Spain. Out of these, 3357 samples (1664 females, 1693 males) with ages between 6–16 years fulfilled the established criteria and, therefore, were used in the present study. Fig 1 shows the distribution of individuals diagnosed with dyslexia (Dys), ADHD or characterized as controls (Ctr_Dys, Dyslexia Controls; Ctr_ADHD, ADHD controls). Note that each sample could meet the criteria for more than one phenotype. Particularly, we considered as comorbids (Com) 45 participants who were classified both as dyslexic and ADHD, and we identified 106 comorbid-controls (Ctr_Com) who shared the criteria for controls of both disorders.

Different marker associations found depending on the inclusion/exclusion of the comorbid samples within the groups compared

SNP genotyping raw data for each comparison can be downloaded from the S1 Dataset as *.ped and *.map files. The frequencies of the analyzed SNPs in our population and in the general European population can be found in S4, S5, S6 and S7 Tables and single marker allelic associations are summarized in Table 2. Note that most of the described associations do not pass the multiple testing correction. It is worth mentioning that the statistical significance varied notably when comorbids were included or not in the case group. Interestingly, markers within KIAA0319 and FOXP2 were significantly related to dyslexia when comorbid samples were included (Dys+Com-vs-Ctr_Dys), as well as when comorbids were analyzed as an independent group (Com-vs-Ctr_Dys/Com-vs-Ctr_ADHD/Com-vs-Ctr_Com).

thumbnail
Table 2. Genetic association results (p values) for single-markers (allelic model).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.t002

Further associations were identified when considering paired-SNPs as an epistatic variant (Table 3). The composite genotype of DCDC2-DYX1C1 SNPs presented significant association with ADHD (ADHD-vs-Ctr_ADHD), with lower p-values when comorbid samples were considered jointly (ADHD+Com-vs-Ctr_ADHD), and also in the Com-vs-Ctr_ADHD test. In contrast, the combination DCDC2-KIAA0319 correlated with dyslexia independently of the inclusion/exclusion of comorbid samples. Additionally, rs4680-COMT showed significant association in the genotypic comparatives Dys+Com-vs-Ctr_Dys and Com-vs-Ctr_Dys (p-values = 0.019/0.018, respectively, S1 Table). However, when filtering these data by gender (S2 and S3 Tables), rs4680 does not appear significantly linked, although for males (S3 Table), some trend of association (p<0.1) was observed at allelic, dominant and recessive models.

thumbnail
Table 3. Genetic association results (p values) for pair-wise SNPs interactions (allelic model).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.t003

For the analysis of rs6323 located in MAOA at chromosome X, only female samples were considered. The results showed that rs6323 was significantly associated at the recessive comparative ADHD+Com-vs-Ctr_ADHD (p-value = 0.022, S1 Table). The recessive tests within the comorbid comparatives were not performed for rs6323 because the sample size of the compared groups was too small.

Comorbids exhibited extreme discriminant values compared to dyslexic and ADHD samples

In order to understand the effect of comorbids in the analyses performed, we plotted the discriminant function values to the studied samples, which resulted in the distribution of phenotypes shown in Fig 2. According to this scattering, the comorbid group showed extreme discriminant values. The samples situated at the opposite side of the graph correspond to the controls, while the dyslexics and ADHD fall in between.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Distribution of the values of the discriminant function for each phenotypic group.

Each histogram was assessed through a kernel density estimation. The comorbid samples were considered as a single group, and therefore, excluded from the dyslexia and ADHD cohorts. The vertical color bars of the upper and lower borders represent the samples of each group (blue = comorbids, green = ADHD samples, red = dyslexics and black = controls). Dashed vertical lines represent the average values of each distribution (blue = comorbids, green = ADHD samples, red = dyslexics and black = controls).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.g002

Dyslexia and ADHD candidate SNPs are associated with cognitive traits in the general Spanish population

