Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Development of body, head and brain features in the Australian fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata; Marsupialia: Dasyuridae); A postnatal model of forebrain formation

  • Rodrigo Suárez ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Rodrigo Suárez, Annalisa Paolino

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    rsuarezsaa@gmail.com (RS); richards@uq.edu.au (LJR)

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

  • Annalisa Paolino ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Rodrigo Suárez, Annalisa Paolino

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

  • Peter Kozulin,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

  • Laura R. Fenlon,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

  • Laura R. Morcom,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

  • Robert Englebright,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, School of Agriculture & Food Science, Gatton, Queensland, Australia

  • Patricia J. O’Hara,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, School of Agriculture & Food Science, Gatton, Queensland, Australia

  • Peter J. Murray,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The University of Queensland, School of Agriculture & Food Science, Gatton, Queensland, Australia

  • Linda J. Richards

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    rsuarezsaa@gmail.com (RS); richards@uq.edu.au (LJR)

    Affiliations The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia, The University of Queensland, School of Biomedical Sciences, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

Abstract

Most of our understanding of forebrain development comes from research of eutherian mammals, such as rodents, primates, and carnivores. However, as the cerebral cortex forms largely prenatally, observation and manipulation of its development has required invasive and/or ex vivo procedures. Marsupials, on the other hand, are born at comparatively earlier stages of development and most events of forebrain formation occur once attached to the teat, thereby permitting continuous and non-invasive experimental access. Here, we take advantage of this aspect of marsupial biology to establish and characterise a resourceful laboratory model of forebrain development: the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata), a mouse-sized carnivorous Australian marsupial. We present an anatomical description of the postnatal development of the body, head and brain in dunnarts, and provide a staging system compatible with human and mouse developmental stages. As compared to eutherians, the orofacial region develops earlier in dunnarts, while forebrain development is largely protracted, extending for more than 40 days versus ca. 15 days in mice. We discuss the benefits of fat-tailed dunnarts as laboratory animals in studies of developmental biology, with an emphasis on how their accessibility in the pouch can help address new experimental questions, especially regarding mechanisms of brain development and evolution.

Introduction

The six-layered cerebral cortex, also known as the isocortex, is a region of the forebrain only present in mammals, and participates in daily functions such as sensory integration, motor planning, attention and learning. Comparative studies of brain development across species have provided critical insights not only into the mechanisms of circuit formation, but also the conservation and change of such mechanisms during evolution. However, a critical challenge in comparative neuroscience is to determine whether and how developmental processes in one species can be compared to those of another species. Direct comparisons of development between species are often obscured by differences in the relative rates of trait formation (heterochrony). For example, the three major mammalian groups, i.e., monotremes, marsupials and eutherians, show remarkable heterochronies in face, forelimb and brain formation that likely reflect differences in pre- and postnatal development [111]. Therefore, to facilitate our understanding of mechanistic and evolutionary questions about mammalian development, we need to integrate the timing of developmental events in different species into a shared scale.

Previous efforts to categorise development as discrete series have helped overcome this issue by establishing comparable stages across model organisms [4, 1215]. Here, we characterise postnatal development of the Australian marsupial fat-tailed dunnart, Sminthopsis crassicaudata (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae), using a standardised staging system that matches human and mouse development (i.e., the Carnegie and Thieler staging systems, respectively). Notably, while most of the neurons and connections of the cerebral cortices form before birth in eutherian species, in marsupials this occurs almost exclusively after birth and during a prolonged period of time, thus offering experimental access to early events of forebrain development in vivo [6, 7, 1020]. We outline our protocols for the breeding and care of this species in captivity to facilitate its use as a laboratory model of developmental biology, and identify its major developmental milestones based on whole body, craniofacial and brain anatomy features across stages, from birth to weaning. Finally, we compare dunnart development with that of other mammals and highlight the advantages of using this species as an experimental model to study specific events of forebrain development in vivo.

Materials and methods

Animal welfare

All animal procedures, including laboratory breeding, were approved by The University of Queensland Animal Ethics Committee and the Queensland Government Department of Environment and Heritage Protection, and were performed according to the current Australian Code for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes (NHMRC, 8th edition, 2013), as well as international guidelines on animal welfare.

