Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Prosocial Bonuses Increase Employee Satisfaction and Team Performance

  • Lalin Anik ,

    lalin.anik@duke.edu

    Affiliation Marketing Department, Fuqua School of Business, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America

  • Lara B. Aknin,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada

  • Michael I. Norton,

    Affiliation Marketing Department, Harvard Business School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Elizabeth W. Dunn,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Jordi Quoidbach

    Affiliation Department of Economics and Business, Pompeu Fabra University, Barcelona, Spain

Abstract

In three field studies, we explore the impact of providing employees and teammates with prosocial bonuses, a novel type of bonus spent on others rather than on oneself. In Experiment 1, we show that prosocial bonuses in the form of donations to charity lead to happier and more satisfied employees at an Australian bank. In Experiments 2a and 2b, we show that prosocial bonuses in the form of expenditures on teammates lead to better performance in both sports teams in Canada and pharmaceutical sales teams in Belgium. These results suggest that a minor adjustment to employee bonuses – shifting the focus from the self to others – can produce measurable benefits for employees and organizations.

Introduction

A recent survey revealed that just 46% of Americans are satisfied with their jobs, the lowest level recorded by the Conference Board [1] in the past two decades. Yet over the same time frame, Americans have come to spend more and more of their time at work [2]. Taken together, this trend suggests that employees are becoming more and more unhappy more and more of the time at work, hardly a formula for a healthy and productive workplace. In this increasingly negative environment, how can employers incentivize their employees to increase their happiness, job satisfaction, and ultimately their job performance?

Certainly, designing effective incentive schemes is a central challenge for a wide range of organizations, from multi-national corporations to academic departments. In pursuit of identifying the most effective strategies, organizations have devised an impressive variety of such bonuses, from fixed salaries to pay-per-performance, from commissions to end-of-year bonuses. We suggest that the wide variety in such schemes masks a shared assumption: That the best way to motivate employees is to reward them with money that they then spend on themselves. We propose an alternative means of incentivizing employees – what we term “prosocial bonuses” – in which organizations provide employees with bonuses used to engage in prosocial actions towards charities and co-workers.

Below, we first review research exploring existing methods of increasing workplace performance, including individual-based and team-based bonus schemes, which tend to reveal both benefits and unexpected costs. We then briefly review the literature on the benefits of improving social life in the work place, such as increasing employee citizenship behaviors. Next, we argue that prosocial bonuses mitigate some of the issues that arise with individual- and team-based compensation schemes, while retaining the benefits of improving employee’s social lives in the workplace. Finally, we examine the impact of these prosocial bonuses on employee satisfaction and team performance, by reporting results from three “proof of concept” field experiments conducted in different countries.

Individual- and Team-Based Incentive Schemes

When asked why they work, individuals most commonly reply “money” [3]. But what is the effect of money on employees’ job satisfaction and performance? On one hand, monetary bonuses have been found to produce positive effects – increased productivity, effort, performance, and job satisfaction [4][9]. Individual bonuses increase job satisfaction in part because employees see their time and effort being rewarded [10][13]. From pay-per-performance to piece rate compensation schemes to profit sharing to bonuses, individual-based incentive schemes can lead to improved employee outcomes [8], [14][18].

On the other hand, individual incentives – such as large bonuses – are often surprisingly ineffective in increasing employee morale and productivity [19][20]. Rewarding individual employees can produce negative outcomes by eroding workplace cohesion [21], as employees become reluctant to share information with others even at the expense of reduced output [22]. Relative comparisons at the individual level create competition which results in decreased trust, sharing and teamwork [23][25]; in Drago and Turnbull [26], for example, tournament-based compensation led to decreased helping behavior and increased the potential for sabotaging other workers.

In an effort to prevent such negative competitive dynamics that can result from individual-based bonuses, organizations often turn to incentivizing employees for their collective performance, encouraging cooperation and teamwork rather than competition [27][29]. Indeed, a growing body of research suggests that interpersonal relationships enable employees to experience their work as important and meaningful [30][36]. Furthermore, evidence suggests that interpersonal relationships often enhance employees’ motivations, opportunities, and resources at work [37][40]. Positive interpersonal relationships with coworkers provide social support and a buffer from stressful events [41][43], which in turn predict team commitment [44], job engagement [45][46], and job satisfaction [47][49].

In some cases, team-based compensation schemes have been shown to raise this sense of cooperation and cohesiveness between team members [22], [50], inducing them to exert additional effort toward helping one another [51][54]. Importantly, such increased cooperation due to interdependent rewards has been shown to improve team performance [55], suggesting that team-based bonuses may be an effective means of improving employee social life. As with individual-based bonuses, however, team-based bonuses offer important advantages but also potential drawbacks – such as free riding [56], motivational loss due to the perception of inequity [57], and suboptimization of team goals [58]. Thus while team-based bonuses have the potential to improve relationships between co-workers, they can also lead to “antisocial” behaviors – and decreased employee outcomes.

Prosocial Bonuses

We suggest that prosocial bonuses offer an alternative approach that has the potential to provide some of the same benefits as team-based compensation – increased social support, cohesion, and performance – while carrying fewer drawbacks. Research suggests that the desire to help others is a need deeply rooted in human nature [59][60], and that giving to others has a causal impact on increasing happiness and life satisfaction [61][62]. At the organizational level, previous correlational research suggests that prosocial behavior in the workplace – often termed citizenship behaviors – is linked to employee morale and performance [63]: the extent to which employees perceive themselves and their organizations as prosocial predicts organizational commitment [64][66]. We suggest that prosocial bonuses can have a causal impact on employee satisfaction and performance, such that providing employees with money to help others would have a greater organizational impact than providing employees with money to spend on themselves.