For QTL analyses, the genotypes and values of variables from the total population (N = 3357) were analyzed (see Table 4). Remarkably, some SNPs showed associations with tasks related either to dyslexia or to ADHD, while others showed associations with tasks related to both disorders. The four SNPs previously related to dyslexia showed very different associations. rs2274305-DCDC2 was coherently associated with PA and RAN. Similarly, DYX1C1 was also accordingly associated with word-reading and RAN, but also with the performance in the Verbal and Numerical-Stroop tasks that involve attentional processes. Interestingly, rs12533005-FOXP2, described in dyslexia studies, was related to the performance in the Verbal and Numerical-Stroop tasks. Finally, rs4504469-KIAA0319 was related to word reading, RAN, syllable discrimination and Numerical Stroop.

thumbnail
Table 4. Genetic association of the analyzed SNPs to the variables measuring cognitive processes related to reading and attention.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.t004

The analyses of the three SNPs previously related to ADHD also showed very different outcomes. While rs4680-COMT1 was associated with the ANT tasks, the rs6323-MAOA, a marker for ADHD, showed association not only with Numerical-Stroop but also with word and pseudoword reading time, PA, RAN and syllable discrimination. Finally, rs1611115-DBH, another marker for ADHD, was related to PA and syllable discrimination.

Discussion

The present reading/attentional candidate gene association study based on case-control status and QTL approach shows that variants of selected genes are related to the phenotype and highlights the importance of accurate characterization of phenotypes. In addition, the study shows genetic associations to cognition by analyzing the relationship of specific SNPs with cognitive quantitative measures such as endophenotypes in a large sample of individuals.

In particular, we replicated previous associations for KIAA0319 and FOXP2 variants in dyslexic samples, with the relevant finding that higher significance values were obtained for those SNPs when comorbid samples were included. This observation is concordant with the fact that individuals who suffer from both disorders present a more severe manifestation of the symptoms as shown in previous studies [20,34] and in Fig 2. According to Wilcutt et al.[20] the dyslexia-ADHD comorbid condition shows a more extensive and severe profile of neuropsychological weakness. Furthermore, the heritability of reading disabilities was significantly higher in dyslexics if the individuals also met criteria for ADHD according to Willcutt et al.[20], which could support our data of a more significant p value when comorbids are considered. Importantly, Scerri et al. [7] signaled the importance of not excluding comorbid samples in association analyses, based on the statistical significance that was observed for DCDC2 markers in dyslexic individuals, but only when comorbid samples were considered.

Interestingly, DYX1C1and DCDC2 SNPs, both candidates for the susceptibility of developing dyslexia, did not show any individual association with this disorder. The lack of relationship with DYX1C1 is not unexpected, since the significant results found in a Finnish population study of Taipale et al. [9] in have not been replicated in subsequent reports [10,35]. Either different causal variants or singular linkage disequilibrium patterns may exist within this gene in each population [36], or its effect may only be present under environmental disadvantage [37]. In addition, the genetic relation of the DCDC2 variants within the dyslexia phenotype has been found to differ across the subgroup classification of the disorder [38]. Notably, we found significant effects when considering the composite genotype of SNPs within KIAA0319 and DCDC2 (Table 3), which have been previously reported [6,3942]. Although these two genes are localized in the same chromosome, the lack of linkage disequilibrium between them in the studied cohort points to an independent but cooperative genetic association of these genes with dyslexia. Furthermore, the combined effect of these two genes had been described previously by Powers et al.’s [43], who showed that a DCDC2 risk haplotype interacts synergistically with a KIAA0319 haplotype, giving more extreme results when both risk haplotypes where presented together rather than separately. Also, DCDC2 seems to interact with DYX1C1 in our ADHD samples when both genes are analyzed epistatically. Markers previously associated to dyslexia have been found to be also linked to hyperactivity and/or inattention, as shown in Couto el al. [25] and Mascheretti et al. [41]. DYX1C1 was also evaluated in ADHD samples [44,45] and a haplotype of 6 SNPs was connected to the attentional symptoms of this disorder.

Concerning the previously reported allelic association of ADHD candidate SNPs in DBH, COMT and MAOA, none of these was replicated in our ADHD samples, although we found some significant effects following other approaches (S1 Table). COMT appears to be associated in the genotypic analysis to our dyslexia samples and has been connected recently to reading skill tasks (PA and spelling) [46], as well as to reading comprehension [47], adding importance to the possible role of this gene in reading-related cognitive process. As shown by Grigorenko et al. [47], there was significant divergence in the frequencies of 4 COMT haplotypes between individuals with and without comprehension difficulties.

