Peer Review History
Original SubmissionJune 17, 2019 |
---|
PONE-D-19-17118 Mobile phones: The effect of its presence on learning and memory PLOS ONE Dear Dr. Yong, , Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Your study addresses an interesting question about the impact of mobile phones on memory. One area that raised o concerns was your assessment of phone conscious thought. First you need to provide a clear conceptual definition of this construct and also your rationale for how to assess it. In the discussion you seem to imply that phone conscious thought is measuring separation anxiety while there was no assessment of anxiety. Also what is the rationale for measuring affect before and after the memory assessment/? This point needs to be clarified. There are also concerns about the analysis of mood changes before and after the memory assessment. These analyses need to be described more clearly. Both reviewers raised concerns about your design in terms of your control group. You need to acknowledge the limitations of your design in the discussion and discuss how it limits your theoretical interpretation. Overall much more care must be given to the writing of the manuscript. Reviewer 1 has pointed out numerous examples of how the writing could be improved or clarified. You must address all points raised by both reviewers in your revised manuscript. We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by . October 18 2019. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Barbara Dritschel, PhD Academic Editor PLOS ONE Journal Requirements: 1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf 2. Please ensure that you have included a section on statistical analysis in your Methods section. 3. Thank you for your ethics statement : "This study was approved by the Department of Psychology Ethics Committee (20171090)." Please amend your current ethics statement to include the full name of the ethics committee/institutional review board(s) that approved your specific study. Once you have amended this/these statement(s) in the Methods section of the manuscript, please add the same text to the “Ethics Statement” field of the submission form (via “Edit Submission”). For additional information about PLOS ONE ethical requirements for human subjects research, please refer to http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research. [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Partly Reviewer #2: Partly ********** 2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: No ********** 5. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: The present study examined the mnemonic consequences associated with the presence of a smartphone. Overall, the authors found that participants without their cellphones had higher accuracy scores than those who had their cell phones present. They also found a negative correlation between accuracy and "phone conscious thought." Overall, I think this is an interesting area of research. However, the following issues need to be addressed before I can recommend publication. I will start with the larger issues before moving to the smaller issues: Larger issues -Probably the biggest issue I found was the interpretation of their results. For example, on pg. 17, the authors state that "Although we did not find a significant relationship between SAS to memory accuracy, our measurements to 'phone conscious thought' is more relevant and meaningful because it measured participants separation anxiety..." This simply cannot be true: First, the question representing" phone conscious thought" asks "During the memory test how often do you think of your smartphone?" What does this even mean, exactly? How did participants interpret this question? Either way, I think it is quite a stretch to consider this anxiety. And, second, the SAS included a "'Withdrawal' sub-factor [that] describ[ed] the feeling of anxiety when separated from one's mobile phone." (pg. 9), but the authors found no significant correlations for any of the subfactors. Thus, not sure how a vague question about thoughts better represents anxiety than the specify subfactor of the SAS. -Additionally, the authors suggestion that the decrease in positive affect is the result of "prohibited usage/or separation from their phone" (pg. 18). But the authors have no data to support this. For all they know, the participants had a decrease in positive affect simply because they were participating in a study since both groups exhibited this. -In terms of the procedure, I'm a little concerned that only the "HS group" were told "no phone use." Obviously, I get the logic of this given that the phone was present for them but not for the "LS group." However, this could be a significant confound. Indeed, this could have drawn the participants attention to the fact that they couldn't use it and, in turn, could have distracted them, not simply because it was present but because of the fact that they were told they couldn't use their phone. -Additionally, did the authors run any preliminary analysed based on how many participants were in each group when they participated? Given the importance just the mere presence of a cell is for the present study, the present of others could have influenced their results as well. Smaller Issues: -How is the reader supposed to know what "phone conscious though" means in the abstract? -Pg. 2, Lines 13-14: A citation is needed to support this. -Pg. 2 and throughout: "e.g." and "i.e." should only be used in parentheses. Otherwise, it should be "for example" and "that is" respectively and should always have commas around them. -Pg. 2, Line 19: "Undoubtedly, the constant connectivity is applauded and desired..." This is way too editorial. -Pg. 3, Line 38: Describe what the "digit cancelation task" is -Pg. 3, Lines 41-42: "a mobile phone or a phone-sized notebook placed on participant's table before complete the tasks." Is not a complete sentence. -Pg. 3, Line 42: "...showed no significant on..." Awkward. Reword -Pg. 3, Line 43: Insert "the" between "during" and "simple" -Pg. 3, Line 52: "in" should be "on" (there are a lot of typos throughout. I won't highlight them all, but a careful proofreading is necessary -Pg. 3, Lines 54 & 57: Why do the authors provide the citation number to Ward et al., at the second instance and not the first? -Pg. 4, Lines 73-78: I think all those sentences could be integrated and stated much