Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Male Courtship Pheromones Induce Cloacal Gaping in Female Newts (Salamandridae)

Abstract

Pheromones are an important component of sexual communication in courting salamanders, but the number of species in which their use has been demonstrated with behavioral evidence remains limited. Here we developed a behavioral assay for demonstrating courtship pheromone use in the aquatically courting Iberian ribbed newt Pleurodeles waltl. By performing an in-depth study of the courtship behavior, we show that females invariably open their cloaca (cloacal gaping) before engaging in pinwheel behavior, the circling movement that is the prelude to spermatophore uptake. In contrast, cloacal gaping was not observed in failed courtships, where females escaped or displayed thanatosis. Since gaping mainly occurred during male amplexus and cloacal imposition, which is the obvious period of pheromone transfer, we next investigated whether male courtship water (i.e., water holding courtship pheromones) alone was able to induce this reaction in females. These tests showed that courtship water induced cloacal gaping significantly more than water, even in the absence of a male. Cloacal gaping thus provides a simple and robust test for demonstrating courtship pheromone use in the Iberian ribbed newt. Since opening the cloaca is an essential prerequisite for spermatophore pick-up in all internally fertilizing salamanders, we hypothesize that variations on this assay will also be useful in several other species.

Introduction

Courtship pheromones are chemical signals that elicit a specific reaction in conspecifics of the opposite gender during courtship [1]. Males of internally fertilizing salamanders use multiple courtship pheromones to enhance female responses during their courtship rituals [26], and researchers have developed various behavioral tests in trying to demonstrate effective use of these molecules. In the terrestrially courting lungless salamanders (Plethodontidae), courtship behavior among species is remarkably homogeneous [3], and consists of a tail-straddling walk in which the female holds her chin on the male’s tail base, while both move forward until the spermatophore transfer has occurred. For this family, a behavioral test has been designed in which the pheromone-producing mental glands of the male were removed and courtship duration of couples was compared with and without the application of pheromones [7,8]. In contrast to plethodontids, salamandrids are mainly aquatically courting, have no mental gland, and are characterized by a wide diversity in courtship strategies [3,9,10]. Pheromone communication in Salamandridae, which possess dorsal glands that open into the cloaca, has mostly been studied using two-choice tests. Such tests, which give an animal the choice between a chemical and an empty source, have been applied in several variations, such as two floating sponges [2,1114], linear olfactometers [15], Y-mazes [1619], and two-choice aquaria [20]. However, these tests are choice-based and do not necessarily allow the natural courtship responses of the animals. Recently, the development of a two-female test in alpine newts (Ichthyosaura alpestris) [21] and palmate newts (Lissotriton helveticus) [6] showed to be effective for inducing the natural female responses under influence of courtship pheromones. In nature, males of these species typically tail-fan pheromones from their cloaca towards the female, and both sexes have limited contact during the entire courtship display [9]. The female responds to pheromones by showing following behavior and tail-touching [21], which are the prelude for a successful spermatophore transfer with a male [22,23]. In the two-female test, the animals display identical behavioral responses when exposed to courtship water (i.e. water in which a male has been courting, thus containing pheromones) in the absence of a male. By exposing the females to isolated courtship water molecules, it was possible to identify the courtship pheromones by scoring the typical following and tail-touching responses [6].

While most species in the nested clade of Modern Eurasian salamandrids court with limited contact, several other salamanders use amplexus (i.e. the male grasps the female with his limbs) during courtship, rendering the quantification of following behavior less appropriate as a pheromone test for these animals. In aiming to construct a bioassay that might be useful for identifying courtship pheromones across salamandrids, we here investigated whether pheromones can induce a common female response that is independent of species-specific courtship behavior. We used Pleurodeles waltl, a newt that displays a sequence of several courtship behaviors (tail-fanning, ventral amplexus, head-to-head amplexus, pinwheel sperm transfer) [24,25] to search for such a response. Our observations and subsequent tests show that females of this species display cloacal gaping (i.e. they open their cloaca) as a response to male pheromones, and that quantification of this behavior allows demonstrating the use of chemical communication during courtship.