Dyslexia and ADHD prevalence dissimilarities between males and females is well reported in numerous studies [27], and it is known that dopaminergic neurotransmission, implicated in many cognitive functions, could be modulated by gender [48]. Accordingly, the COMT gene has been identified in several ADHD studies as having sex specific effects [49,50] and its susceptibility alleles may differ [51]. Given this fact, COMT emerges as a good candidate to scrutinize for possible divergences in cognitive processes between boys and girls. Our results do not show significant differences for this locus, but a trend (p<0.1) is observed in our dyslexic group (S2 and S3 Tables).

An essential point that should be taken into consideration when trying to replicate results of genetic association studies is the diagnostic criteria of the samples studied, which is particularly hard to establish for certain syndromes. In many studies, dyslexia and ADHD are classified as separate clinical groups, and usually considered independently, although usually there is a substantial percentage of cases that share both conditions [19,52,53]. Despite the difficulty in defining the boundaries of certain neurological disorders and the complications of searching for genetic markers for complex traits, several loci have been proposed as potential biomarkers for dyslexia [4] and ADHD [54]. However, the results obtained in different studies are often contradictory and their reproducibility is limited. The heterogeneity of the criteria or the psychometric tests used to describe the categorical groups makes the comparison between studies difficult, and this might be one reason for the inconsistencies found among different studies and populations. Moreover, the ethnicity of the cohorts may also influence this low reproducibility, as the frequencies of the polymorphisms analyzed are heterogeneous around the world. Furthermore, different genetic variants for the same genes are considered in each study, and causative gene variants have been shown to be different depending on the population [55].

Given that some of the previous studies relating dyslexia or ADHD candidate genes with these disorders have shown quite inconsistent results, then perhaps establishing genetic relationships by focusing on cognitive skills, rather than centering on contrasting strictly-diagnosed dyslexia or ADHD, may produce successful results. Following this approach, our study shows some systematic relationships between cognitive quantitative traits and genes:

  1. Different research groups [5,31,5658] have found correlations of DCDC2, KIAA0319 and DYX1C1 with irregular, regular or pseudoword reading, while others did not [8,25]. The association of KIAA0319 and DYX1C1 with word reading was replicated in our population. In addition, we also found a relationship between reading and MAOA, although results obtained for this gene should be considered with caution as it is located in chromosome X and we analyzed boys and girls together.
  2. Another cognitive trait directly connected to the reading process is fluency. The association of DCDC2 with this trait has been reported previously [7,59]. A marker of DYX1C1, as well as several SNPs in KIAA0319 have also been associated to the digit-RAN task [57,60,61]; the results of the current study add to this list rs4504469-KIAA0319 and rs6323-MAOA, as related to the RAN.
  3. Phonological decoding, which plays a central role in both normal and abnormal reading development, has been consistently reported as related to reading ability [6264]. In fact, deficits in phonological awareness are considered the main proximal cause of cases of reading disability [65]. Dyslexia candidate genes have been correlated with tasks measuring phonological abilities, such as DYX1C1 and FOXP2 with short-term memory [56,58] or phonological memory [12,60], or DCDC2 [66] and KIAA0319 [5,7,61,66] with phoneme awareness itself. In our study, in addition to replicating the association of DCDC2 with a phonological awareness task (reaction-time in the phoneme-picture matching task), we have also detected a possible implication of both DBH and MAOA in phonological awareness.
  4. Speech perception deficits in dyslexic samples have long been proposed [67], particularly deficits in the pre-attentive and automatic information processing measured by the Mismatch Negativity (MMN) component. In fact, the MMN, established as an objective measure of speech discrimination, has been suggested as a neurophysiological endophenotype for dyslexia [68]. Furthermore, some research groups have linked MMN with genetic variants, such as SLC2A3 [69] or rare variants in a region between the genes KIAA0319 and DCDC2 [70]. In the present study, we found a relationship between KIAA0319, DBH and MAOA with the task measuring syllable discrimination. Although they measure different processes (e.g., automatic versus attentive processing of speech), both reveal gene-cognition association in speech processing.
  5. Weaknesses in executive domains such as verbal working memory, planning, and response inhibition are consistent cognitive traits in ADHD symptoms [7173]. We failed to replicate the association found by Fosella et al. [74] between MAOA and executive attention and alerting, but the link between rs4680-COMT and the conflict index, based on ANT evaluation, shows that COMT is related to executive control. Several studies have recently associated variants of COMT with cognitive domains such as working memory [75] or cognitive flexibility [76]. In addition, this gene has previously been found to be associated with Stroop attentional tasks [77]. These attentional tasks are not usually analyzed with dyslexia candidate genes, therefore, one intriguing result obtained from our cohort is the relationship of this type of task with KIAA0319, FOXP2 and DYX1C1 SNPs.