Breeding colony

We established a breeding colony of fat-tailed dunnarts (Sminthopsis crassicaudata) at the Native Wildlife Teaching and Research Facility, The University of Queensland, from founder animals sourced from captive colonies at the University of Newcastle (NSW), Remabi Park (SA), and the University of South Australia (SA), between June 2012 and December 2013. The genetic integrity of our colony is kept via rotation of breeders using the Poiley outbreeding system [21]. Breeding boxes were set in a male:female ratio of 1:1 (virgin males) to 1:3 (experienced males) per cage, with cage dimensions of 520 x 335 x 95 (mm). Female dunnarts reach sexual maturity at approximately 5 months (average age of females’ first-litter is 210 days), and the first females to conceive per litter were kept as new breeders to select for fertility. Weaned and adult dunnarts not included in mating groups were housed individually in cages containing 10 mm corn cob substrate, shredded newspaper, nest boxes (cardboard tubes and boxes), a sand bowl, a water bowl, a 7–12 cm rock, a food bowl, and a drinking bottle. Animals were fed a daily diet consisting of cat biscuits (Adult Cat Total Wellbeing, chicken, Advance) ad libitum, beef mince-mix (w/w, 78% lean beef mince, 5% ground cat kibble, 14% Wombaroo small carnivore food, and 3% of balanced calcium powder; 0.5g for weaners/non-breeders, and 4g lactating females), and live mealworms (Tenebrio molitor larva; 3 for weaners/non-breeders, and 6–10 for lactating females). Additionally, once per week, animals received 5-10g sunflower seeds, 5-10g hard-boiled egg and 5-10g apple. The room was maintained on a 16:8h light:dark cycle, humidity between 30 and 60% and temperature between 22 and 26°C. To optimise breeding success, false winters were set, usually around June and/or December, by gradually reversing the light cycle and dropping temperature by 2–3°C for 2–4 weeks.

To check for joeys’ presence, females in breeding cages were gently retrieved from a bottomless hiding box with one hand, allowing gentle inspection of the pouch with the other hand. In non-parous females, the pouch is usually tight and full of pale and dry hair. On the other hand, the pouch of pregnant and oestrous females is easier to open and hairless [22]. Females with pouch-young can be detected by a moist pouch and presence of joeys attached to the teats. Teats not used by joeys are small and opaque, while the ones with attached young are prominent and highly vascularised.

Anaesthesia and tissue collection

For temporary sedation, adult female dunnarts with joeys were transferred into a gas anaesthesia induction chamber with isoflurane 5%, delivered in oxygen at a flow rate of 200 mL/Kg/min, followed by isoflurane 2–5% supplied through a rubber mask throughout the procedure (ZDS MINI Qube Anaesthetic System). This allows careful examination of joeys in a non-invasive manner. Joeys were removed from the teat by gently pulling with forceps and euthanised with an intraperitoneal injection of 0.05–0.5 mL solution of sodium pentobarbitone (1/50 v/v Lethabarb, Virbac, corresponding to 190 mg Lethabarb per kg body weight), or 5–15 min ice anaesthesia for joeys younger than postnatal day (P) 45, followed by transcardial perfusion of 0.9% NaCl and 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA), or immersion-fixation in 4% PFA for joeys younger than P15.

Histology and microscopy

Following perfusion, joeys younger than 2 weeks of age underwent paraffin embedding and had their heads sectioned in the coronal plane at 12 μm in a sliding microtome (Leica SM 2000R). Sections were mounted, dewaxed and stained with haematoxylin and eosin. Older joeys had their brains dissected, photographed (Lumix DMC-LX7, Panasonic), embedded in 3.4% agarose blocks and sectioned at 50 μm using a vibratome (Leica VT1000S). These sections were stained for 10 minutes with 0.1% DAPI (Invitrogen), and then washed and coverslipped with ProLong Gold (Invitrogen). Brightfield and fluorescence microphotographs were obtained using a Zeiss upright Axio-Imager microscope fitted with Axio-Cam HRc and HRm cameras, and captured using AxioVision software (Carl Zeiss). Photoshop and Illustrator (Adobe) were used to adjust levels, contrast and prepare figure panels.

Morphometry and statistics

To track joey morphometry, pouches were exposed as described above and the joeys were photographed (Lumix DMC-LX7, Panasonic) at regular intervals until P45. Crown-rump length, skull width (bi-parietal diameter), and forelimb and hindlimb length were measured from the pictures that included identical calibration tools (Fiji, NIH). Details about the appearance of the joeys were noted across stages, paying particular attention to fur, mouth, eyes, ears, nose, skin, forelimbs, fingers, hindlimbs, and toes. Growth curves and 95% confidence intervals were generated using non-linear semilog fitting of morphometric measurements following the least-squares method. Goodness-of-fit was quantified as R squared (R2) with 95% confidence intervals and degrees of freedom (d.f.) > 12. Normality tests of residuals were passed in all cases (D’Agostino-Pearson omnibus K2; Prism 7, GraphPad). Measurements are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean.

Results

Developmental morphometry and growth curves of postnatal S. crassicaudata

To characterise growth and developmental morphometry of postnatal S. crassicaudata, we measured crown-rump length, maximum head width, forelimb length and hindlimb length from at least four different individuals (median 9, mean 14.3) per measurement and age range (see Table 1). Semi-logarithmic relationships between each of these measurements and ages were determined, including best-fit regression curves and 95% confidence intervals (Fig 1). Crown-rump length, skull width and forelimb length showed good non-linear fit (R2 ≥ 0.985; Table 2), indicating that morphometry alone can be used to infer developmental age in the case of incomplete data, such as for example from field measurements where date of birth is unknown [22]. Interestingly, however, body size is often more similar between littermates than to age-matched joeys of different sized litters, suggesting that differences in the metabolic/nutritional state of the mothers and/or the number of teats/joeys per litter could be sources of variability [7, 9].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Developmental growth curves of postnatal S. crassicaudata.

