Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

"What-Where-Which" Episodic Retrieval Requires Conscious Recollection and Is Promoted by Semantic Knowledge

  • Anne-Lise Saive ,

    anne-lise.saive@inserm.fr

    Affiliation Olfaction: from coding to memory team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028, University Lyon1, Lyon, F-69366, France

  • Jean-Pierre Royet,

    Affiliation Olfaction: from coding to memory team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028, University Lyon1, Lyon, F-69366, France

  • Samuel Garcia,

    Affiliation Olfaction: from coding to memory team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028, University Lyon1, Lyon, F-69366, France

  • Marc Thévenet,

    Affiliation Olfaction: from coding to memory team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028, University Lyon1, Lyon, F-69366, France

  • Jane Plailly

    Affiliation Olfaction: from coding to memory team, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U1028, University Lyon1, Lyon, F-69366, France

Abstract

Episodic memory is defined as the conscious retrieval of specific past events. Whether accurate episodic retrieval requires a recollective experience or if a feeling of knowing is sufficient remains unresolved. We recently devised an ecological approach to investigate the controlled cued-retrieval of episodes composed of unnamable odors (What) located spatially (Where) within a visual context (Which context). By combining the Remember/Know procedure with our laboratory-ecological approach in an original way, the present study demonstrated that the accurate odor-evoked retrieval of complex and multimodal episodes overwhelmingly required conscious recollection. A feeling of knowing, even when associated with a high level of confidence, was not sufficient to generate accurate episodic retrieval. Interestingly, we demonstrated that the recollection of accurate episodic memories was promoted by odor retrieval-cue familiarity and describability. In conclusion, our study suggested that semantic knowledge about retrieval-cues increased the recollection which is the state of awareness required for the accurate retrieval of complex episodic memories.

Introduction

Episodic memory is defined as the conscious retrieval of personal experiences occurring within a specific context [13]. Although recognition memory is known to comprise at least two different states of awareness: recollection and feeling of knowing (or familiarity) [46], their role in rich and detailed episodic memory remains unresolved. The respective involvement of each of these two processes has been typically assessed using the Remember/Know (R/K) procedure based on participants’ introspection [4]. The participants must report whether they recognize items on the basis of remembering contextual details or associative information (e.g., an image, an emotion, a personal experience) or knowing that the item is familiar without any conscious recollection. These two states of awareness represent two different cognitive processes that rely on partially distinct neural substrates [79] and are affected differently by factors such as retention delay and intentional encoding [10]. Currently, associative or relational recall, such as that involved in episodic memory, is assumed to rely mainly on recollection, because only a “Remember” response would provide precise and specific information from the studied event [1113]. The state of awareness underlying complex episodic memory has been experimentally tested only twice, with inconsistent results [14,15]. The objective of the present study was to examine the state of awareness associated with the retrieval of rich multidimensional episodes or, in other words, to determine whether accurate episodic retrieval requires a recollective experience or if a feeling of knowing is sufficient.

Researchers either test autobiographical memory by interrogating participants about real-life memories encoded in their past [1620] or they test the memorization of artificial episodes created in the laboratory using recognition tasks [2125]. To combine the richness of real-life memories investigated through an ecological approach and the control of memory accuracy that is possible in a laboratory-based approach, we recently developed a novel laboratory-ecological task [2628]. This approach allows for the controlled study of the cued-retrieval of trial-unique and complex multimodal episodes composed of unnamable odors (What) located spatially (Where) within a visual context (Which context).

In the current study, we combined the R/K procedure with our laboratory-ecological approach [26,28] in an original way to investigate (i) the respective requirement of odor recollection and odor familiarity in the accurate retrieval of odor-evoked episodic memories and (ii) the properties of the odor retrieval-cues and the breathing modulations accompanying each of these states of awareness.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

The study has been approved by the local Institutional Review Board in accordance with French regulations for biomedical experiments with healthy volunteers (Ethical Committee of CPP Sud-Est IV: CPP 11/007, ID RCB: 2010-A-01529-30). The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The participants provided written informed consent.

Participants

Twenty-three healthy participants [15 women, age: 21.9 ± 2.02 years (mean ± standard deviation)] consented to participate in the experiment and received 30 euros in compensation. All participants were right-handed and reported normal senses of smell and no visual impairments.

Stimuli and materials

Stimuli.

Eighteen odorants, of which most have been used in earlier studies [26,28], were selected based on their distinctiveness and relatively low identifiability and familiarity and were subdivided into two sets (Sets 1 and 2) of nine odorants each. Set 1 consisted of butanol, carrot, cis-3-hexenyl salicylate, dihydromyrcenol, heptanon, methyl octine carbonate, musk, rosemarel and stemone. Set 2 consisted of 9-decen-1-ol, basil, birch oil, citronellol, ethyl acetyl acetate, linalyl acetate, rose oxide, tobacco and tomato.