We note that we are not the first researchers to examine the interplay of incentives and prosocial behavior; indeed, several investigations point to the potential risk in mixing money with altruism [67]: paying children to collect money for charity decreases their efforts [68], publicly rewarding adults for earning money for charity also decreases effort [69], and paying friends to help with a move reduces the amount of help received [70]. Unlike these kinds of “prosocial incentives,” however, the prosocial bonuses we provide in the experiments below are not contingent upon or linked to any behavior – employees are simply given money by the firm to spend prosocially. In this sense, our investigation uses a version of a “reciprocity by proxy” strategy outlined by Goldstein, Griskevicius, and Cialdini [71]. In this investigation, guests who were informed that a hotel had already given a donation to an environmental cause were more likely to reuse their towels than those who were told the hotel would make a donation only if they reused their towels; their results showed that providing the prosocial bonus up front was more effective than linking the incentive directly to the behavior. Following this logic, we predicted that offering employees prosocial bonuses that were not linked to any current behavior or expectation of future behavior would be effective in increasing employee satisfaction.

Overview of the Present Research

We examine whether randomly assigning employees to engage in prosocial behavior – via prosocial bonuses – can have a causal impact on employee well-being, job satisfaction, and job performance. In our field studies, some employees and teammates are given non-contingent “prosocial bonuses” – money that they receive as a windfall that they are encouraged to spend in a prosocial manner. In Experiment 1, we give some employees of a company the opportunity to donate money to charity, examining the impact of this intervention on both employee well-being and job satisfaction. In Experiments 2a and 2b, we move beyond assessment of psychological constructs to behavioral measures; by comparing prosocial versus personal bonuses, we investigate their impact on team performance in the two different contexts of sales teams and sports teams.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

Data collection for Experiment 1 was approved by the Harvard University Behavioral Research Ethics Board. Data collection for Experiment 2a was approved by the University of British Columbia’s Behavioral Research Ethics Board (B06-0557). Data collection for Experiment 2b was overseen by University of Liège. Written informed consent was obtained for all studies.

Experiment 1

In Experiment 1, we examine the impact of prosocial bonuses on the most widely studied attitude in the field of organizational behavior, job satisfaction – broadly defined, employees’ subjective evaluation of their work experience [72][73]. The large number of investigations examining factors that influence job satisfaction have tended to focus on two fundamental determinants: (1) aspects of employees, such as individual differences in self-esteem or education [74][78] and (2) aspects of the job itself, such as communicating clear task goals and giving feedback when those goals are achieved [79][84]. Adding a novel contribution to the literature on job satisfaction, we examine the impact of prosocial bonuses. To do so, we assigned some employees of a large bank to receive a prosocial bonus in the form of money from the company to donate to charity, and examined the impact of spending this bonus on job satisfaction, compared to employees not given this bonus.

Participants.

A total of 300 employees at an Australian bank were invited by their employer to participate in an experiment; 121 of these employees did not respond to the initial email and were therefore not included in our sample. Of the 179 employees that did respond to the invitation, 46 employees completed only the Time 1 survey in which they reported their age, gender, salary, years at company as well as their happiness and job satisfaction. These 46 participants did not differ from our main sample in terms of age, gender, income or years at company, Time 1 happiness or job satisfaction (ts<1.13, ps >.26). Employees completing only the Time 1 survey were not included in the analyses below, leaving a final sample of 133 bank employees (59 percent female; Ncontrol = 48, N$25 = 41, and N$50 = 44) with a wide range of income, age, and years at the company (Table 1).

Design and procedure.

On November 17, 2008, all employees received an email from their employer asking them to participate in a multi-stage experiment on workplace attitudes. Employees were assured that their participation was voluntary and that their responses would be anonymous. If employees followed a link indicating their willingness to participate, they were directed to the Time 1 survey. On the Time 1 survey, participants reported their gender, age, and salary. Because this was a field experiment conducted during a work day, we asked participants to complete single-item measures of happiness and job satisfaction at Time 1 and Time 2. Participants rated how happy they felt on the 5-point scale (1: very slightly or not at all to 5: extremely) used in the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [85]. This single-item measure has been previously shown to be highly correlated with the full scale (r = .48, p<.001) [86], and similar single-item measures of happiness have been widely used in the well-being literature [87][88]. To assess job satisfaction, participants completed a measure drawn from the Michigan Organizational Assessment Questionnaire, rating their agreement with the statement “All in all I'm satisfied with my job” on a 7-point scale (1: strongly disagree to 7: strongly agree; 89). Single-item measures of job satisfaction have been shown to correlate with longer assessments, and yield adequate validity [90][92].

Two weeks later, on December 3, 2008, based on random assignment, employees in the control condition were sent an email that directed them to complete the Time 2 survey. Employees randomly assigned to one of the two experimental conditions were informed that the company had given them a charity voucher worth approximately $25 or $50 US at the time to donate to a charity of their choice. Participants in the two charity voucher conditions followed a link that took them to a charity website (KarmaCurrency.com.au) where they could donate to a wide range of charities of their choice. After completing the donation, participants were automatically redirected to the Time 2 survey. Voucher redemption data shows that about half of the employees redeemed their charity vouchers on the day they received it (December 3, 2008). The remaining vouchers were redeemed during the following two weeks with the last redemption on December 18, 2008.

Experiments 2a & 2b

Experiment 1 revealed that providing employees with the opportunity to spend prosocial bonuses can yield two psychological benefits: increased happiness and job satisfaction. Indeed, employees who donated $50 to charity on behalf of their company reported increased happiness and job satisfaction. Do the benefits of prosocial bonuses extend beyond employee well-being to improving actual performance – and the organizations’ bottom line? As with job satisfaction, previous research has focused on two categories of predictors of job performance, some examining the links between employees’ individual differences (e.g., their general aptitude or conscientiousness) and their performance, and other research examining how aspects of the job itself can improve or undermine performance [77], [93][96]. We suggest that prosocial bonuses offer an additional approach to impacting job performance; we expected that compared to personal bonuses, prosocial bonuses would have a larger impact on job performance.