Materials and Methods

Animal housing and husbandry

A population of 31 animals (10 males and 21 females) was bought from a hobby breeder, and was housed in 3 large containers (L: 78 cm, W: 60 cm, H: 50 cm) with aged tap water (water depth: 25cm; water temperature: 18°C). Genders were separately housed and distributed over the 3 containers. To induce their courtship mood, the animals were thoroughly fed and the water depth was increased with 15 cm, and 25% of the water was refreshed daily. The population was recognized as ready-to-mate by the development of nuptial pads in males and the presence of swollen bellies in females [26]. Animals were subjected to one set-up every other day. The animals are currently kept in the lab under the same conditions, where they will be used for future pheromone research.

Ethical note

To approximate environmental conditions, housing containers were placed in front of large windows that allowed natural sunlight and natural day-night rhythms. Additionally, the containers were supplied with water plants, stones and wooden hiding places for maximum comfort of the animals. Animals were disturbed as little as possible by handling them only once per other day with clean wetted gloves. To minimize stress during the tests, carton fences were placed around the transparent test containers. All experiments complied with EU and Belgian regulations concerning animal welfare. All experiments were approved by the Ethical Committee for Animal Experiments of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (project number ECAE14-220-35), and performed accordingly.

Analysis of courtship behavior

To observe the courtship of P. waltl in detail, one male and one female were placed in a plastic container (25x16x14 cm) filled with 2L of aged tap water. 42 combinations were observed from top, side and bottom view. Each female was observed twice with two different males. Eleven randomly picked couples were timed during the entire courtship sequence.

Cloacal gaping test

Two females were placed in a plastic container (25x16x14 cm) filled with 2L of courtship water (CW; see below) for 60 minutes (this duration was chosen based on the above observations of courtship behavior; see Results and Discussion). As a negative control, the same test was performed in 2L of aged tap water. Every five minutes, we filmed each female’s cloaca from the bottom of the container without interrupting the test. Three pictures per video were taken, and the average of the three pictures per female and per event (every five minutes) was used to calculate the cloacal gaping response. The latter was calculated as Relative Cloacal Gaping (RCG), i.e., the width of the cloaca (measured orthogonal to the centre of the cloacal length) divided by the width of the tail base (measured at the hind limb initiation region). If the cloacal opening was invisible, the RCG was set to 0.00. Measurements were taken using ImageJ [27]. For statistical calculations, data of the two females were averaged for each testing container. The differences in [RCG tmax−RCG t0]-values (with RCG tmax being the RCGmax between t0 and t60) between courtship water and aged tap water were tested with the non-parametric two-tailed Mann-Whitney U test [28] using SPSS [29]. Twenty females were used in the test, and each female was used twice: one time for the CW test and once for the negative control. Ten females were first subjected to the CW test, the other 10 females were first subjected to the negative control.

Courtship water sampling for behavioral tests

A male and a female were placed in a plastic container (25x16x14 cm) filled with 2L of aged tap water. Once in amplexus, couples were monitored for courtship behavior and the amount of time that a male released one forelimb and imposed his protruded cloaca onto the female’s nose was measured. We termed this behavior cloacal imposition, and interpreted it as the moment of pheromone transfer [25]. We only retained water of courting couples if at least 30 minutes of male cloacal imposition occurred and if the female responded with cloacal gaping and a pinwheel movement. The couple was manually separated before spermatophore drop and animals were returned to their housing tanks. The successfully obtained courtship waters were mixed and equally divided over the test containers for the cloacal gaping tests.