Semilogarithmic relationships between the postnatal age (in days) and crown-rump length (A), head width (B), forelimb length (C), and hindlimb length (D). Mean ± standard error of the mean, as well as best-fit regression curves (red lines) and 95% confidence intervals (dotted lines) are indicated in each case. Non-linear functions whereby y indicates the corresponding measurements (in mm) and x the age in days, as well as R2 values are indicated. See Tables 1 and 2 for additional information.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Morphometric measurements in postnatal S. crassicaudata.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Non-linear regression statistics for S. crassicaudata growth curves.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.t002

A postnatal staging system for S. crassicaudata

To provide a developmental staging system that would account for inter-individual size variability, while allowing inter-species comparisons, we categorised dunnart development within both the Carnegie staging system for human development [13, 14], and the Thieler staging system for mouse development [15]. We considered formation of the eye as a comparative “anchor point”, as its developmental timing is highly conserved from birds to mammals [23]. The Carnegie human system comprises 23 stages from conception to the formation of most adult structures (ca. 60 days/8-9 weeks), after which the embryo is considered a foetus. The Thieler mouse system is based on milestones that match the Carnegie stages, and includes five additional stages that end postnatally with eye opening. For dunnarts, we have developed a comparable system, whereby embryos are born at equivalent stage 18 and leave the pouch at stage 29, ca. 70 days later (Fig 2). Intrauterine gestation lasts for 13–14 days [24], after which multiple 3–5 mm newborns must reach the pouch and attach to one of the 8–10 teats to complete development. One mother out of 105 in our study had 11 teats and 11 joeys, prompting the speculation that more newborns than teats available might be born and, therefore, that teat number is a limiting factor for litter size. Mothers in our study had an average of six and a mode of seven joeys.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Developmental series of postnatal S. crassicaudata.

A system of twenty-nine arbitrary stages from conception to weaning is included to match equivalent stages in human (Carnegie staging system) and mouse (Thieler staging system). Birth occurs at stage 18. Corresponding age of each stage is included as a range of postnatal days (P). See Table 3 for further details. Scale bar: 5 mm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g002

Early postnatal anatomy of newborn S. crassicaudata (stage 18)

Newborn dunnarts have translucent and glossy skin. The forelimbs display alternating movements and are larger and more developed than the hindlimbs. The forelimbs have a distinguishable elbow joint and hand digits are partially fused (digital serration), while the forelimbs are proximally fused to the tail, mostly not mobile, and the feet digits are completely fused (paddle shape), although digital grooves are noticeable (Fig 3 and Table 3). The small eyes have a faint but noticeable ring of pigment. While eutherian mammals at equivalent stages have not yet undergone formation of the mouth apparatus, newborn dunnarts have a fully fused palate and well developed tongue (Figs 3 and 4). The lateral margins of the tongue press tightly around the teat and against the palate to allow active milk suckling. The presence of an olfactory and vomeronasal neuroepithelium (Fig 4E and 4F) in newborns raises the question of whether these systems are functional at birth. The olfactory bulb receives sensory axons traversing the cribiform plate, and the telencephalic vesicles include well-developed ganglionic eminences, an olfactory (piriform) cortex and a thin dorsal pallium (presumptive isocortex) that consists of a proliferative ventricular zone and preplate (Fig 4D). Eye development at birth has just undergone closure of the lens vesicle but no lens fibres are present as yet (Fig 4G).

thumbnail
Fig 3. External body features of newborn S. crassicaudata.

Microphotograph of a stage 18 newborn dunnart as seen from the right and from the front. Note the prominent orofacial features, including fused mandible (Mb), and wide nostrils (N). The eyes (Ey) are small and lightly pigmented. Forelimbs are more advanced than hindlimbs, with digits serrated as compared to completely fused. Scale bar: 1mm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Craniofacial features of newborn S. crassicaudata.

(A) Schematic of a stage 18 dunnart head (postnatal day 0–3) indicating the relative size and position of nostrils, mouth, tongue, eyes and brain, as well as the section planes shown in (B-D). (B-D) Haematoxylin/eosin staining of a rostral-caudal series through the planes depicted in panel A reveals orofacial, sensory and brain structures of newborn dunnarts, including a prominent tongue (T) that encloses the teat inside the mouth (Mo) reaching the lateral margins of the palate (Pal). Large nasal cavities (NC) with fused palate (Pal) and nasal septum (NSe) can be seen, including condensation of mandibular (Mb) and maxillary (Mx) processes, eyes (Ey) fully covered by skin, and scattered vibrissal follicles (VF) in the snout. Telencephalic structures include the olfactory bulb (OB), with surrounding olfactory nerve layer (onl), preplate (Pp), ventricular zone (VZ) and ganglionic eminences (GE) as well as a large lateral ventricle (LV). E-F, inset of the regions highlighted in B-D depicting developing sensory structures. E, the vomeronasal organ (VO) includes a lumen (VLu), neuroepithelium (VE) and capsule (VC) at the base of NSe and Pal. F, a thick olfactory neuroepithelium (OE) is located at the roof of the NC, immediately below the presumptive cribiform plate (CrP). G, eye development has just undergone closure of the lens vesicle (LVe), the pigment epithelium (Pi) outlines a dark ring around cells of the retina (Re). Et, ethmoid bone; Orb, orbital bone; PoA, preoptic area; PSp; presphenoid bone. Scale bar: 500 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g004