In addition to documenting the impact of prosocial bonuses on team performance, we also widened our investigation in three ways. First, we sought to extend the time course of our experiment to examine the longer-term effects of prosocial bonuses. In Experiment 1, we measured job satisfaction immediately after the prosocial bonus, which we acknowledge is likely when the impact of giving was at its greatest. We assess more delayed or extended benefits of prosocial bonuses in Experiments 2a and 2b. Second, we explored the impact of a different form of prosocial bonuses; to do so, we redirected generous spending from external charitable causes to co-workers and teammates within the organization. Third, Experiment 1 compared the effects of prosocial bonuses to a control condition; in Experiments 2a and 2b we directly compared the impact of prosocial and personal bonuses, by giving members of some teams money to spend on their teammates and members of other teams money to spend on themselves. Due to logistical reasons, a control condition could not be included in Experiments 2a and 2b.

Experiment 2a: Sports Teams Methods

Participants.

Sixty-two students (83 percent male; Mage = 20.49, SD = 2.6) on 11 recreational dodge ball teams (Mmembers = 4.71, SD = 1.4) completed the experiment at the University of British Columbia for a chance to win $100. Potential participants were informed that one person would be selected to win the $100 cash prize.

Procedure.

Teams were approached in person by a research assistant in a recreation center on campus and invited to participate in a study. Members of participating teams completed a basic demographics survey in which they noted their age, gender, annual income and student status. Each team was randomly assigned to the personal or prosocial bonuses condition. Within each team, approximately one-third of team members were randomly selected to receive $20 USD (∼$20 CDN) to spend over the subsequent week. Participants in the personal bonus condition were instructed to “spend the money on a bill, expense, or gift for yourself”, while participants in the prosocial bonus condition were instructed to “spend the money on a teammate” who was randomly selected. Both personal and prosocial spending instructions were presented in written form and then explained by a research assistant to ensure participants understood the instructions.

Team performance.

Performance was assessed with the percentage of games won out of total games played on the date of the initial survey (Time 1) and approximately two weeks later (Time 2). Only team level performance could be measured, as individual players’ statistics were not collected by the recreational dodge ball league.

Experiment 2b: Sales Teams Methods

Participants.

One hundred and twelve salespersons at a Belgian pharmaceutical company were emailed by their Human Resources Department with an invitation to take part in an experiment. All of the salespersons indicated willingness to participate and provided their demographic information. Twenty-four salespeople were excluded from the experiment for various reasons. Specifically, for ten salespersons we could not get performance data from the company. Some salespersons, for example, were active in two different sales territories, sharing their sales performance with multiple teams. Others were in charge of special projects for which we could not have access to a performance indicator. An additional fourteen salespersons who were team leaders were excluded as we wanted to examine giving among peers, rather than between employees and supervisors. The remaining 88 salespersons (50 percent male; Mage = 36.0, SD = 6.9) working in 14 teams (Mmembers = 6.3, SD = 3.0) completed this experiment in exchange for a chance to win an iPod. Participants were assured that participation was voluntary and their responses would remain confidential.

Design and Procedure.

The pharmaceutical salespersons worked in teams that were in charge of the same geographical region. Although each salesperson worked alone, team members would share strategic information about prospects (e.g., “You should go to that business because the owner doesn’t like me”). Each sales team was randomly assigned to the prosocial or personal bonuses condition. Because teams varied in size, we randomly selected approximately one-third of team members, and at a companywide event two weeks after the initial email, we gave these individuals $22 USD (15 Euros) to spend by the end of the week. Participants were informed that the funds were provided as part of a study conducted by independent researchers. On personal bonus teams, participants who received money were asked to “spend it on a bill, expense, or gift for yourself” (as in [62]), whereas on prosocial bonus teams, participants who received money were instructed to “spend it on a teammate” who was randomly selected from the remaining team members and specified for each spender. All participants receiving funds to spend were asked to complete the spending by the end of the week. While one-third of the salespersons were assigned to be spenders (i.e. someone given money to spend on themselves or a coworker), the remaining participants were assigned as receivers (i.e. someone who received a gift from a coworker) or third-party observers (i.e. someone not assigned as a spender or receiver). Receivers were not informed that they would receive a gift from a co-worker. In order to avoid confusion at the companywide event, participants assigned to be receivers and third-party observers also received envelopes with a brief note thanking them for their participation in the study but they were not informed of the spending manipulation. Everyone was instructed to open the envelopes alone at home.

At the end of the week, spenders reported how they had used the money, and receivers were reported whether they had received any gifts, enabling us to confirm spenders’ reports. The twenty-four salespersons excluded from our study were not eligible to be spenders or receivers. We provided the company with the funds to be distributed to the salespersons, who were fully informed that the study was conducted only by independent researchers, and that the company would not have access to any of the data.

Team performance.

Performance was assessed immediately before (Time 1) and one month after our spending intervention (Time 2). Pharmaceutical salespeople promote their product to physicians, pharmacies, and hospitals, rather than selling directly to customers. As such, the standard indicator of pharmaceutical sales team success is the total monthly sales collected by each pharmaceutical sales team (in Euros) in the geographical region under their purview. Therefore, we used monthly team sales as our measure of team performance.

Results

Experiment 1

Happiness.

A preliminary ANOVA confirmed that there was no difference between conditions in Time 1 happiness, F(2, 130)  = .12, p>.85, ηp2 = .02; we therefore entered experimental condition into an ANCOVA predicting Time 2 happiness, controlling for Time 1 happiness. We observed a significant main effect of condition, F(2, 129)  = 5.85, p<.005, ηp2 = .08. Follow-up analyses showed that participants who received a $50 USD charity voucher reported being significantly happier, t(43)  = 5.12, p<.001, whereas happiness levels were unchanged from Time 1 to Time 2 for those in the control and $25 USD conditions, ts <1 (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Change in happiness and job satisfaction between Time 1 and Time 2 as a function of condition (Experiment 1).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075509.t002

Job Satisfaction.

As with happiness, a preliminary ANOVA confirmed that there were no between-group differences in Time 1 job satisfaction, F(2, 130)  = .54, p>.77, ηp2 = .004. Entering condition into an ANCOVA predicting Time 2 job satisfaction, controlling for Time 1 job satisfaction, revealed a significant main effect of condition, F(2, 129)  = 3.14, p<.05, ηp2 = .05. As with happiness, participants who received a $50 USD charity voucher showed an increase in job satisfaction, t(43)  = 2.46, p<.02, which was unchanged for those in the control and $25 USD conditions, ts<1.19 (Table 2).