Results and Discussion

Cloacal gaping precedes pinwheel behavior

Successful insemination in Pleurodeles waltl always results from pinwheel behavior (PW), i.e. the couple going into a circular movement that leads to spermatophore deposition by the male, and subsequent uptake by the female cloaca [3,24,25,30]. Our observations of the courtship behavior of 42 couples showed that, after the first contact in which they nudge the snout and/or body flank of the female, males could reach such pinwheel behavior in two ways (Fig 1A). First, when the female immediately responded positively to the nudging male (2/42 observations), the male was triggered to pivot around the female, while she followed him in pinwheel behavior without interlocked forelimbs (Fig 1A, outcome 1; S1 Video). Insemination without a preceding amplexus has rarely been described in the genus Pleurodeles [31], and seems to be most successful when a female is already highly receptive. Male courtship behavior that is dependent on the female’s responsiveness (facultative amplexus) has also been found in the red-spotted newt (Notophthalmus viridescens) [30,32]. As far as known, other salamandrid species never (most Modern Eurasian newts, Echinotriton, some Tylototriton species, Salamandrina) or always (Taricha, Calotriton, Euproctus, some Tylototriton species, Salamandra, Mertensiella) display a type of amplexus behavior during courtship [3,33].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Courtship behavior of Pleurodeles waltl.

A) Observed outcomes. The first outcome results from a female that is interested from the first male approach onwards. After male nudging, the couple immediately continues into pinwheel behavior without contact (i.e., skipping amplexus). The second and most common outcome is obtained in three steps, in the following order: nudging (first approach; FA), ventral amplexus (VA) altered with cloacal imposition (CI), and pinwheel behavior (PW) with one interlocked forelimb. In outcomes 1 and 2, female cloacal gaping (FCG) was always evident (indicated in green). In the third and fourth outcomes, female thanatosis and female struggle both lead to abortion of courtship, and no female cloacal gaping occurred (indicated in red). The number of times that each outcome occurred (out of 42 observations) is added. B) Graph of courtship observations. Number of times that courtship outcomes occurred during the observation of 42 couples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.g001

Most often (40/42 observations), the female did not directly engage in pinwheel behavior and showed no interest, in which cases the male swam under the female to clasp her forelimbs in a ventral amplexus (VA) with his arms in a 90° angle [24,25] (Fig 1A). Amplexus in newts enables the male to monopolize a female, but also facilitates applying his courtship pheromone glands to the restrained female [30]. Once in ventral amplexus, the male’s cloaca became swollen and protruded, and he started alternating his ventral amplexus position with cloacal impositions (CI; thereby releasing one forelimb—in 92.5% of the cases the left one; Fig 1A; S2 Video). Glands in the cloacal region are a known source of pheromones in multiple salamandrid species [2,4,6,30,3436], and these cloacal impositions are thus the obvious moments where the male applies his pheromones to the female’s nose [30]. After a period of alternating ventral amplexus with cloacal imposition, 29 females showed a first sign of receptivity by cloacal gaping (i.e. opening their cloaca; FCG), which finally (more than four hours later) resulted in coordinated pinwheel behavior, with the male still holding one of the female's forelimbs (Fig 1A, outcome 2; S3 Video).

It is generally believed that an amplected female amphibian does not have much choice other than to reproduce with the clasping male [4,23,37,38]. However, 11 amplected females avoided the stage of pinwheel behavior by either thanatosis (i.e., feigning death) or escaping from amplexus. Eight females avoided spermatophore pick-up by thanatosis (Fig 1A, outcome 3; S4 Video), during which males were not able to properly reach the female’s nose with their cloacal glands. Thanatosis was shown to be an effective strategy that invariably terminated the amplexus. To our knowledge, thanatosis in amphibians is so far only known as a defense mechanism against predators [4,39], but has not been observed in a sexual context. In fact, throughout the entire Animal Kingdom, this behavior is exceptional during courtship and is known only in spiders and insects. In spiders, thanatosis is performed by the male during courtship to avoid being eaten by the female [40], while in the fly Efferia varipes, it is similarly used as a strategy to get rid of harassing mates [41]. Finally, three females struggled themselves out of the male’s grip at the moment the male started his first cloacal imposition (Fig 1A, outcome 4). In each case, the male responded by tail-fanning, likely to transfer his pheromones in an alternative way. This behavior in itself never resulted in cloacal gaping, but once resulted in a new successful ventral amplexus with subsequent pinwheel behavior. Tail-fanning has already been described as a prelude to, and once even as a replacement of amplexus in Pleurodeles waltl [25,31], but we only observed it as this secondary, rather unsuccessful tactic.