Interestingly, FOXP2 and COMT have been found to be associated with dyslexia and with attentional tasks in our case-control study, although these genes are not related to reading variables in the general population. KIAA0319, also associated with dyslexia, is associated with syllable discrimination, RAN and high frequency word reading but also with Numerical-Stroop. In addition, DBH and MAOA did not show any relationship with the defined phenotypes, but appear to have some correlation with both attention and reading-skills in the general population. This fact reinforces the importance of studying the quantitative measures used to define each phenotype considered, in order to discover evidence of cause-consequence pairs between genes and cognitive processes respectively, instead on focusing just on the categorical groups extrapolated from these quantitative measures.

We are aware of the limitations of this study and, therefore would like to specify them before concluding: (i) the hyperactivity or other possible disorder comorbidities were not diagnosed, (ii) the potential effects of the environment were not taken into account; (iii) the comorbid group is small, and (iv) the sample size in some of the comparisons performed is different.

In summary, significant association of some candidate SNPs with dyslexia and ADHD has been replicated in our Spanish population, but the significance depends on the particular phenotypic groups compared. These outcomes support the importance of a clear definition of the phenotype, especially when comorbid samples are present. Dyslexia and ADHD are complex disorders and so the search for interacting patterns of genes as well as environmental influences will give rise to more successful and reproducible results. Moreover, identification of the relationships between some DNA variants and cognitive tasks, especially when the polymorphisms themselves are not associated with any disorder, adds value to the research on endophenotypes, instead of the traditional dichotomist classification. These improvements will help us find precise and more specific genetic causes of these cognitive dysfunctions.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Association results for single markers at different genetic models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Association results for single markers at different genetic models in female samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Association results for single markers at different genetic models in male samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Association results for single markers at allelic model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Association results for single markers at allelic model in female samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Association results for single markers at allelic model in male samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s006

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Candidate SNP´s frequencies in European population.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s007

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Mean values and standard deviation (SD) of the psychometric characteristics across ages for dyslexia and dyslexia-control samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s008

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Mean values and standard deviation (SD) of the psychometric characteristics across ages for ADHD and ADHD-control samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s009

(DOCX)

S10 Table. Mean values and standard deviation (SD) of the psychometric characteristics across ages for comorbid and comorbid-control samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s010

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Principal component analysis.

Graphic representation of the two principal components obtained through the PLINK program for the cases and controls of dyslexia, ADHD and comorbidity after whole genome genotyping with OmniExpress Beadchips (Illumina Inc.). Green dots = Dyslexia; grey = Dyslexia controls; pink = ADHD; black = ADHD controls; blue = comorbids; yellow = Comorbid controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s012

(PDF)

S1 Dataset. Contains the SNP genotyping raw data for each performed comparative as *.ped and *.map files, and a subfolder named “Freq_two locus” containing the frequency of the composite genotypes (two by two).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206431.s013

(RAR)

Acknowledgments

First, we would like to apologize to those whose related publications were not cited due to space limitations. We are grateful to James D. Sutherland for the revision of English grammar. The work of MSM and MC was supported by CONSOLIDER-Ingenio-2010_COEDUCA (CSD2008-00048). AMA, LB and AG-L’s work was supported by the Basque Department of Industry, Tourism and Trade (Etortek Program), Innovation Technology Department of Bizkaia and CIBERehd Network. MC was also supported by grants (PSI2015-67353-R), and Ayuda Centro de Excelencia Severo Ochoa SEV-2015-0490 from the MINECO, and by grant (ERC-2011-ADG-295362) from the European Research Council. We thank Arkaitz Carracedo for critical reading of the manuscript and all the participants in the study.