Experiment 2a

Spending examples.

Participants who received a personal or prosocial bonus were asked to report how they spent this money. On personal bonus teams, spenders reported buying items for themselves such as sportswear, small jewelry, CDs, food, and alcohol. On prosocial bonus teams, spenders reported buying items for others such as books, wine, a plant, a stuffed animal, a piñata and paying a teammate’s sports league fee.

Spending condition and team performance.

To confirm that there were no significant differences in initial performance, we entered condition (personal bonus vs. prosocial bonus) into an ANOVA predicting Time 1 performance; this analysis revealed no significant effect, F(1, 10)  = .10, p = .77. As in Experiment 1, therefore, we entered the same variables into an ANCOVA predicting Time 2 performance, controlling for Time 1 performance. We found a marginal main effect in the predicted direction, whereby prosocial bonus teams performed better than personal bonus teams, F(1, 8)  = 3.75, p = .09, ηp2 = .32 (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Change in sports and sales team performance between Time 1 and Time 2 as a function of condition (Experiments 2a and 2b).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075509.t003

Next, we examined the impact of prosocial vs. personal bonuses on the change in performance from Time 1 to Time 2. In the prosocial bonuses condition, sports teams showed a large, but statistically marginally significant increase in performance, t(5) = 1.87, p = .12, d = .76. Meanwhile, in the personal bonuses condition, there was no evidence for improved performance, t(4)  = 0.39, p = .72, d = .17 (Table 3).

Another way to demonstrate the effectiveness of these interventions is to calculate the return on investment for prosocial and personal bonuses. On sports teams, every $10 people spent on themselves led to a two percent decrease in winning percentage, whereas every $10 spent prosocially led to an 11% increase in winning percentage.

Experiment 2b

Spending examples.

The salespeople who received a personal or prosocial bonus were asked to report how they spent the allotted funds. On personal bonus teams, spenders reported buying items for themselves such as food, alcohol and groceries. On prosocial bonus teams, spenders reported buying items for others such as gift card, chocolate, wine, and treating a teammate to lunch.

Spending condition and team performance.

As in Experiment 2a, to confirm that there were no significant differences in initial performance, we entered condition (personal bonus vs. prosocial bonus) into an ANOVA predicting Time 1 performance; this analysis revealed no significant effect, F(1, 12)  = .24, p = .63. Therefore, we entered the same variables into an ANCOVA predicting Time 2 performance, controlling for Time 1 performance. As in Experiment 2b, we found a marginal main effect, whereby prosocial bonus teams performed better than personal bonus teams, F(1, 11)  = 2.31, p = .16, ηp2 = .17 (Table 3).

Although the simple effect should be interpreted with caution given the very small sample size, closer examination suggests that prosocial bonuses were effective in improving performance from Time 1 to Time 2. That is, in the prosocial bonuses condition, sales teams showed a large and significant increase in performance from Time 1 to Time 2, t(6)  = 2.70, p<.04, d = 1.02. Meanwhile, in the personal bonuses condition, there was no evidence for a performance improvement, t(6)  = 0.10, p = .92, d = .04 (Table 3).

Once again, it is possible to conceptualize the effectiveness of these interventions by calculating the return on investment for prosocial and personal bonuses. On sales teams, for every $10 USD given to a team member to spend on herself, the firm gets just $3 USD back – a net loss; because sales do not increase with personal bonuses, personal bonuses are wasted money. In sharp contrast, for every $10 USD given to a team member to spend prosocially, the firm reaps $52 USD.

The results of Experiments 2a and 2b are similar; teams that received prosocial bonuses outperformed teams that were given personal bonuses. These results emerged despite the logistical and statistical limitations of samples of team data. Indeed, the small sample size may explain why the effects are marginal in both experiments.

Therefore, to more accurately estimate the true effect size of prosocial bonuses on performance, we conducted a meta-analysis. Meta-analyses are frequently used to combine the results of two or more studies, allowing researchers to arrive at more accurate conclusions than can be presented in a single study [97][100]. This method is advantageous when several experiments favor the same result but fail to reach significance due to small sample size [101].

Taking this approach with our data, across Experiments 2a and 2b, we combined the effect sizes for the change from Time 1 to Time 2 performance in prosocial and personal teams. The meta-analysis revealed that prosocial teams performed significantly better from Time 1 to Time 2 as revealed by the significant Z = 2.66, 95% CI (.31, 2.02). We repeated the same analysis for the change in performance from Time 1 to Time 2 for personal teams, which revealed a nonsignificant Z = .03, 95% CI (-.67,.88). These results from the meta-analysis show that the change in performance from pre- to post-bonuses was significant in prosocial teams while not significant in personal teams.

Discussion

We offer initial evidence of the causal impact of increasing prosocial behavior via the provision of prosocial bonuses to employees at an Australian bank, members of dodge ball teams in Canada, and pharmaceutical salespeople in Belgium. Taken together, our studies show that when organizations give employees the opportunity to spend money on others – whether their co-workers or those in need – both the employees and the company can benefit, with increased happiness and job satisfaction and even improved team performance. Specifically, in Experiment 1, employees who had the opportunity to make a substantial donation to charity ($50 USD) on behalf of their company reported enhanced happiness and job satisfaction in the short term, compared to those in the control condition. In Experiments 2a and 2b, we extended these findings to team performance in the longer term, showing that teams performed better when participants were assigned to spend money on their fellow team members than when given a more standard bonus: money to spend on themselves. Across the studies, we show that prosocial bonuses can benefit both individuals and teams, on both psychological and “bottom line” indicators, in both the short and long-term. Unlike some research suggesting a weak link between factors that improve job satisfaction and those that improve job performance [102][104] our results suggest that prosocial bonuses have a meaningful impact on both metrics.