Importantly, our observations showed that pinwheel behavior (whether obtained directly or after ventral amplexus) was always preceded by cloacal gaping, i.e., the female opening her cloaca as a prelude of spermatophore pick-up (Fig 1B). Our timing of complete courtship sequences indicated that this reaction occurred 12 to 51 minutes (N = 11; mean = 31 min; standard deviation = 14 min) after the first male cloacal imposition, while the transition from the first cloacal imposition to pinwheel behavior (for spermatophore pick-up; see below) took much longer, from 149 up to 310 minutes (N = 11; mean = 227 min; standard deviation = 55 min) (Table 1). In contrast, none of the females that escaped or feigned death showed cloacal gaping, indicating that this modification is a reliable indicator for a female being ready for spermatophore pick-up. Since most of the successful pinwheel behaviors were induced by cloacal imposition, and thus use of pheromones, we next investigated whether this link could be used for constructing a behavioral test with control over the presence of courtship pheromones in the absence of a male.

thumbnail
Table 1. Timing of courtship behaviors in Pleurodeles waltl couples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.t001

Female cloacal gaping as a bioassay for demonstrating courtship pheromone use in newts

To check whether female cloacal gaping could be evoked by male pheromones, we monitored the cloaca of females in courtship water (water in which a male has been courting, thus containing pheromones; see Methods) for 60 minutes. When females (N = 20) were exposed to courtship water, their cloaca observably changed from a closed to a more open state (Fig 2; Table 2). Although not every cloaca was in a completely closed state at the start of the test, gaping was enhanced in all of them during exposition to courtship water. Overall, the difference in relative cloacal gaping between the maximum (at tmax) and onset of the test (at t0) was significantly larger than that observed in the negative control (U = 0.00, N1 = N2 = 10, P < 0.001). These results suggest that pheromones in courtship water induce cloacal gaping in females, even in the absence of males. Cloacal gaping as a female response to males is also known in several snake species, where males perform caudocephalic waving (i.e. muscular contractions from cloaca to head) towards females [42,43], and the female responds by lifting her tail and gaping her cloaca [44,45]. In snakes and salamanders, a gaped female cloaca results from successful male courtship, and serves as the prelude for either inserting the hemipenis [46] or picking up the spermatophore, respectively.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Cloacal gaping in females.

The graphs represent female Relative Cloacal Gaping (RCG) at t0 and tmax in courtship water and aged tap water (negative control). RCG was measured as the cloacal width (cw) divided by the tail base width (tb). The width of the cloaca was measured orthogonal to the center of the cloacal length (cl). The tail base was measured at the hind limb initiation region. If the cloaca was closed, RCG was set to 0.00.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Relative Cloacal Gaping (RCG) in females of Pleurodeles waltl.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.t002

In internally fertilizing salamanders, males do not make use of a copulatory organ, but instead deposit a spermatophore in the environment that females take up with their cloaca. Opening of the cloaca is thus a logical prelude to insemination. Although cloacal gaping does not necessarily have to be dependent on pheromone use, our observations during a two-female test with Lissotriton helveticus in a previous study [6] indicate that cloacal modification—in the absence of a male but in the presence of male pheromones—is also evident in this species (S5 Video). We therefore hypothesize that our bioassay, with small modifications, will be useful for identifying pheromones in a broad range of internally fertilizing species with different courtship strategies.