References

  1. 1. Fisher SE, DeFries JC. Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3: 767–780. pmid:12360321
  2. 2. Thapar A, O’Donovan M, Owen MJ. The genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum Mol Genet. 2005/10/26. 2005;14: R275–82. pmid:16244326
  3. 3. Stergiakouli E, Thapar A. Fitting the pieces together: current research on the genetic basis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2010/09/22. 2010;6: 551–60. pmid:20856918
  4. 4. Carrion-Castillo A, Franke B, Fisher SE. Molecular genetics of dyslexia: An overview. Dyslexia. 2013. pp. 214–240. pmid:24133036
  5. 5. Francks C, Paracchini S, Smith SD, Richardson AJ, Scerri TS, Cardon LR, et al. A 77-kilobase region of chromosome 6p22.2 is associated with dyslexia in families from the United Kingdom and from the United States. Am J Hum Genet. 2004/10/30. 2004;75: 1046–1058. S0002-9297(07)60072-X [pii] pmid:15514892
  6. 6. Harold D, Paracchini S, Scerri T, Dennis M, Cope N, Hill G, et al. Further evidence that the KIAA0319 gene confers susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11: 1085–1091. pmid:17033633
  7. 7. Scerri TS, Morris AP, Buckingham LL, Newbury DF, Miller LL, Monaco AP, et al. DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biol Psychiatry. 2011/04/05. 2011;70: 237–245. S0006-3223(11)00135-1 [pii] pmid:21457949
  8. 8. Newbury DF, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Winchester L, Addis L, Richardson AJ, et al. Investigation of Dyslexia and SLI Risk Variants in Reading- and Language-Impaired Subjects. Behav Genet. 2011;41: 90–104. pmid:21165691
  9. 9. Taipale M, Kaminen N, Nopola-Hemmi J, Haltia T, Myllyluoma B, Lyytinen H, et al. A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003/09/05. 2003;100: 11553–11558. [pii] pmid:12954984
  10. 10. Bellini G, Bravaccio C, Calamoneri F, Donatella Cocuzza M, Fiorillo P, Gagliano A, et al. No evidence for association between dyslexia and DYX1C1 functional variants in a group of children and adolescents from Southern Italy. J Mol Neurosci. 2005;27: 311–314. JMN:27:3:311 [pii] pmid:16280601
  11. 11. Hannula-Jouppi K, Kaminen-Ahola N, Taipale M, Eklund R, Nopola-Hemmi J, Kaariainen H, et al. The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genet. 2005/10/29. 2005;1: e50. pmid:16254601
  12. 12. Peter B, Raskind WH, Matsushita M, Lisowski M, Vu T, Berninger VW, et al. Replication of CNTNAP2 association with nonword repetition and support for FOXP2 association with timed reading and motor activities in a dyslexia family sample. J Neurodev Disord. 2011/04/13. 2011;3: 39–49. pmid:21484596
  13. 13. LaHoste GJ, Swanson JM, Wigal SB, Glabe C, Wigal T, King N, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 1996;1: 121–124. pmid:9118321
  14. 14. Cook EH Jr., Stein MA, Krasowski MD, Cox NJ, Olkon DM, Kieffer JE, et al. Association of attention-deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. Am J Hum Genet. 1995/04/01. 1995;56: 993–998. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=7717410 pmid:7717410
  15. 15. Tong J, McKinley L-A, Cummins TDR, Johnson B, Matthews N, Vance A, et al. Identification and functional characterisation of a novel dopamine beta hydroxylase gene variant associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2015;16: 610–618. pmid:25975715
  16. 16. Hawi Z, Millar N, Daly G, Fitzgerald M, Gill M. No association between catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in an Irish sample. Am J Med Genet—Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2000;96: 282–284.
  17. 17. Fan J, Fossella J, Sommer T, Wu Y, Posner MI. Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003/05/30. 2003;100: 7406–7411. [pii] pmid:12773616
  18. 18. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Chhabildas N, Hulslander J. Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of the common deficit. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;27: 35–78. pmid:15737942
  19. 19. Greven CU, Harlaar N, Dale PS, Plomin R. Genetic Overlap between ADHD Symptoms and Reading is largely Driven by Inattentiveness rather than Hyperactivity-Impulsivity. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20: 6–14. pmid:21286364