more succinctly -Pg. 5, Line 89 and throughout: The authors use the term "memory" throughout. However, there are many different types of memory. They should specify what they mean exactly by "memory" at each instance. -Pg. 5, Prior to "present study": I think the authors could do a better job of more explicitly stating what gap in the literature the their study will fill. -Results: Generally speaking, all t-tests should include cohen's d -Pg. 7, Line 138: "begun" should be "began." -Pg. 8, Line 153 & 161: Technically, the 5 should be spelled out. However, at the very least, keep it consistent. That is, the authors us 5 and spell out six. -Smartphone addiction Scale: Many of the sentences in this section have errors and need to be fixed. Additionally, the authors use "secondly" on line 167, but there's no "first" and there's no "third," etc... Also, examples of each of the sub-factors should be included. -Pg. 10: Some of this should be in the materials, not the procedure. -Pg. 11, Gender: Why not include this analysis as a preliminary analysis. If gender, alternatively, is an important issue, then is should be set up as such in the lit review and the authors should examine the interaction with an F-test. -Pg. 12, Lines 215-220: This should be a preliminary analysis. There's no reason to expect a difference between the groups assuming they were assigned randomly -Pg. 13 (and elsewhere): The authors sometimes repeat the question in the results. This isn't needed. It's redundant. -Pg. 14: Why didn't the authors run an ANOVA to examine for an interaction between mood change and condition? -Pg. 15: More information is needed in terms of the variables included in the model. -Pg. 18: There are no studies suggested under "Further Studies." The closest is a meaningless sentence: "Future studies should look into the online learning environment." -Pgs. 18-19: "These behaviors are likely to remain the same when students graduate and move into the workforce." Can the authors provide a citation to back this up or what are the authors basing this on? -Pg. 19, Lines 327-330: I don't understand this sentence or example... -Pg. 19, Lines 342-343: "...the extent of the device purpose..." is awkward sounding. Overall, many typos and awkward phrases. A careful proofreading is necessary. Reviewer #2: 1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? • How was sample size determined? Seems arbitrary, with no power analysis. • The addition of “phone-conscious thought” is a construct that does not seem to be validated in the peer reviewed literature. It’s ok to include this, but the methods behind the development of these questions should be clearly stated, and the authors must define this construct. There are some problems with how it is defined, because the question used relies specifically on phone-related thoughts during the task, while the phone is either in their presence (HS) or absent (LS). So, this question appears to serve as more of a manipulation check rather than a true measurement of phone-conscious thought. There are many issues with the construct of “phone-conscious thought” in the current manuscript. • Why is affect measured both before and after the memory test? Explain the rationale. Is the memory test expected to influence mood in any way? • The inclusion of the phone-conscious thought question in the beginning of the study may have primed participants to think about their phones more overall, and this may have inflated the differences between the LS and HS groups. • There should have been a 3rd control group where participants were given no instruction about what to do with their phone. This would help assess whether the LS group experienced lower recall or if the HS group experienced higher recall, relative to baseline. *2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? • Results for the affect/mood changes are very unclear and should be edited to be more precise. Needs to be much more descriptive. *3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? Yes *4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? The writing is unclear at times with strange vocabulary choices (e.g. “Undoubtedly, the constant connectivity is applauded and desired but this has also spiralled into an obsession with the device for many individuals” lines 19-20). What do the authors mean by “applauded and desired”? Further, writing around the explanations of the relevant literature is imprecise and should be cleaned up so that no previous findings can be mischaracterized. Requires rigorous editing to be publishable, in my opinion. Lines 57-58: In which direction? And in which tasks? All of them? Needs much greater precision. ********** 6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: No [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 1 |
PONE-D-19-17118R1 Mobile phones: The effect of its presence on learning and memory PLOS ONE Dear DR. Yong, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. I am very sorry for the delay in getting a decision for you. It was difficult to get reviewers for your paper. However I now have the response of two reviewers. The first reviewer thought that your changes made the paper much better. However there is still a fundamental question about what your paper is addressing. The second reviewer raised this point as well and made an excellent suggestion that you need to discuss what mechanisms may explain your results and describe how they might be investigated. You also need to work more on the overall writing style and make sure that the grammar is correct. If you feel that you can address these issues please submit a revised version of the paper.Please note Reviewer One's points about the role of emotion and discuss how you might investigate its role in a future design as well as why that would be important. Address all comments raised by the reviewers in your revision or justify why you are not addressing them. We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by April 30 2020. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Barbara Dritschel, PhD Academic Editor PLOS ONE [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation. Reviewer #1: (No Response) Reviewer #3: (No Response) ********** 2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #3: Partly ********** 3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #3: Yes ********** 4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #3: Yes ********** 5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #3: Yes ********** 6. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: Overall, I congratulate the authors on the revisions they've made already. The paper is much better for it. However, I still have some concerns. Most notably, as far as I can tell, the contribution this manuscript makes to the literature is in the inclusion of emotion. Indeed, the authors make this point quite clear in their "Present Study" section. Indeed, they state "But it is uncertain whether one's current positive or negative affective/mood states plays a bigger role on cognitive function..." but this doesn't seem to cohere very well with the rest of the paper. Barely anything is mentioned about emotion in the intro/lit. review (till the very end). In the analyses, the stats including emotion seem almost like an afterthought. Additionally, emotion is barely mentioned in the discussion. I realize that the authors found no statistical difference across groups, and therefore don't focus on them, but that raises another possible issue: interpreting a null result. If the primary motivation for this study was emotion, it seems to me that one would devise a different design whereby you also manipulate emotion and then examine the different conditions in terms of mobile phone salience. Thus, at the heart of it, the present paper replicates prior research and then finds a null effect for their primary research question, making interpretations difficult. For these reasons, I, unfortunately, am recommending rejection. For the authors reference moving forward, the paper was still a bit hard to parse in places due to language issues throughout. I know the authors state that a native English speaker proofread it, but more diligent proofreading is needed in the future. Reviewer #3: The experiment presented in this paper is aimed at primarily investigating whether the salience of a phone (high vs. low) impacts memory accuracy. It is a fairly straightforward experimental design and set of results. My main concern is that the paper lacks a clear mechanism to explain the results. Is the main result (i.e., HS leads to lower memory accuracy than LS) due to the fact that high salience participants are distracted during encoding? Is it due to retrieval deficits? Do they not consolidate the information properly? Is it evidence of a bandwidth effect, by which phone-related thought intrusion interferes with memory processes? My sense is that not only that the experimental design did not attempt to answer the question mechanistically, but there is no attempt to theoretically scaffold the results in a potential mechanism. I would advise the authors to at least speculate as to what could explains the set of results they obtained and to hint at possible investigation of the mechanism involved. ********** 7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #3: No [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 2 |
PONE-D-19-17118R2 Mobile phones: The effect of its presence on learning and memory PLOS ONE Dear Dr. Yong, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. I and another reviewer have carefully read your paper and the revisions that you made. You have addressed many of the points but a few more changes need to be implemented before it can be accepted. The main issues concern the introduction. One phrase in Line 62 has no verb and therefore can not be a sentence. You need to correct the grammatical structure. Secondly it would be good if the introductory sentence to the paragraph beginning on line 61 indicated the relationship between cognition and affect is important for understanding the impact of mobile phone use on memory. As it is written the paragraph does link well to the previous paragraphs. Further line 82 makes reference to smart phone addiction very briefly but the discussion focuses a great deal on smart phone addiction. You need to define smart phone addiction and indicate why it is important to examine this construct in your study. Further you need to define the subscales in of the SAS in the methods and also justify under phone consious thought why you are including this question. Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 09 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter. If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Barbara Dritschel, PhD Academic Editor PLOS ONE [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation. Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed ********** 2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #3: Partly ********** 3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #3: Yes ********** 4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #3: No ********** 5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #3: Yes ********** 6. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #3: The authors addressed my comments satisfactorily, so I recommend acceptance for the manuscript. I doubt, though, that anxiety plays a huge role in these dynamics, given that it is hard to imagine that one would be able to create the levels of anxiety necessary for the disruption of cognitive function by simply temporarily removing their phones. ********** 7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #3: No [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 3 |
Mobile phones: The effect of its presence on learning and memory PONE-D-19-17118R3 Dear Dr. Yong, We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements. Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication. An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org. Kind regards, Barbara Dritschel, PhD Academic Editor PLOS ONE Additional Editor Comments (optional): Reviewers' comments: |
Formally Accepted |
PONE-D-19-17118R3 Mobile phones: The effect of its presence on learning and memory Dear Dr. Yong: I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org. If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org. Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access. Kind regards, PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff on behalf of Dr. Barbara Dritschel Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Open letter on the publication of peer review reports
PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.
We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.
Learn more at ASAPbio .