Conclusion

It is generally acknowledged that many salamanders make use of courtship pheromones to persuade females into reproduction. These courtship pheromones are species-specific, are secreted by various glands, and are delivered by several behavioral tactics. However, studies that have effectively characterized such molecules until now have been limited [47], partly due to the difficulties of designing appropriate behavioral tests [48]. A behavioral test is often specifically designed and optimized for pheromone identification in a single species and its closest relatives. However, when aiming at understanding courtship pheromone evolution in salamanders, the availability of an assay that involves a comparable response across multiple species, genera or families, would be a strong asset. This study describes a behavioral test for identifying courtship pheromones that opens perspectives for use in multiple species of salamanders.

Supporting Information

S1 Video. Pinwheel behavior without interlocked forelimbs.

The male nudges the female during first contact. The female (recognized by her thicker belly) immediately responds with pinwheel behavior, so the couple circles around without interlocked forelimbs until sperm transfer takes place. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1612191.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.s001

(DOC)

S2 Video. Cloacal imposition.

When a male (below) is in ventral amplexus with a female (above), his cloaca protrudes and the male starts alternating this behavior with cloacal impositions: he releases one forelimb, and rotates around the female’s head to impose his cloaca on the female’s nose. After a while (Mean value of 31 minutes; calculated from the data in Table 1), the female responds by opening her cloaca, but other observable responses are not performed until much later. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1612192.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.s002

(DOC)

S3 Video. Pinwheel behavior with interlocked forelimbs.

After alternating ventral amplexus with cloacal imposition, the couple initiates pinwheel behavior, while one of the female’s forelimbs is still held by the male’s forelimb. This courtship behavior usually ends in a successful sperm transfer. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1612194.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.s003

(DOC)

S4 Video. Female thanatosis.

One of the strategies that females use to escape from an amplexus is thanatosis. The female feigns death until the male gives up. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1612195.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.s004

(DOC)

S5 Video. Cloacal gaping in Lissotriton helveticus.

A two-female test with two palmate newts in courtship water illustrates that the female showing following behavior under influence of SPF courtship pheromones [6] has a clearly extended cloaca. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1612196.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144985.s005

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We thank Jorgen De Winne for his advice on keeping P. waltl. We thank Ines Van Bocxlaer, David M Sever, Max Sparreboom and one anonymous reviewer for valuable comments on an earlier draft.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SJ FB. Performed the experiments: SJ FB. Analyzed the data: SJ FB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: FB. Wrote the paper: SJ FB.