  20. 20. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, DeFries JC. Understanding comorbidity: a twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B: 709–714. pmid:17440942
  21. 21. Plourde V, Boivin M, Forget-Dubois N, Brendgen M, Vitaro F, Marino C, et al. Phenotypic and genetic associations between reading comprehension, decoding skills, and ADHD dimensions: Evidence from two population-based studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2015;56: 1074–1082. pmid:25683090
  22. 22. Wadsworth SJ, DeFries JC, Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK. Genetic Etiologies of Comorbidity and Stability for Reading Difficulties and ADHD: A Replication Study. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2016; 1–5.
  23. 23. Loo SK, Fisher SE, Francks C, Ogdie MN, MacPhie IL, Yang M, et al. Genome-wide scan of reading ability in affected sibling pairs with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: unique and shared genetic effects. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9: 485–493. pmid:14625563
  24. 24. Wadsworth SJ, DeFries JC, Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK. The Colorado Longitudinal Twin Study of Reading Difficulties and ADHD: Etiologies of Comorbidity and Stability. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2015;18: 755–61. pmid:26537134
  25. 25. Couto JM, Gomez L, Wigg K, Ickowicz A, Pathare T, Malone M, et al. Association of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder with a Candidate Region for Reading Disabilities on Chromosome 6p. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66: 368–375. pmid:19362708
  26. 26. Field LL, Shumansky K, Ryan J, Truong D, Swiergala E, Kaplan BJ. Dense-map genome scan for dyslexia supports loci at 4q13, 16p12, 17q22; suggests novel locus at 7q36. Genes, Brain Behav. 2013;12: 56–69. pmid:23190410
  27. 27. Boada R, Willcutt EG, Pennington BF. Understanding the comorbidity between Dyslexia and Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder. Top lang Disord. 2012;32: 264–284.
  28. 28. Bishop D. The interface between genetic and psychology: lessons from developmental dyslexia. Proc R Soc London B. 2015;282.
  29. 29. Flint J, Munafò MR. The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychol Med. 2007;37: 163–80. pmid:16978446
  30. 30. Paracchini S, Steer CD, Buckingham LL, Morris AP, Ring S, Scerri T, et al. Association of the KIAA0319 dyslexia susceptibility gene with reading skills in the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 2008/10/03. 2008;165: 1576–1584. appi.ajp.2008.07121872 [pii] pmid:18829873
  31. 31. Lind PA, Luciano M, Wright MJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Bates TC. Dyslexia and DCDC2: normal variation in reading and spelling is associated with DCDC2 polymorphisms in an Australian population sample. Eur J Hum Genet EJHG. 2010;18: 668–673. pmid:20068590
  32. 32. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007/08/19. 2007;81: 559–575. S0002-9297(07)61352-4 [pii] pmid:17701901
  33. 33. Hochberg Y. A sharper bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika. 1988;75: 800–802.
  34. 34. Willcutt EG, Betjemann RS, McGrath LM, Chhabildas NA, Olson RK, DeFries JC, et al. Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex. 2010;46: 1345–1361. pmid:20828676
  35. 35. Cope NA, Hill G, van den Bree M HD, Moskvina V, Green EK, Owen MJ W, O’Donovan J M. No support for association between dyslexia susceptibility 1 candidate 1 and developmental dyslexia. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10: 237–238. pmid:15477871
  36. 36. Paracchini S, Scerri T, Monaco AP. The genetic lexicon of dyslexia. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2007/04/21. 2007;8: 57–79. pmid:17444811
  37. 37. Mascheretti S, Bureau A, Battaglia M, Simone D, Quadrelli E, Croteau J, et al. An assessment of gene-by-environment interactions in developmental dyslexia-related phenotypes. Genes, Brain Behav. 2013;12: 47–55. pmid:23176554
  38. 38. Wilcke A, Weissfuss J, Kirsten H, Wolfram G, Boltze J, Ahnert P. The role of gene DCDC2 in German dyslexics. Ann Dyslexia. 2009;59: 1–11. pmid:19238550