How might prosocial bonuses lead to increased happiness, job satisfaction and team performance? Because our studies were conducted in the field, we were unable to conduct extensive surveys assessing likely mediators of the impact of prosocial bonuses. While the beneficial impact of prosocial spending on happiness is well-established [62], [86], a key goal for future research is to explore underlying mechanisms of the prosocial bonus-performance link, with several clear possibilities worthy of investigation. First, prosocial bonuses may lead to the strengthening of existing relationships and even the formation of new relationships; such positive interpersonal relationships predict job engagement [45], [46] and job satisfaction [47][49]. Second, and relatedly, prosocial bonuses might lead to increased cooperation and cohesiveness between team members, which can improve team performance in part by encouraging helping behaviors [51][55]. Finally, prosocial spending may increase general feelings of reciprocity among members of organizations, leading both to greater cooperation and punishment of “shirkers” or “free riders” – those employees who are not contributing to the goals of the organization [105][110].

Along similar lines, future work should examine whether the impact of prosocial bonuses on team performance is driven by actions of the spenders, receivers, or a combination of the two. Since we were not able to measure individual performance in sales and sports teams, we could not pinpoint whether prosocial bonuses increased team performance by motivating individual-level contributions or team-level operations. Assessing individual level contributions would also allow researchers to examine how additional team members -- who were neither spenders nor receivers -- respond to this type of intervention. Future experiments that include both prosocial and personal bonuses while assessing these – and other – constructs will add to our understanding of the benefits of prosocial bonuses.

We note that Experiment 1 included a prosocial bonus condition and a control condition but not a personal bonus condition, whereas Experiments 2a and 2b included prosocial and personal bonus conditions but not a control condition; in addition, Experiment 1 included two levels of bonuses, whereas in Experiments 2a and 2b the bonus amount was kept constant. These decisions were driven by logistics. Our study sites were not interested in including a personal bonus in Experiment 1 but did allow us to include two levels of prosocial bonus; they were interested in including both personal and prosocial bonuses of a fixed amount but not a control condition in Experiments 2a and 2b. Of clear interest for future research is more systematic and comprehensive variation of all of these factors, crossing many bonus levels with both personal and prosocial bonuses. In addition, as we noted in Experiment 2, our observations at the team level are low in number (150 participants become just 25 teams across Experiments 2a and 2b); scaled-up experiments that utilized more teams would also build on the “proof of concept” experiments we present here.

It would be particularly interesting to examine employees’ sensitivity to bonus levels as a function of whether those bonuses are personal or prosocial. Receiving $10 or $20 for oneself is likely to lead only to the purchase of one or two additional coffees, and therefore seems unlikely to impact employee satisfaction or job performance. Buying a $20 gift for a coworker instead of a $10 gift, on the other hand, may encourage people to be even more creative and thoughtful in their gift choice, making the experience more impactful for both the giver and the receiver – and possibly leading to a bigger return on investment for the organization. More broadly, a $10 personal bonus from one’s organization may seem like a trifling or insufficient reward, leading to a decrease in motivation [71] – “I worked all year and they only gave me $10?” – whereas our results suggest that the same small sum of money spent prosocially has a markedly different, and positive, effect.

Related to the above, $25 USD was not sufficient to increase employee satisfaction in Experiment 1, but the meta-analysis for Experiments 2a and 2b suggests that $20 USD may be able to increase team performance. We suggest that this difference is likely due to the different form that prosocial bonuses took in the two studies. Recent research suggests that face-to-face giving has a larger impact on happiness than giving at a distance: not only are people more likely to donate money to toward single individuals than to larger organizations [111][112], but the closer the link between giver and receiver, the bigger the happiness benefits: people who give money to others are happier when they give face-to-face rather than remotely, and spending money on close friends leads to more happiness than spending on more distant acquaintances [113][114]. As a result, it is not surprising that the same amount of money (∼$20 USD) goes further in Experiments 2a and 2b than in Experiment 1, given the social nature of the team expenditure compared to the relatively impersonal donation to charity. Perhaps even more importantly, whereas in Experiment 1 employees were givers only, in Experiments 2a and 2b teammates were both givers and receivers: for every salesperson who gave a gift, there was a salesperson who received that gift, likely another contributor to the greater impact of prosocial bonuses in Experiments 2a and 2b. Importantly, the observed boost in employee satisfaction and happiness only for the $50 USD and not for the $25 USD in Study 1 helps rule out the possibility that our results are simply due to demand effects. Demand effects should have influenced both of the prosocial donation conditions (e.g., $25 USD and $50 USD) equally. Thus, if employees felt that they should be happy after giving, then the boost in happiness would have been observed across all prosocial spenders, not just for employees who gave $50.

Our experiments provide preliminary evidence for the potential utility of prosocial bonuses, though future research is needed. Given that existing incentive schemes have important drawbacks, it is worthwhile to consider creative new approaches to incentivizing employees. That said, we assume that prosocial bonuses may have drawbacks of their own, which future research should document. In particular, it seems likely that prosocial bonuses could backfire if they were introduced by companies as a replacement for more standard bonuses. Because many companies already allocate funds for charitable giving and employee entertainment, however, it may be possible for companies to reap the benefits of prosocial bonuses by providing some of these existing funds directly to employees, who can then use this money to make donations to charity or to benefit co-workers—potentially increasing job satisfaction and performance in the process. Relatedly, prosocial bonuses were unconditional in our experiments; future research could examine whether bonuses conditional on performance or based on competition would prove as effective in increasing job satisfaction and performance.

We opened by noting that recent surveys indicate that job satisfaction is at a twenty-year low in the United States even as Americans have come to spend more and more of their time at work. This additional time at work, of course, often comes at the expense of devoting time to pursuits known to be linked to well-being, from forming social connections to engaging in prosocial acts such as volunteering [2], [115][116]. We suggest that rather than force employees to make a losing tradeoff between social life and work life, employers can focus instead on using prosocial bonuses to create a more altruistic, satisfying, and productive workplace.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LA LBA MIN EWD JQ. Performed the experiments: LA LBA MIN EWD JQ. Analyzed the data: LA LBA MIN EWD JQ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LA LBA MIN EWD JQ. Wrote the paper: LA LBA MIN EWD JQ.