thumbnail
Table 3. Developmental features across postnatal stages of S. crassicaudata.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.t003

Craniofacial and brain development in stages 19–21 of S. crassicaudata

Head features of subsequent stages 19, 20, 21 of S. crassicaudata (postnatal days 4–7, 8–11, and 12–15, respectively) are presented in Fig 5A–5C. Hair follicles can be seen increasing in abundance between these stages (Figs 4 and 5), and subsequent development of thick hair over the parietal scalp remains a salient feature of early-to-mid postnatal development. Telencephalic development includes growth of the piriform cortex and expansion of the lateral olfactory tract, which becomes visible by stage 20 (Fig 5B). The ganglionic eminences are apparent by stage 20 and become striated at stage 21 by axons forming the internal capsule (Fig 5C). The isocortical preplate has split to form the marginal zone and the subplate by stage 20, followed by the emergence of a thin cortical plate by stage 21. The anterior commissure, carrying olfactory, piriform and temporal isocortical axons appears at stage 21. Eye development shows a similar developmental sequence to stage-matched mice and humans (Fig 5D–5F). Stage 19 includes formation of lens fibres, by stage 20 a thin lens epithelium, enlarged lens fibres, and neuroblastic and ganglion cell layers in the retina are apparent, and stage 21 is characterised by a mature-looking lens, a thicker cornea and a distinct optic nerve. Accordingly, pioneering axons of the dunnart optic nerve have been reported to begin growth, cross the midline, and reach their central targets, respectively, during these three stages [25, 26].

thumbnail
Fig 5. Craniofacial features of stages 19–21 S. crassicaudata.

Haematoxylin/eosin staining through rostral (left) and caudal (right) regions of the head of S. crassicaudata at stages 19 (A), 20 (B), and 21 (C) in the coronal plane (postnatal days 4–7, 8–11, and 12–15, respectively). Insets of the developing eye at corresponding ages (D-F, respectively). ac, anterior commissure; BG, basal ganglia; Cg, cingulate cortex; Cla, claustrum; Co, cornea; CP, cortical plate; ec, external capsule; En, endopiriform nucleus; Ey, eye; Et, ethmoid bone; F, follicle; Ga, ganglion layer; GE, ganglionic eminence; Hp, hippocampus; ic, internal capsule; IG, indusium griseum; L, lens; LE, lens epithelium; LF, lens fibres; Li, eyelid; lot, lateral olfactory tract; LV, lateral ventricle; LVe, lens vesicle; Mb, mandibulary process; Mo, mouth; Mx, maxillary process; MZ, marginal zone; NC, nasal cavity; Nb, neuroblastic layer; OE, olfactory neuroepithelium; ON, optic nerve; Orb, orbital bone; OS, optic stalk; OT, olfactory tubercle; Pi, pigment epithelium; Pir, piriform cortex; PoA, preoptic area; Pp, preplate; PSp; presphenoid bone; Re, retinal layer; Se, septum; Sp, subplate; St, striatum; SVZ, subventricular zone; T, tongue; VZ, ventricular zone. Scale bars: 500 μm (A-C), 100 μm (E-F).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g005

Mid and late pouch development: Cortical layering and body changes in stages 22–29 of S. crassicaudata

Subsequent stages of S. crassicaudata comprise a progressive succession of general body features and brain development. Between stages 22–25, dunnarts undergo changes in facial morphology, with mandibular and maxillar processes progressing from a marked prognathism (underbite), to alignment and progression of the snout (Fig 2 and Table 3). Digit development begins in the forelimbs, and is not evident in the hindlimbs until stage 22.

Principal neuron cell layers of the piriform cortex and olfactory tubercle are evident by stage 22, and become angled by the enlargement of the lateral olfactory tract (Fig 6). This is followed by the expansion of the cortical plate and underlying subplate from latero-ventral to medio-dorsal regions. In fact, the prospective claustrum and endopiriform nuclei, which arise from lateral and ventral pallial derivatives [27, 28], can be distinguished from stage 22 onwards, before the respective enlargement of the overlaying insular/perirhinal and piriform cortices, and formation of the rhinal fissure by stage 25 (Fig 6, arrowheads). In eutherians, most interhemispheric projections from the isocortex and cingulate cortex cross the midline through the corpus callosum, while in non-eutherians these projections course through the anterior commissure. In dunnarts, as in other marsupials, the anterior commissure forms at least one week before the hippocampal commissure, while in eutherians they arise closer in time, with the corpus callosum being the last telencephalic commissure to form [6, 19, 2931]. Interestingly, dunnarts also lack the astrogliogenic remodelling of the septal midline, a process required for callosal formation in eutherians [32, 33] that could relate to heterochronies in the formation of interhemispheric circuits in the brain of mammals.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Brain features of S. crassicaudata between stages 21 and 26.

