Peer Review History
Original SubmissionJanuary 24, 2022 |
---|
PONE-D-22-02319Contrasting speed and accuracy approaches to measure executive functions in three low-and middle-income countriesPLOS ONE Dear Dr. Wray, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. You will see from the reviewers' comments that there are some concerns relating to methodology (mainly) as well as some clarity / depth of information on more conceptual issues. Please consider the comments carefully and make sure that you also revise the manuscript, following any changes to data / statistical analysis, to ensure any changes in the findings are appropriately reflected and discussed in the manuscript throughout. Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 02 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter. If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Theodoros M. Bampouras Academic Editor PLOS ONE Journal requirements: When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. 1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 2. Please include a complete copy of PLOS’ questionnaire on inclusivity in global research in your revised manuscript. Our policy for research in this area aims to improve transparency in the reporting of research performed outside of researchers’ own country or community. The policy applies to researchers who have travelled to a different country to conduct research, research with Indigenous populations or their lands, and research on cultural artefacts. The questionnaire can also be requested at the journal’s discretion for any other submissions, even if these conditions are not met. Please find more information on the policy and a link to download a blank copy of the questionnaire here: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/best-practices-in-research-reporting. Please upload a completed version of your questionnaire as Supporting Information when you resubmit your manuscript. 3. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript: “We would like to thank all of the participants in this study. This study was funded by the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation (OPP1164115).” We note that you have provided additional information within the Acknowledgements Section that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. Please note that funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form. Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows: “This study was funded by the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation (OPP1164115). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.” Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf. 4. One of the noted authors is a group or consortium [COHORTS]. In addition to naming the author group, please list the individual authors and affiliations within this group in the acknowledgments section of your manuscript. Please also indicate clearly a lead author for this group along with a contact email address 5. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please delete it from any other section. Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Partly Reviewer #2: Partly ********** 2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: Thank you for the opportunity to review this very interesting manuscript. There has been increased attention to the use of EF tasks in low- and middle-income countries as well as to different methods of EF task scoring that jointly utilize accuracy and speed. In this manuscript, the authors compare two different scoring methods: RT difference scores and NIH Toolbox two-vector scores. 1. Why did the authors decide to only compare these two types of scoring methods? Recently, latent variable models that make joint use of accuracy and RT data have been proposed (e.g., Camerota, Willoughby, Magnus, & Blair, 2020; Magnus, Willoughby, Blair, & Kuhn, 2017). These seem like they could be particularly useful given the range of accuracy across the three countries studied. 2. Have the NIH Toolbox EF tasks been validated for use in low- and middle-income countries? This seems important to note for this study, especially since some of the participants in Guatemala seemed to misunderstand the task directions. 3. It seems like some of the weaknesses of the difference scores could be remedied by log transforming reaction time prior to calculating the scores. The authors note that the NIH Toolbox does perform a log transformation prior to calculating scores. Doing so for the difference scores would help make a fairer comparison of the two methods, especially given the deviations from normality observed for the difference scores. 4. It might help the reader to call the difference scores “Flanker Conflict Score” and “DCCS Switch Score” as I had a hard time remembering which task the conflict and switch scores were from. 5. In the attrition section (page 12), the authors list the number of participants for whom scores could not be calculated. It would be helpful to show these as percentages of the full samples. Similarly, for the statement on page 13 that “nearly all participants who scores seven…” it would be helpful to know how many scored 7 overall (raw number and percentage of the cohort) and of these, how many scored zero on the shape trials (raw number and percentage). 6. The DCCS switch score was positively correlated with the DCCS computed score in Guatemala and Philippines but negatively correlated in the South African cohort. Why would this be? Also why is the Flanker conflict score negatively correlated with RSPM scores while the DCCS conflict score is positively correlated? This seems to suggest that these two difference scores are telling us different things. 7. The summary of results on page 15 does not match the tables. Non-verbal IQ was associated with switch scores in Guatemala and the Philippines, but not in South Africa. Reviewer #2: Tests of executive functions (EF) are most frequently evaluated based on different scores describing reaction time and/or accuracy of performances. Results and interpretations depend on the chosen scoring method. The presented study evaluates two scoring methods of inhibition and cognitive flexibility assessed via the NIH Toolbox across three large-scale samples from different countries. Results highlight differences in these methods and possible implications are discussed. The right choice of measurement is an important issue researchers face when conducting studies and evaluating data. Therefore, comparing strengths and weaknesses of possible scoring methods can prove valuable for future studies. However, I find the focus of the presented comparison to miss critical aspects enabling that choice and have a few additional concerns. Major Points: 1. I find the introduction and discussion of the scores regarding their theoretical and specific cognitive implications to be a little scarce. Given that the study’s aim is to compare the suitability of scores across populations the theoretical conclusions enabled based on both kinds of scores should also be considered. This is touched upon in one paragraph of the discussion but could be further expanded and introduced. Considerations about conclusions regarding cognitive functionality, etc. provide valuable information to researchers since the study of interindividual differences might not always be the focus for applications. 2. I do not fully understand the reasoning behind the correlation between scores and non-verbal IQ. EFs are linked to general intelligence. However, I would not use an associated construct, i.e., intelligence, to examine whether scores validly reflect a different construct, i.e., EF, as the authors seem to conclude. 