References

  1. 1. Conference Board (2010) I can’t get no...job satisfaction, that is: America’s unhappy workers. (Report No. 1459-09-RR).
  2. 2. Schor J (1991) The overworked American: The unexpected decline of leisure. New York: Basic Books.
  3. 3. Jurgensen CE (1978) Job preferences (what makes a job good or bad?). J Appl Psychol 63: 267–276.
  4. 4. John G, Weitz B (1989) Salesforce compensation: An empirical investigation of factors related to use of salary versus incentive compensation. J Marketing Res 26: 1–14.
  5. 5. Lazear EP (2000) Performance pay and productivity. National Bureau of Economic Research.
  6. 6. Mueller CW, Price JL (1990) Economic, psychological, and sociological determinants of voluntary turnover. J Behav Econ 19: 321–335.
  7. 7. Paarsch H, Shearer B (2000) Piece rates, fixed wages, and incentive effects, Statistical evidence from payroll records. Int Econ Rev 41: : 59 – 92.
  8. 8. Parent D (1999) Methods of pay and earnings, a longitudinal analysis. Ind Labor Relat Rev 53: 71–86.
  9. 9. Sager JK, Futrell CM, Varadarajan R (1989) Exploring salesperson turnover: A causal model. J Bus Res 18: 303–326.
  10. 10. Eisenberger R, Rhoades L, Cameron J (1999) Does pay for performance increase or decrease perceived self determination and intrinsic motivation? J Pers Soc Psychol 77: : 1026 –1040.
  11. 11. Greene CN (1973) Causal connections among managers’ merit pay, job satisfaction, and performance. J Appl Psychol 58: 95–100.
  12. 12. Miceli MP, Jung I, Near JP, Greenberger DB (1991) Predictors and outcomes of reactions to pay-for-performance plans. J Appl Psychol 76: : 508 –521.
  13. 13. Smith PL, Smits SJ, Hoy F (1992) Human resource policies in small firms, Linkages to employee satisfaction. Appl Hum Resour Manage Res 3: 1–19.
  14. 14. Ewing BT (1996) Wages and performance-base pay: Evidence from the NLSY. Econ Lett 51: 241–246.
  15. 15. Green C, Heywood JS (2008) Does performance pay increase job satisfaction? Economica 75: 710–728.
  16. 16. MacLeod WB, Parent D (1998) Job characteristics and the form of compensation. In: Polacheck S, editor. Res in Lab Econ. California: JAI Press. pp. 177–242.
  17. 17. Oettinger GS (2001) Do piece rates influence effort choices? Evidence from stadium Vendors. Econ Lett 73: 117–123.
  18. 18. Stajkovic AD, Luthans F (2001) Differential effects of incentive motivators on work performance. Acad Manage J 44: 580–590.
  19. 19. Ariely D, Gneezy U, Loewenstein G, Mazar N (2009) Large stakes and big mistakes. Rev Econ Stud 76: 451–469.
  20. 20. Oyer P (1998) Fiscal year ends and nonlinear incentive contracts: The effect on business seasonality. Quart J Econ 113: 149–185.
  21. 21. Drago R (1988) Turnbull GK (1988) Individual and group piece rates under team technologies. J Jpn Int Econ 2: 1–10.
  22. 22. Lazear EP (1989) Pay Equality and Industrial Politics. J Polit Econ 97: 561–580.
  23. 23. Bloom M (1999) The performance effects of pay dispersion on individuals and organizations. Acad Manage J 42: 25–40.
  24. 24. Gardner T (1999) When pay for performance works too well: The negative impact of pay dispersion. Acad Manage Exec 13: 101–103.
  25. 25. Grant AM, Christianson MK, Price RH (2007) Happiness, health, or relationships? Managerial practices and employee well-being tradeoffs. Acad Manage P 21: 51–63.
  26. 26. Drago R, Turnbull GK (1987) An analysis of non-insurance work incentives. Econ Lett 23: 301–304.
  27. 27. Cohen SG, Bailey DE (1997) What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research from the shop floor to the executive suite. J Manage 23: 239–290.
  28. 28. Grant AM (2007) Relational job design and the motivation to make a prosocial difference. Acad Manage Rev 32: 393–417.
  29. 29. Hamman J, Rick S, Weber RA (2007) Solving coordination failure with “all-or-none” group-level incentives. Exp Econ 10: 285–303.
  30. 30. Barry B, Crant JM (2000) Dyadic communication relationships in organizations: An attribution/expectancy approach. Organ Sci 11: 648 – 664.
  31. 31. Bradbury H, Bergmann Lichtenstein BM (2000) Relationality in organizational research: Exploring the space between. Organ Sci 11: 551–564.
  32. 32. Gersick CJG, Bartunek JM, Dutton JE (2000) Learning from academia: The importance of relationships in professional life. Acad Manage J 43: : 1026 –1044.
  33. 33. Kahn WA (1990) Psychological conditions of personal engagement and disengagement at work. Acad Manage J 33: 692–724.
  34. 34. Kahn W (1998) Relational systems at work. Res Organ Behav 20: : 39 –76.
  35. 35. Kozlowski SWJ, Bell BS (2003) Work groups and teams in organizations. In: Borman WC, Ilgen DR, Klimoski RJ, editors. Handbook of psychology: Industrial and organizational psychology. London: John Wiley & Sons Inc. pp. 333–375.
  36. 36. Wrzesniewski A, Dutton JE, Debebe G (2003) Interpersonal sensemaking and the meaning of work. Res Organ Behav 25: 93–135.
  37. 37. Adler PS, Kwon S (2002) Social capital: Prospects for a new concept. Acad Manage Rev 27: 17–40.
  38. 38. Ibarra H (1993) Personal networks of women and minorities in management: A conceptual framework. Acad Manage Rev 18: : 56 – 87.
  39. 39. Leana CR, Rousseau DM (2000) Relational wealth: The advantages of stability in a changing economy. New York: Oxford University Press.
  40. 40. Rangan S (2000) The problem of search and deliberation in economic action, when social networks really matter. Acad Manage Rev 25: : 813– 828.
  41. 41. Cunningham MR, Barbee AP (2000) Social support. In: Hendrick C, Hendrick SS, editors. Close relationships: A sourcebook California: Sage Publications Inc. pp. 273–285.