Developmental series of fixed dunnart brains showing a dorsal view of the whole brain (left column) and corresponding DAPI-stained coronal section (right column) through the approximate region indicated by dotted lines. The main brain regions are colour-coded, and stages and approximate postnatal (P) days are indicated. Arrowheads show the prospective rhinal fissure (rf). ac, anterior commissure; BS, brainstem; CA, cornu ammonis hippocampi; Cb, cerebellum; Cd, caudate; Cg, cingulate cortex; Cla, claustrum; CP, cortical plate; DG, dentate gyrus; Di, diencephalon; ec, external capsule; En, endopiriform; f, fornix; hc, hippocampal commissure; Hp, hippocampus; ic, internal capsule; IC, inferior colliculus; Ins, insular cortex; IsoCx, isocortex; IZ, intermediate zone; L5/6, isocortical layers 5/6; lot, lateral olfactory tract; MZ, marginal zone; OB, olfactory bulb; OT, olfactory tubercle; Pir, piriform cortex; Pir2, piriform cortex, layer 2; PRh, perirhinal; Pu, putamen; rf, rhinal fissure; SC, superior colliculus; Se, septum; SP, subplate; SVZ, subventricular zone; VZ, ventricular zone. Scale bars: 2000 μm in whole brains, and 200 μm in coronal sections.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g006

A dorsal view of the brain reveals the progressive growth of the isocortical mantle (Figs 6 and 7, green schematics), which does not fully cover the dorsal diencephalon until stage 28. Development of the isocortex is largely protracted in this species, as in other marsupials [7, 8, 10], and isocortical layers do not become apparent until stage 27 (Fig 7). The piriform/insular cortex boundary (arrowheads in Figs 6 and 7) reveals the ventral expansion of the isocortex, as the rhinal fissure is positioned more ventrally between stages 24–27. Infragranular neurons that give rise to layers 5/6 of the isocortex, as well as subplate neurons, are recognisable superficial to the intermediate zone by stage 26, once supragranular neurons have begun their radial migration towards the cortical plate. The thalamo-recipient cell dense layer 4 as well as upper layers 2/3 become evident by stage 27 onwards (Fig 7). By this time, sensory axons from the thalamus accumulate in layer 4, forming modality-specific sensory areas that subsequently expand tangentially, further displacing the rhinal fissure ventrally (Fig 7).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Brain features of S. crassicaudata between stages 27 and adulthood.

Developmental series of fixed dunnart brains showing a dorsal view of the whole brain (left column) and corresponding DAPI-stained coronal section (right column) through the approximate region indicated by dotted lines. The main brain regions are colour-coded, and stages and approximate postnatal (P) days are indicated. Arrowheads show the rhinal fissure (rf). ac, anterior commissure; BS, brainstem; CA, cornu ammonis hippocampi; Cb, cerebellum; Cd, caudate; Cg, cingulate cortex; Cla, claustrum; DG, dentate gyrus; ec, external capsule; En, endopiriform; f, fornix; hc, hippocampal commissure; ic, internal capsule; IC, inferior colliculus; Ins, insular cortex; IsoCx, isocortex; L1-6, isocortical layers 1–6; lot, lateral olfactory tract; MZ, marginal zone; OB, olfactory bulb; OT, olfactory tubercle; Pir, piriform cortex; Pir2, piriform cortex, layer 2; PoA, preoptic area; Pu, putamen; SC, superior colliculus; Se, septum; SP, subplate; SVZ, subventricular zone; VZ, ventricular zone. Scale bars: 2000 μm in whole brains, and 200 μm in coronal sections.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184450.g007

Pre- and peri-weaning stages are characterised by the formation of the mouth lining from a deep groove, noticeable from stage 26, which allows full opening of the mouth for the first time around stage 28, once isocortical neurons are established in their final layers. After this time, joeys can be observed swapping teats and leaving the pouch temporarily. Fur begins covering the whole body by stage 26 and joeys achieve a mature appearance by stage 29, the last stage of our study, which culminates with eye opening and weaning at around 70 postnatal days.