  39. 39. Ludwig KU, Roeske D, Schumacher J, Schulte-Körne G, König IR, Warnke A, et al. Investigation of interaction between DCDC2 and KIAA0319 in a large German dyslexia sample. J Neural Transm. 2008;115: 1587–1589. pmid:18810304
  40. 40. Shao S, Kong R, Zou L, Zhong R, Lou J, Zhou J, et al. The Roles of Genes in the Neuronal Migration and Neurite Outgrowth Network in Developmental Dyslexia: Single- and Multiple-Risk Genetic Variants. Mol Neurobiol. 2016;56: 3967–75.
  41. 41. Mascheretti S, Trezzi V, Giorda R, Boivin M, Plourde V, Vitaro F, et al. Complex effects of dyslexia risk factors account for ADHD traits: Evidence from two independent samples. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2016. pmid:27501527
  42. 42. Trezzi V, Forni D, Giorda R, Villa M, Molteni M, Marino C, et al. The role of READ1 and KIAA0319 genetic variations in developmental dyslexia: Testing main and interactive effects. J Hum Genet. 2017;62: 949–955. pmid:29066855
  43. 43. Powers NR, Eicher JD, Butter F, Kong Y, Miller LL, Ring SM, et al. Alleles of a polymorphic ETV6 binding site in DCDC2 confer risk of reading and language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2013;93: 19–28. pmid:23746548
  44. 44. Wigg KG, Couto JM, Feng Y, Crosbie J, Anderson B, Cate-Carter T, et al. Investigation of the Relationship of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder to the EKN1 Gene on Chromosome 15q21. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2005. pp. 261–283. _5
  45. 45. Wigg KG, Feng Y, Crosbie J, Tannock R, Kennedy JL, Ickowicz A, et al. Association of ADHD and the Protogenin gene in the chromosome 15q21.3 reading disabilities linkage region. Genes, Brain Behav. 2008;7: 877–886. pmid:19076634
  46. 46. Landi N, Frost SJ, Mencl WE, Preston JL, Jacobsen LK, Lee M, et al. The COMT Val/Met polymorphism is associated with reading related skills and consistent patterns of functional neural activation. Dev Sci. 2013;16: 13–23. pmid:23278923
  47. 47. Grigorenko EL, Deyoung CG, Getchell M, Haeffel GJ, Klinteberg BAF, Koposov RA, et al. Exploring interactive effects of genes and environments in etiology of individual differences in reading comprehension. Dev Psychopathol. 2007;19: 1089–1103. pmid:17931436
  48. 48. Miller DI, Halpern DF. The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014. pp. 37–45. pmid:24246136
  49. 49. Akutagava-Martins GC, Salatino-Oliveira A, Kieling C, Genro JP, Polanczyk G V., Anselmi L, et al. COMT and DAT1 genes are associated with hyperactivity and inattention traits in the 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort: evidence of sex-specific combined effect. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2016;41: 405–412. pmid:27327562
  50. 50. Gurvich C, Rossell S. Dopamine and cognitive control: Sex-by-genotype interactions influence the capacity to switch attention. Behav Brain Reserach. 2015;281: 96–101.
  51. 51. Biederman J, Kim JW, Doyle AE, Mick E, Fagerness J, Smoller JW, et al. Sexually dimorphic effects of four genes (COMT, SLC6A2, MAOA, SLC6A4) in genetic associations of ADHD: a preliminary study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008/10/22. 2008;147B: 1511–1518. pmid:18937309
  52. 52. Sheikhi AR, Martin N, Hay D, Piek JP. Phenotype refinement for comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. Am J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2013;162: 44–54. pmid:23197436
  53. 53. McGrath LM, Pennington BF, Shanahan MA, Santerre-Lemmon LE, Barnard HD, Willcutt EG, et al. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2011;52: 547–557. pmid:21126246
  54. 54. Thapar A, O’Donovan M, Owen MJ. The genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum Mol Genet. 2005/10/26. 2005;14 Spec No: R275-82. 14/suppl_2/R275 [pii] pmid:16244326
  55. 55. Becker J, Czamara D, Scerri TS, Ramus F, Csépe V, Talcott JB, et al. Genetic analysis of dyslexia candidate genes in the European cross-linguistic NeuroDys cohort. Eur J Hum Genet. 2014;22: 675–80. pmid:24022301