  42. 42. Langston CA (1994) Capitalizing on and coping with daily-life events, Expressive responses to positive events. J Pers Soc Psychol 67: 1112–1125.
  43. 43. Terry DJ, Nielsen M, Perchard L (1993) Effects of work stress on psychological wellbeing and job satisfaction: The stress-buffering role of coworker support. Aust J Psychol 45: 168–175.
  44. 44. Bishop JW, Scott K (2000) An examination of organizational and team commitment in a self-directed team environment. J Appl Psycol 85: 439–450.
  45. 45. Avery DR, McKay PF, Wilson DC (2007) Engaging the aging workforce: The relationship between perceived age similarity, satisfaction with coworkers, and employee engagement. J Appl Psychol 92: 1542–1556.
  46. 46. Heaphy ED, Dutton JE (2008) Positive social interactions and the human body at work: Linking organizations and physiology. Acad Manage Rev 33: 137–162.
  47. 47. Chiaburu D, Harrison D (2008) Do peers make the place? Conceptual synthesis and meta-analysis of coworker effects on perceptions, attitudes, OCBs, and performance J Appl Psychol 93: 1082–1103.
  48. 48. Morrison R (2004) Information relationships in the workplace, Association with job satisfaction, organizational commitment and turnover intentions. New Zeal J Psychol 33: 114–128.
  49. 49. Simon LS, Judge TA, Erez A (2008) Capitalizing on positive events at work, The impact on positive events on mood and job satisfaction. In: Ilies R, Fandre J, chairs. Effects of work experiences on behavior and well-being Explanatory mechanisms. Symposium conducted at the annual meeting of the Society for Industrial & Organizational Psychology. San Francisco, CA.
  50. 50. Karau SJ, Williams KD (1997) The effects of group cohesiveness on social loafing and social compensation. Group Dynam 1: 156–168.
  51. 51. Boning B, Ichniowski C, Shaw K (2007) Opportunity Counts: Teams and the effectiveness of production incentives. J Labor Econ 25: 613–50.
  52. 52. Itoh H (1991) Incentives to help in multi-agent situations. Econometrica 59: 611–636.
  53. 53. Itoh H (1992) Cooperation in hierarchical organizations, an incentive perspective. J Law Econ Organ 8: 321–345.
  54. 54. Itoh H (1993) Job design and incentives in hierarchies with team production. Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan: Mimeo.
  55. 55. Wageman R, Baker G (1997) Incentives and cooperation, the joint effects of task and reward interdependence on group performance. J Organ Behav 18: 139–158.
  56. 56. Holmstrom B (1979) Moral hazard and observability. Bell J Econ 10: 74–91.
  57. 57. DeMatteo JS, Eby LTL, Sandstrom E (1998) Team-Based Rewards, Current Empirical Evidence and Directions for Future Research. Res Organ Behav 20: 141–183.
  58. 58. Mohrman SA, Lawler EE, Mohrman AM (1992) Applying employee involvement in schools. Educ Eval Policy An 14: 347–360.
  59. 59. Warneken F, Tomasello M (2006) Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 311: 1301–1303.
  60. 60. Weinstein N, Ryan RM (2010) When helping helps: An examination of motivational constructs underlying prosocial behavior and their influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. J Pers Soc Psychol 98 222–224.
  61. 61. Andreoni J (1990) Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm-glow giving. Econ J 100: 464–477.
  62. 62. Dunn EW, Aknin LB, Norton MI (2008) Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319: 1687–1688.
  63. 63. Bateman TS, Organ DW (1983) Job satisfaction and the good soldier: The relationship between affect and employee “citizenship”. Acad Manage J 26: 587–595.
  64. 64. Grant AM (2008) Designing jobs to do good: Dimensions and psychological consequences of prosocial job characteristics. J Positive Psychol 3: 19–39.
  65. 65. Harrison DA, Newman DA, Roth PL (2006) How important are job attitudes? Meta-analytic comparisons of integrative behavioral outcomes and time sequences. Acad Manage J 49: 305–325.
  66. 66. Meyer JP, Stanley DJ, Herscovitch L, Topolnytsky L (2002) Affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization: A meta-analysis of antecedents, correlates, and consequences. J Vocat Behav 61: 20–52.
  67. 67. Gneezy U, Meier S, Rey-Biel P (2011) When and how incentives (don’t) work to modify behavior? J Econ Perspect 25: 1–21.
  68. 68. Gneezy U, Rustichini A (2000) Pay enough or don’t pay at all. Q J Econ 115: 791–810.
  69. 69. Ariely D, Bracha A, Meier S (2009) Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. Am Econ Rev 99: 544–555.
  70. 70. Heyman J, Ariely D (2004) Effort for payment: A tale of two markets. Psychol Sci 15: 787–793.
  71. 71. Goldstein NJ, Griskevicius V, Cialdini RB (2011) Reciprocity by proxy: A novel influence strategy for stimulating cooperation. Admin Sci Quart 56: 441–473.
  72. 72. Judge TA, Klinger R (2008) Job satisfaction: Subjective well-being at work. In: Eid M, Larsen R, editors. The Science of Subjective Well-Being. New York: Guilford Publications. pp. 393–413.
  73. 73. Locke EA (1976) The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In: Dunnette MD, editor. Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Chicago: Rand-McNally. pp. 1319–1328.
  74. 74. Bruck CS, Allen TD, Spector PE (2002) The relationship between work–family conflict and job satisfaction: A finer-grained analysis. J Vocat Behav 60: 336–353.
  75. 75. Chen G, Casper WJ, Cortina JM (2001) The roles of self-efficacy and task complexity in the relationships among cognitive ability, conscientiousness, and task performance: A meta-analytic examination. Hum Perform 14: 209–230.
  76. 76. Chen G, Gully SM, Eden D (2001) Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale. Org Res Method 4 62–83.