Discussion

Eutherian mammals, such as rodents and humans, differ from marsupials and monotremes in that they have a prolonged intrauterine period that extends beyond embryonic development (past Carnegie/Thieler stage 23). Non-eutherian mammals, on the other hand, are born at comparatively earlier stages and must complete development by feeding from their mothers’ milk, outside the uterus. Accordingly, dunnarts are born with a fully functional mouth apparatus, whereas eutherians at an equivalent stage have unfused pharyngeal arches and orofacial development occurs after the onset of telencephalic development [1, 12, 34]. Another difference is that the anterior skeleton forms much earlier than the posterior skeleton in marsupials, as compared to the more uniform development of eutherians [1, 2, 3538]. As functional forelimbs are crucial for marsupial embryos to reach the teat, such early behaviour may act as a developmental constraint limiting phenotypic variability of the mouth and forelimbs in marsupials. Accordingly, eutherians display a comparatively larger repertoire of mouth and forelimb morphologies across species than marsupials (e.g. mouth of elephants and star-nosed moles, or forelimbs of bats and dolphins) likely due to the release of such constraints by the extended intrauterine retention of the embryo [34, 39]. Therefore, a major challenge of translating staging systems between species are differences in the relative rates of development between different body parts. However, as the rate of brain development is less variable than that of the body across mammals [11, 4042], information about the timing of neural structures are optimal for staging purposes. Importantly, later events in brain development are slower in marsupials than in eutherians [7, 10, 11], possibly due to metabolic differences of lactation versus placentation during development [8, 9], further highlighting their potential as animal models brain wiring.

As compared to eutherians, development of the dunnart isocortex is largely protracted and occurs mostly between the second and the sixth postnatal week. The olfactory cortex, on the other hand, acquires a layered organisation much earlier, along with the growth of the lateral olfactory tract and formation of the anterior commissure. It is not clear whether the olfactory and/or vomeronasal systems of dunnarts are functional at birth, however newborn wallabies orient themselves towards odours from the mother’s pouch [43], despite the immature appearance of their olfactory systems [44]. In any case, peripheral and central olfactory structures are present in dunnart newborns, well before the arrival of thalamic inputs from other senses and formation of layers in the isocortex. Whether early olfactory function is related to the subsequent formation of isocortical circuits is a possibility that has ignited recent interest [4547]. Developmental interactions between the olfactory systems and the rest of the forebrain appear conserved across mammals, even in species whose adults lack functional olfactory systems [4850]. Moreover, evidence from mice suggest that the olfactory system can influence the cellular and electrical features of isocortical development [5153], further supporting the hypothesis that the olfactory system could be a driver of mammalian brain development.