3. The results of the Guatemala cohort, as the authors hypothesize themselves, are likely due to participants performing the task wrong. For one sixth of participants the switch cost score could not be computed. Solely based on the computation criteria of the scores the DCCS Computed Score can still be calculated. However, I am unsure whether this should be the case since results reflect performance in a different task, i.e., there is no switching component. Results in and interpretations about this cohort are thus substantially influenced by a group of participants who did not perform the task. Maybe as control, results without this subgroup should be considered. Minor Points: 1. A suggestion related to my first major point: A key aspect of the study is the benefit provided by scores including both accuracy and speed information rather than just one of the above. A lot of the differences found between scores in this study can be boiled down to this distinction. Although the chosen scoring methods are examples of both cases, I feel that the discussion could benefit from a more general discussion of combined speed and accuracy measures. 2. Although the sample it described elsewhere a little more information like mean/ median age and SD or gender distributions would be informative. 3. The descriptions of the task procedures are rather unspecific. For instance, in the Flanker task, where are the icons presented and are they mapped to different hands? In the DCCS, is stimulus presentation sequentially? 4. Table 1 is missing information on which information is presented in parentheses (SD/ SEM/ IQR)? 5. The bins of the histograms of Figures 2 and 3 for the difference scores differ between cohorts and seem coarse given that they present reaction time data which often depict and find much smaller differences on the level of milliseconds. 6. The labels of the graphs describing the difference scores could reflect more clearly what is depicted, i.e., difference between respective conditions in seconds. 7. Could you explain in more detail how the reaction times are rescaled in the computed scores/ state more clearly which score from 0-5 represents which direction of reaction times? I was a little confused by the inversion appearing in the discussion (0 = slow, 5 = fast) ********** 6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes: Lukas Recker ********** [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 1 |
PONE-D-22-02319R1Contrasting speed and accuracy approaches to measure executive functions in three low-and middle-income countriesPLOS ONE Dear Dr. Wray, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. You will see that although the reviewers were largely satisfied with the amendments to the manuscript, there are still some methodological points to be considered and addressed, before the manuscript is recommended for publication. I would, therefore encourage you to consider them carefully and address them accordingly. Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 16 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Theodoros M. Bampouras Academic Editor PLOS ONE Journal Requirements: Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice. [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation. Reviewer #1: (No Response) Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed ********** 2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Partly Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: N/A ********** 4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 6. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: The authors have been mostly responsive to my comments. One area where they were not is in response to my comment #6. The DCCS switch score and DCCS computed score are positively correlated in two cohorts (Guatemala, Philippines) but negatively correlated in South Africa. The authors replied that the negative association in South Africa was not statistically significant. However, it is marked with an asterisk in Table 5 (-.06*) indicating that the negative association was significant. The authors need to resolve this discrepancy and provide additional explanation for why the scores may have different associations across contexts. I also disagreed with their response to Reviewer 2, point 3. They claim that participants who scored '7' were not completing the task wrong. However, sorting by color on all trials IS performing the task incorrectly and displays a misunderstanding of the task instructions. This is especially apparent since there was no variability in performance (i.e., all except 1 participant got all repeat trials incorrect). I agree that this necessitates a need for sensitivity analyses excluding these participants. In the discussion, when describing the participants who scored 5 points on the Flanker, the authors state that this means the participants responded slowly but accurately. This raises a point worth mentioning that RT scores are only useful if all participants are following the same instructions (i.e., "respond as quickly as you can") -- which is hard to determine. This is problematic whether or not you are using RT difference scores or combined accuracy/RT scores and may be a bigger issues in cultures where accuracy is considered to be more important than speed. Another limitation worth mentioning when comparing NIH toolbox computed scores to latent variable scores is that NIH Toolbox weighs each item that goes into the vector score equally, whereas a latent variable score would empirically determine how strongly to weight each item. This could lead to more precise quantification of individual differences. Reviewer #2: (No Response) ********** 7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: No ********** [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 2 |
Contrasting speed and accuracy approaches to measure executive functions in three low-and middle-income countries PONE-D-22-02319R2 Dear Dr. Wray, We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements. Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication. An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org. Kind regards, Theodoros M. Bampouras Academic Editor PLOS ONE Additional Editor Comments (optional): Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation. Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed ********** 2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: (No Response) ********** 3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: (No Response) ********** 4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: (No Response) ********** 5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: (No Response) ********** 6. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: (No Response) ********** 7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No ********** |
Formally Accepted |
PONE-D-22-02319R2 Contrasting speed and accuracy approaches to measure executive functions in three low-and middle-income countries Dear Dr. Wray: I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org. If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org. Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access. Kind regards, PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff on behalf of Dr. Theodoros M. Bampouras Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Open letter on the publication of peer review reports
PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.
We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.
Learn more at ASAPbio .