  77. 77. Judge TA, Bono JE (2001) Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction and job performance, a meta analysis. J Appl Psychol 86: 80–92.
  78. 78. Weaver CN (1980) Job satisfaction in the United States in the 1970s. J Appl Psychol 65: 364–367.
  79. 79. Bedeian AG, Armenakis AA (1981) A path-analysis experiment of the consequences of role conflict and ambiguity. Acad Manage J 24: 95–112.
  80. 80. Fried Y, Ferris GR (1987) The validity of the job characteristics model: A review and meta-analysis. Personnel Psychol 40: : 287–322.
  81. 81. Gross N, Mason WS, McEachern AW (1958) Explorations in role analysis. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  82. 82. Hackman RJ, Lawler EE III (1971) Employee reactions to job characteristics. J Appl Psychol 55: 259–286.
  83. 83. Haynes K (1979) Job satisfaction of mid-management social workers. Admin Soc Work 3: 207–217.
  84. 84. Rizzo JR, House RJ, Lirtzman SI (1970) Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations. Admin Sci Quart 15: 150–163.
  85. 85. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 54: 1063–1070.
  86. 86. Dunn EW, Ashton-James CE, Hanson MD, Aknin LB (2010) On the costs of self-interested economic behavior: How does stinginess get under the skin? J Health Psychol 15: 627–633.
  87. 87. Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade D, Schwarz N, Stone AA (2006) Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science 312: 1908–1910.
  88. 88. Oishi S, Kesebir S, Diener E (2011) Income inequality and happiness. Psychol Sci 22: 1095–1100.
  89. 89. Cammann C, Fichman M, Jenkins GD Jr, Klesh JR (1983) Assessing the attitudes and perceptions of organizational members. In: Lawler EE III, Mirvis PH, Cammann C, Seashore S, editors. Assessing organizational change: A guide to methods, measures, and practices. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc. pp. 71–138.
  90. 90. Ironson GH, Smith PC, Brannick MT, Gibson WM, Paul KB (1989) Construction of a job in general scale: A comparison of global, composite, and specific measures. J Appl Psychol 74: 193–200.
  91. 91. Kunin T (1955) The construction of a new type of attitude measure. Personnel Psychol 8: 65–78.
  92. 92. Wanous JP, Reichers AE, Hudy MJ (1997) Overall job satisfaction: How good are single-item measures? J Appl Psychol 82: 247–252.
  93. 93. Judge TA, Bono JE (2000) Five-factor model of personality and transformational leadership. J Appl Psychol 85: 751–765.
  94. 94. Schmidt FL, Hunter J (2004) General mental ability in the world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. J Pers Soc Psychol 86: 162–173.
  95. 95. Vinchur A, Schippmann J, Switzer F, Roth P (1998) A meta-analytical review of predictors of job performance for sales people. J Appl Psychol 82: 30–43.
  96. 96. Wright TA, Cropanzano R (2000) Psychological well-being and job satisfaction as predictors of job performance. J Occup Health Psychol 5: 84–94.
  97. 97. Cook TD, Leviton LC (1980) Reviewing the literature: A comparison of traditional methods with meta-analysis1. J Pers 48: 449–472.
  98. 98. Rosenthal R, DiMatteo MR (2001) Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annu Rev Psychol 52: 59–82.
  99. 99. Smith ML, Glass GV, Miller TI (1980) The benefits of psychotherapy. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
  100. 100. Twenge JM, Catanese KR, Baumeister RF (2002) Social exclusion causes self-defeating behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 83: 606–615.
  101. 101. Rosenthal R (1991) Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Sage.
  102. 102. Brayfield AH, Crockett WH (1955) Employee attitudes and employee performance. Psychol Bull 52: 396–424.
  103. 103. Iaffaldano MT, Muchinsky PM (1985) Job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 97: 251–273.
  104. 104. Judge TA, Thoresen CJ, Bono JE, Patton GK (2001) The job satisfaction job performance relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychol Bull 127: 376–407.
  105. 105. Akerlof GA, Kranton RE (2005) Identity and the economics of organizations. J Econ Perspect 19: 9–32.
  106. 106. Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ (2003) Evolution of altruistic punishment. P Natl Acad Sci 100 3531–3535.
  107. 107. Carpenter J (2007) Punishing free-riders: How group size affects mutual monitoring and the provision of public goods. Game Econ Behav 60: 31–51.
  108. 108. Carpenter J, Bowles S, Gintis H, Hwang S (2009) Strong reciprocity and team production. J Econ Behav Organ 71: 221–232.
  109. 109. Fehr E, Gächter S (2000) Cooperation and punishment. Am Econ Rev 90: 980–994.
  110. 110. Fuster A, Meier S (2010) Another hidden cost of incentives: The detrimental effect on norm enforcement. Manage Sci 56: 57–70.
  111. 111. Kogut T, Ritov I (2007) One of us: Outstanding willingness to help save a single identified compatriot. Organ Behav Hum Dec 104: 150–157.
  112. 112. Small DA, Loewenstein G (2003) Helping a victim or helping the victim: Altruism and identifiability. J Risk Uncertainty 26: 5–16.
  113. 113. Aknin LB, Dunn EW, Sandstrom GM, Norton MI (2012) Putting the “social” in prosocial spending: Social contact as a catalyst for turning good deeds into good feelings. In press.
  114. 114. Aknin LB, Sandstrom GM, Dunn EW, Norton MI (2011) It's the recipient that counts: Spending money on strong social ties leads to greater happiness than spending on weak social ties. PLoS ONE 6: e17018.
  115. 115. Putnam R (2000) Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American Community. New York: Simon and Schuster.
  116. 116. Thoits PA, Hewitt LN (2001) Volunteer work and well-being. J Health Soc Behav 42: : 115–.