  56. 56. Bates TC, Lind PA, Luciano M, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Wright MJ. Dyslexia and DYX1C1: deficits in reading and spelling associated with a missense mutation. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15: 1190–1196. pmid:19901951
  57. 57. Carrion-Castillo A, Maassen B, Franke B, Heister A, Naber M, van der Leij A, et al. Association analysis of dyslexia candidate genes in a Dutch longitudinal sample. Eur J Hum Genet. 2017; 1–9.
  58. 58. Venkatesh SK, Siddaiah A, Padakannaya P, Ramachandra NB. Association of SNPs of DYX1C1 with developmental dyslexia in an Indian population. Psychiatr Genet. 2014;24: 10–20. pmid:24362368
  59. 59. Zhang Y, Li J, Song S, Tardif T, Burmeister M, Villafuerte SM, et al. Association of DCDC2 polymorphisms with normal variations in reading abilities in a Chinese population. PLoS One. 2016;11. pmid:27100778
  60. 60. Lim CK, Ho CS, Chou CH, Waye MM. Association of the rs3743205 variant of DYX1C1 with dyslexia in Chinese children. Behav Brain Funct. 2011/05/24. 2011;7: 16. 1744-9081-7-16 [pii] pmid:21599957
  61. 61. Lim CK-P, Wong AM-B, Ho CS-H, Waye MM-Y. A common haplotype of KIAA0319 contributes to the phonological awareness skill in Chinese children. Behav Brain Funct. 2014;10: 23. pmid:25015435
  62. 62. Vellutino F, Fletcher JM, Snowling M, Scanlon D. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45: 2–40. pmid:14959801
  63. 63. Wagner RK. Phonological processing abilities and reading: implications for disabled readers. J Learn Disabil. 1986;19: 623–630. pmid:3540167
  64. 64. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA, Hecht S A, Barker T A, Burgess S R, et al. Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. 1997;33: 468–79. pmid:9149925
  65. 65. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 1994. pp. 73–87.
  66. 66. Deffenbacher KE, Kenyon JB, Hoover DM, Olson RK, Pennington BF, DeFries JC, et al. Refinement of the 6p21.3 quantitative trait locus influencing dyslexia: linkage and association analyses. Hum Genet. 2004;115: 128–138. pmid:15138886
  67. 67. Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Auditory processing and dyslexia: evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. Neuroreport. 1998.
  68. 68. Neuhoff N, Bruder J, Bartling J, Warnke A, Remschmidt H, Müller-Myhsok B, et al. Evidence for the late MMN as a neurophysiological endophenotype for dyslexia. PLoS One. 2012;7. pmid:22606227
  69. 69. Roeske D, Ludwig KU, Neuhoff N, Becker J, Bartling J, Bruder J, et al. First genome-wide association scan on neurophysiological endophenotypes points to trans-regulation effects on SLC2A3 in dyslexic children. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16: 97–107. pmid:19786962
  70. 70. Czamara D, Bruder J, Becker J, Bartling J, Hoffmann P, Ludwig KU, et al. Association of a rare variant with mismatch negativity in a region between KIAA0319 and DCDC2 in dyslexia. Behav Genet. 2011;41: 110–119. pmid:21104116
  71. 71. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121: 65–94. pmid:9000892
  72. 72. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone S V, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57: 1336–1346. pmid:15950006
  73. 73. Willcutt EG, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Nigg JT, Sergeant JA. Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24: 195–226.
  74. 74. Fossella J, Sommer T, Fan J, Wu Y, Swanson JM, Pfaff DW, et al. Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neurosci. 2002/10/09. 2002;3: 14. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=12366871 pmid:12366871
  75. 75. Jin J, Liu L, Gao Q, Chan RCK, Li H, Chen Y, et al. The divergent impact of COMT Val158Met on executive function in children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Genes Brain Behav. 2016;15: 271–279. pmid:26560848
  76. 76. Degen C, Zschocke J, Toro P, Sattler C, Wahl H-W, Schönknecht P, et al. The COMT p.Val158Met polymorphism and cognitive performance in adult development, healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;41: 27–34. pmid:26489081
  77. 77. Schneider KK, Schote AB, Meyer J, Frings C. Genes of the dopaminergic system selectively modulate top-down but not bottom-up attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;15: 104–116. pmid:25253063