The extended period of forebrain formation in marsupials, plus the continuous access to developing joeys inside the pouch, offers promising opportunities to study multiple developmental processes sequentially, and in vivo. Such capability cannot be achieved with traditional eutherian models, such as rodents, carnivores and primates, as embryos develop inside the uterus and cannot readily survive if taken prematurely. The excellent breeding under controlled conditions, small body size, large litters, and short life cycles, make dunnarts promising laboratory models to study not only the mechanisms of brain development but also multiple questions of biological relevance.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the help from staff from The University of Queensland Biological Resources, in particular Kym French, Trish Hitchcock and Cora Lau. Additional thanks to Ilan Gobius, Robert Sullivan, Thomas Pollak, Caitlin Bridges and Ching Moey for their help in experimental and/or animal procedures, and to the reviewers for their helpful suggestions to improve the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Smith KK. Comparative Patterns of Craniofacial Development in Eutherian and Metatherian Mammals. Evolution. 1997;51(5):1663–78. pmid:28568626
  2. 2. Smith KK. Heterochrony revisited: the evolution of developmental sequences. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2001;73(2):169–86.
  3. 3. Werneburg I, Sánchez-Villagra MR. The early development of the echidna, Tachyglossus aculeatus (Mammalia: Monotremata), and patterns of mammalian development. Acta Zoologica. 2011;92(1):75–88.
  4. 4. Werneburg I, Tzika AC, Hautier L, Asher RJ, Milinkovitch MC, Sanchez-Villagra MR. Development and embryonic staging in non-model organisms: the case of an afrotherian mammal. J Anat. 2013;222(1):2–18. Epub 2012/04/28. pmid:22537021; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3552411.
  5. 5. Jeffery JE, Richardson MK, Coates MI, Bininda-Emonds OR. Analyzing developmental sequences within a phylogenetic framework. Systematic biology. 2002;51(3):478–91. Epub 2002/06/25. pmid:12079645.
  6. 6. Ashwell KWS, Waite PME, Marotte L. Ontogeny of the Projection Tracts and Commissural Fibres in the Forebrain of the Tammar Wallaby (Macropus eugenii): Timing in Comparison with Other Mammals. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1996;47(1):8–22. pmid:8834781
  7. 7. Darlington RB, Dunlop SA, Finlay BL. Neural development in metatherian and eutherian mammals: variation and constraint. J Comp Neurol. 1999;411(3):359–68. Epub 1999/07/22. pmid:10413772.
  8. 8. Renfree MB, Holt A, Green S, Carr J, Cheek D. Ontogeny of the brain in a marsupial (Macropus eugenii) throughout pouch life. I. Brain growth. Brain Behav Evol. 1982;20:57–1. pmid:7104670
  9. 9. Weisbecker V, Goswami A. Brain size, life history, and metabolism at the marsupial/placental dichotomy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(37):16216–21. pmid:20823252
  10. 10. Workman AD, Charvet CJ, Clancy B, Darlington RB, Finlay BL. Modeling transformations of neurodevelopmental sequences across mammalian species. J Neurosci. 2013;33(17):7368–83. Epub 2013/04/26. pmid:23616543; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3928428.
  11. 11. Halley AC. Minimal variation in eutherian brain growth rates during fetal neurogenesis. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 2017;284(1854). Epub 2017/05/12. pmid:28490626; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5443945.
  12. 12. Nelson JE. Developmental staging in a marsupial Dasyurus hallucatus. Anat Embryol (Berl). 1992;185(4):335–54. Epub 1992/01/01. pmid:1609962.
  13. 13. O'Rahilly R, Müller F. Developmental stages in human embryos: Including a revision of Streeter's "Horizons" and a survey of the Carnegie collection. Connecticut: Merider-Stinehour Press; 1987. 306 p.
  14. 14. Streeter G. Developmental Horizons In Human Embryos Description Or Age Groups XIX, XX, XXI, XXII, And XXIII, Being The Fifth Issue Of A Survey Of The Carnegie Collection. Streeter G, editor. Baltimore1957.
  15. 15. Thieler K. The House Mouse: Atlas of Embryonic Development. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1989. 178 p.
  16. 16. McCrady E. The embryology of the opossum. Am Anat Memoirs. 1938;16:1–233.
  17. 17. Rakic P. Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science. 1988;241(4862):170–6. pmid:3291116
  18. 18. Cummings DM, Malun D, Brunjes PC. Development of the anterior commissure in the opossum: midline extracellular space and glia coincide with early axon decussation. J Neurobiol. 1997;32(4):403–14. Epub 1997/04/01. pmid:9087892.
  19. 19. Shang F, Ashwell KWS, Marotte LR, Waite PME. Development of commissural neurons in the wallaby (Macropus eugenii). The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1997;387(4):507–23. pmid:9373010
  20. 20. Molnar Z, Knott GW, Blakemore C, Saunders NR. Development of thalamocortical projections in the South American gray short-tailed opossum (Monodelphis domestica). J Comp Neurol. 1998;398(4):491–514. Epub 1998/08/26. pmid:9717705.
  21. 21. Poiley S. A systematic method of breeder rotation for non-inbred laboratory animal colonies. Proc Anim Care Panel. 1960;10:159–66.
  22. 22. Morton SR. An ecological study of Sminthopsis crassicaudata (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae) III. Reproduction and life history. Aust Wildl Res. 1978;5:183–211.
  23. 23. Dreher B, Robinson SR. Development of the retinofugal pathway in birds and mammals: evidence for a common 'timetable'. Brain Behav Evol. 1988;31(6):369–90. Epub 1988/01/01. pmid:3046710.
  24. 24. Godfrey GK, Crowcroft P. Breeding the Fat-tailed marsupial mouse in captivity. International Zoo Yearbook. 1971;11(1):33–8.
  25. 25. Dunlop SA, Tee LB, Lund RD, Beazley LD. Development of primary visual projections occurs entirely postnatally in the fat-tailed dunnart, a marsupial mouse, Sminthopsis crassicaudata. J Comp Neurol. 1997;384(1):26–40. Epub 1997/07/21. pmid:9214538.
  26. 26. Dunlop SA, Lund RD, Beazley LD. Segregation of optic input in a three-eyed mammal. Exp Neurol. 1996;137(2):294–8. Epub 1996/02/01. pmid:8635544.