References

  1. 1. Arnold SJ, Houck LD. Courtship pheromones: evolution by natural and sexual selection. In: Nitecki NH, editor. Biochemical aspects of evolutionary biology. Chicago, U.S.A.: University of Chicago Press; 1982. pp. 173–211.
  2. 2. Kikuyama S, Toyoda F. Sodefrin: a novel sex pheromone in a newt. Rev reprod. 1999;4(1): 1–4. pmid:10051096
  3. 3. Houck LD, Arnold SJ. Courthip and mating behavior (chapter 10). In: Sever DM, editor. Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Urodela Volume 1. Enfield, New Hampshire, U.S.A.: Science Publishers, Inc.; 2003. pp. 383–424.
  4. 4. Wells KD. The Ecology and Behavior of Amphibians. Chicago, U.S.A.: Chicago Press; 2007.
  5. 5. Woodley SK. Pheromonal communication in amphibians. J Comp Physiol A. 2010;196(10): 713–727.
  6. 6. Van Bocxlaer I, Treer D, Maex M, Vandebergh W, Janssenswillen S, Stegen G, et al. Side-by-side secretion of Late Palaeozoic diverged courtship pheromones in an aquatic salamander. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio. 2015;282(1803): 20142960.
  7. 7. Rollmann SM, Houck LD, Feldhoff RC. Proteinaceous pheromone affecting female receptivity in a terrestrial salamander. Science. 1999;285(5435): 1907–1909. pmid:10489368
  8. 8. Houck LD, Watts RA, Mead LM, Palmer CA, Arnold SJ, Feldhoff PW, et al. A candidate vertebrate pheromone, SPF, increases female receptivity in a salamander. In: Beynon RJ, Roberts SC, Wyatt TD, editors. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11. New York, U.S.A.: Springer; 2008. pp. 213–221.
  9. 9. Salthe SN. Courtship patterns and the phylogeny of the urodeles. Copeia. 1967;100–117.
  10. 10. Sever DM. Comparative anatomy and phylogeny of the cloacae of salamanders (Amphibia: Caudata) IV. Salamandridae. Anat Record. 1992;233(2): 229–244.
  11. 11. Toyoda F, Tanaka S, Matsuda K, Kikuyama S. Hormonal control of response to and secretion of sex attractants in Japanese newts. Physiol Behav. 1994;55(3): 569–576. pmid:8190778
  12. 12. Kikuyama S, Toyoda F, Ohmiya Y, Matsuda K, Tanaka S, Hayashi H. Sodefrin: a female-attracting peptide pheromone in newt cloacal glands. Science. 1995;267(5204): 1643–1645. pmid:7886452
  13. 13. Iwata T, Umezawa K, Toyoda F, Takahashi N, Matsukawa H, Yamamoto K, et al. Molecular cloning of newt sex pheromone precursor cDNAs: evidence for the existence of species-specific forms of pheromones. FEBS Lett. 1999;457(3): 400–404. pmid:10471817
  14. 14. Nakada T, Toyoda F, Iwata T, Yamamoto K, Conlon JM, Kato T, et al. Isolation, characterization and bioactivity of a region-specific pheromone,-Val8- sodefrin from the newt Cynops pyrrhogaster. Peptides. 2007;28(4): 774–780. pmid:17275139
  15. 15. Secondi J, Haerty W, Lodé T. Female Attraction to Conspecific Chemical Cues in the Palmate Newt Triturus helveticus. Ethology. 2005;111: 726–735.
  16. 16. Dawley M. Identification of sex through odors by male red-spotted newts, Notophthalmus viridescens. Herpetologica. 1984;40(2): 101–105.
  17. 17. Malacarne G, Bottoni L, Massa R, Vellano C. The abdominal gland of the crested newt: a possible source of courtship pheromones. Preliminary ethological and biochemical data. Monit Zool Ital. 1984;18(1): 33–39.
  18. 18. Malacarne G, Vellano C. Behavioral evidence of a courtship pheromone in the crested newt, Triturus cristatus carnifex Laurenti. Copeia. 1987;245–247.
  19. 19. Belvedere P, Colombo L, Malacarne G, Andredetti G. Comparative ethological and biochemical aspects of courtship pheromones in European newts. Monit Zool Ital. 1988;22(4): 397–403.
  20. 20. Poschadel JR, Rudolph A, Plath M. Non-visual mate choice in the Pyrenean mountain newt (Euproctus asper): females prefer small males. Acta Etholog. 2007;10(1): 35–40.
  21. 21. Treer D, Van Bocxlaer I, Matthijs S, Du Four D, Janssenswillen S, Willaert B, et al. Love is blind: indiscriminate female mating responses to male courtship pheromones in newts (Salamandridae). PloS one. 2013;8(2): e56538. pmid:23457580
  22. 22. Halliday TR. The courtship of European newts: an evolutionary perspective. In: Taylor DH, Guttman SI, editors. The reproductive biology of amphibians. New York, U.S.A.: Plenum Press. 1977. pp. 185–232.
  23. 23. Halliday TR. The evolution of courtship behavior in newts and salamanders. Adv Stud Behav. 1990;19: 137–169.
  24. 24. Gallien L. Elevage et comportement du Pleurodèle au laboratoire [Courtship and behavior of Pleurodeles in the laboratory]. B soc zool Fr. 1952;97: 456–461. French.
  25. 25. Arnold SJ. The evolution of courtship behavior in salamanders. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan. 1972.
  26. 26. Griffiths RA. Newts and Salamanders of Europe. London, U.K. and San Diego, U.S.A.: T. & A.D. Poyser Natural History/Academic Press; 1996.
  27. 27. Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9: 671–675. pmid:22930834
  28. 28. Dytham C. Choosing and using statistics: a biologist's guide. Chichester, U.S.A.: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.; 2011.
  29. 29. SPSS for Windows (Rel. 14.0.0). 2005. Chicago: SPSS Inc.
  30. 30. Arnold SJ. The evolution of courtship behavior in New World salamanders with some comments on Old World salamandrids. In: Taylor DH, Guttman SI, editors. The reproductive biology of amphibians. New York, U.S.A.: Plenum Press; 1977. pp. 141–183.
  31. 31. Salvador A, García-París M. Pleurodeles waltl (Michahelles, 1830)—Rippenmolch. In: Grossenbacher K, Thiesmeier B, editors. Handbuch der reptilien und Amphibien Europas, 4(I): Schwanzlurche (Urodela). Wiesbaden, Germany: Aula-Verlag; 1999. pp. 209–228.
  32. 32. Verrell P. The sexual behavior of the red-spotted newt, Notophthalmus viridescens (Amphibia: Urodela: Salamandridae). Anim Behav. 1982;30(4): 1224–1236.
  33. 33. Sparreboom M. Salamanders of the Old World. Zeist, the Netherlands: KNNV Publishing; 2014.
  34. 34. Sever DM. Cloacal anatomy of male salamanders in the families Ambystomatidae, Salamandridae, and Plethodontidae. Herpetologica. 1981;37(3): 142–155.
  35. 35. Osikowski A, Babik W, Grzmil P, Szymura JM. Multiple sex pheromone genes are expressed in the abdominal glands of the smooth newt (Lissotriton vulgaris) and Montandon's Newt (L. montandoni) (Salamandridae). Zool Sci. 2008;25: 587–592. pmid:18624569
  36. 36. Janssenswillen S, Vandebergh W, Treer D, Willaert B, Maex M, Van Bocxlaer I, et al. Origin and diversification of a salamander sex pheromone system. Mol Biol Evol. 2015;32(2): 472–480. pmid:25415963
  37. 37. Dole JW, Durant P. Movements and seasonal activity of Atelopus oxyrhynchus (Anura: Atelopodidae) in a Venezuelan cloud forest. Copeia. 1974;230–235.
  38. 38. Wells KD. The social behavior of anuran amphibians. Anim Behav. 1977;25: 666–693.
  39. 39. Brodie ED Jr. Salamander antipredator postures. Copeia. 1977;523–535.
  40. 40. Hansen LS, Gonzales SF, Toft S, Bilde T. Thanatosis as an adaptive male mating strategy in the nuptial gift–giving spider Pisaura mirabilis. Behav Ecol. 2008;19(3): 546–551.
  41. 41. Dennis DS, Lavigne RJ. Ethology of Efferia varipes with comments on species coexistence (Diptera: Asilidae). J Kansas Entomol Soc. 1976;49(1): 48–62.
  42. 42. Noble GK. The sense organs involved in the courtship of Storeria, Thamnophis and other snakes. B Am Mus Nat Hist. 1937;73: a7.
  43. 43. Gillingham JC. Social behavior. In: Seigel RA, Collins JT, Novak SS, editors. Snakes: ecology and evolutionary biology. New Jersey, U.S.A.: Blackburn; 1987. pp. 184–209.
  44. 44. Carpenter CC. Communication and displays of snakes. Am Zool. 1977;17(1): 217–223.
  45. 45. Shine R, Mason RT. Does large body size in males evolve to facilitate forcible insemination? A study on garter snakes. Evolution. 2005;59(11): 2426–2432. pmid:16396183
  46. 46. Greene MJ, Mason RT. Courtship, mating, and male combat of the brown tree snake, Boiga irregularis. Herpetologica. 2000;56(2): 166–174.
  47. 47. Cummins SF, Bowie JH. Pheromones, attractants and other chemical cues of aquatic organisms and amphibians. Nat Prod Rep. 2012;29: 642–658. pmid:22495567
  48. 48. Wyatt TD. Pheromones and animal behavior—chemical signals and signatures. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press; 2014.