  27. 27. Watson C, Puelles L. Developmental gene expression in the mouse clarifies the organization of the claustrum and related endopiriform nuclei. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2017;525(6):1499–508. pmid:27159785
  28. 28. Puelles L. Development and Evolution of the Claustrum. In: Smythies John, Edelstein Larry, Ramachandran VS, editors. The Claustrum: Structural, Functional, and Clinical Neuroscience. San Diego: Academic Press; 2014. p. 119–76.
  29. 29. Suárez R. Evolution of Telencephalic Commissures: Conservation and Change of Developmental Systems in the Origin of Brain Wiring Novelties. In: Kaas JH, editor. Evolution of Nervous Systems (Second Edition). Oxford: Academic Press; 2017. p. 205–23.
  30. 30. Suárez R, Gobius I, Richards LJ. Evolution and development of interhemispheric connections in the vertebrate forebrain. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:497. Epub 2014/07/30. pmid:25071525; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc4094842.
  31. 31. Ashwell KW, Marotte LR, Li L, Waite PM. Anterior commissure of the wallaby (Macropus eugenii): adult morphology and development. J Comp Neurol. 1996;366(3):478–94. Epub 1996/03/11. pmid:8907360.
  32. 32. Gobius I, Morcom L, Suarez R, Bunt J, Bukshpun P, Reardon W, et al. Astroglial-mediated remodeling of the interhemispheric midline is required for the formation of the corpus callosum. Cell reports. 2016;17(3):735–47. Epub 2016/10/13. pmid:27732850; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5094913.
  33. 33. Gobius I, Suárez R, Morcom L, Paolino A, Edwards TJ, Kozulin P, et al. Astroglial-mediated remodeling of the interhemispheric midline during telencephalic development is exclusive to eutherian mammals. Neural Development. 2017;12(1):9. pmid:28558801
  34. 34. Goswami A, Randau M, Polly PD, Weisbecker V, Bennett CV, Hautier L, et al. Do developmental constraints and high integration limit the evolution of the marsupial oral apparatus? Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2016;56(3):404–15. pmid:27260858
  35. 35. Weisbecker V, Goswami A, Wroe S, Sánchez-Villagra MR. Ossification heterochrony in the Therian postcranial skeleton and the marsupial-placental dichotomy. Evolution. 2008;62(8):2027–41. pmid:18489720
  36. 36. Keyte AL, Smith KK. Heterochrony and developmental timing mechanisms: changing ontogenies in evolution. Seminars in cell & developmental biology. 2014;34:99–107. Epub 2014/07/06. pmid:24994599; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4201350.
  37. 37. Sánchez-Villagra MR. Comparative patterns of postcranial ontogeny in therian Mammals: An analysis of relative timing of ossification events. Journal of Experimental Zoology. 2002;294(3):264–73. pmid:12362432
  38. 38. Chew KY, Shaw G, Yu H, Pask AJ, Renfree MB. Heterochrony in the regulation of the developing marsupial limb. Developmental Dynamics. 2014;243(2):324–38. pmid:24115631
  39. 39. Kelly EM, Sears KE. Limb specialization in living marsupial and eutherian mammals: constraints on mammalian limb evolution. Journal of Mammalogy. 2011;92(5):1038–49.
  40. 40. Halley AC. Prenatal Brain-Body Allometry in Mammals. Brain Behav Evol. 2016;88(1):14–24. Epub 2016/08/27. pmid:27561684.
  41. 41. Passingham RE. Rates of brain development in mammals including man. Brain Behav Evol. 1985;26(3–4):167–75. Epub 1985/01/01. pmid:3936591.
  42. 42. Sacher GA, Staffeldt EF. Relation of Gestation Time to Brain Weight for Placental Mammals: Implications for the Theory of Vertebrate Growth. The American Naturalist. 1974;108(963):593–615.
  43. 43. Schneider NY, Fletcher TP, Shaw G, Renfree MB. The olfactory system of the tammar wallaby is developed at birth and directs the neonate to its mother's pouch odours. Reproduction. 2009;138(5):849–57. pmid:19638443
  44. 44. Ashwell KWS, Marotte LR, Cheng G. Development of the Olfactory System in a Wallaby (Macropus eugenii). Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2008;71(3):216–30. pmid:18322362
  45. 45. Rowe TB, Macrini TE, Luo ZX. Fossil evidence on origin of the mammalian brain. Science. 2011;332(6032):955–7. Epub 2011/05/21. pmid:21596988.
  46. 46. Aboitiz F, Montiel JF. Olfaction, navigation, and the origin of isocortex. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2015;9. pmid:26578863
  47. 47. Rowe TB, Shepherd GM. Role of ortho-retronasal olfaction in mammalian cortical evolution. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2016;524(3):471–95. pmid:25975561
  48. 48. Oelschläger HA, Buhl EH, Dann JF. Development of the nervus terminalis in mammals including toothed whales and humans. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1987;519(1):447–64.
  49. 49. Meisami E, Bhatnagar KP. Structure and diversity in mammalian accessory olfactory bulb. Microsc Res Tech. 1998;43(6):476–99. Epub 1999/01/08. pmid:9880163.
  50. 50. Suárez R, García-González D, de Castro F. Mutual influences between the main olfactory and vomeronasal systems in development and evolution. Front Neuroanat. 2012;6:50. Epub 2012/12/28. pmid:23269914; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3529325.
  51. 51. de Frutos Cristina A, Bouvier G, Arai Y, Thion Morgane S, Lokmane L, Keita M, et al. Reallocation of olfactory Cajal-Retzius cells shapes neocortex architecture. Neuron. 2016;92(2):435–48. pmid:27693257
  52. 52. Conhaim J, Easton CR, Becker MI, Barahimi M, Cedarbaum ER, Moore JG, et al. Developmental changes in propagation patterns and transmitter dependence of waves of spontaneous activity in the mouse cerebral cortex. J Physiol. 2011;589(Pt 10):2529–41. Epub 2011/04/14. pmid:21486817; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3115823.
  53. 53. Easton CR, Weir K, Scott A, Moen SP, Barger Z, Folch A, et al. Genetic elimination of GABAergic neurotransmission reveals two distinct pacemakers for spontaneous waves of activity in the developing mouse cortex. J Neurosci. 2014;34(11):3854–63. Epub 2014/03/14. pmid:24623764; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3951690.