The odorants were presented using a twenty-channel computer-controlled olfactometer that was connected to the nostrils. The participants were requested to breathe normally and avoid sniffing behaviors. Each participant’s respiratory signal was acquired using a nasal cannula and was used to trigger the odor stimulation through an airflow sensor. The airflow rate was set at 3 l/min, and the odorants were delivered over 4 s.

Three landscape pictures presented in full-screen view constituted the visual contexts (a coastal cliff, a countryside and a mountain landscape). Three orange circles in each image symbolized the three spatial locations associated with an odor (Fig 1A).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Episodic memory task design and Remember/Know procedure.

(A) The three spatio-contextual environments of the episodes. Orange circles represent the spatial locations associated with an odor. (B) The temporal course of the encoding and retrieval sessions, with an example of a retrieval trial. During the encoding, the participants discovered one episode per day over three days. On the fourth day, the memory of the episodes was evaluated using an odor recognition task (R/K procedure) followed for the R and K trials by an episodic retrieval task. K, Know; R, Remember; T, Trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143767.g001

Multidimensional episodes.

Three multidimensional episodes were created. Each episode was composed of three odors (What) associated with specific locations (Where) within a visual context (Which context). The contexts, spatial locations and odors differed between episodes. To limit associative semantic processes, the odors, spatial locations and visual contexts were arbitrarily linked. An in-house LabView software (National Instruments®, Austin, TX, USA) connected to the olfactometer controlled the presentation of odors, pictures and circles and recorded the participants’ responses and breathing throughout the experiment. The participants used a trackball to interact with the software. When they clicked on a circle, the odor stimulus was delivered at the beginning of the subsequent expiration, allowing the odor to be perceived at the beginning of the next inspiration. The volume, amplitude and duration of each inspiratory cycle were measured, and the respiratory frequency was calculated.

Experimental Procedure

The experimental procedure consisted of four sessions performed over the course of four successive days. The encoding took place during the first three sessions, and the retrieval occurred on the fourth session (Fig 1B). A full night of sleep followed each of the encoding sessions to promote consolidation and to reduce interference [29,30]. Participants completed the four sessions at the same time of day to limit the differential influences of internal states (hunger, satiety) on olfactory and cognitive processes between sessions [31,32].

Encoding session.

During the encoding, the participants freely discovered one episode per day for 7 min (Fig 1B). They were asked to explore all dimensions of the episode as much as possible by paying attention to the background picture, the circles superimposed on this background, and the odors that were delivered when clicking on the circles. No memorization instruction was given, thereby ensuring free encoding in a manner similar to what occurs in real-life situations. The participants were informed that they would be questioned about their perception of the episodes on the fourth day (see S1 Fig for the complete participant encoding instructions). The order of the three episodes was counterbalanced between participants.

Retrieval session.

To investigate the states of awareness accompanying episodic retrieval, we adapted the retrieval procedure of Saive et al. [28] to allow for a one-step R/K procedure (Fig 1B). The retrieval session consisted of one block of 18 trials, corresponding to the presentation of 9 target odors randomly intermixed with 9 distractor odors. The use of the Set 1 or Set 2 odorants as targets or distractors was counterbalanced between the participants.

Odor recognition task. For each odor, the participants had to decide if they recognized the smell or not (“Yes” or “No” response). If they did, they had to determine whether they “remembered” the odor from the studied episodes (“R” response) or whether they just “knew” that it was previously perceived (“K” response). The R response represented a conscious recollection of some specific contextual information associated with the odor during the encoding (e.g., a picture, a personal experience), whereas the K response represented a feeling of knowing in the absence of conscious recollection of the odor’s previous presentation. When giving their responses, the participants were asked to simultaneously rate their subjective level of confidence using a slider on a non-graduated scale, a procedure adapted from Ingram et al. [33]. The distinction between the R/K responses and the confidence strength was emphasized, and the participants were told that both recollection and familiarity can vary in strength [33,34]. Detailed instructions and examples explaining the differences between R and K judgments were given to the participants, and their comprehension was checked before the retrieval session (see S2 Fig for the complete participant retrieval instructions).

Episodic retrieval task. Following the R and K responses, the participants were asked to retrieve the entire episode associated with the odor by choosing both a visual context (one of the three pictures) and a location (one of the nine circles superimposed on the chosen picture). They also had to rate their level of confidence for both the picture and the location using a slider on a non-graduated scale. A response was considered correct if the participants selected both the accurate context and the specific location previously associated with the odor during the encoding. If the participants rejected the odor (“No” response), they rested until the next trial for 3 s. All the retrieval steps were self-paced. At the end of each trial, the participants were asked to explain their R/K responses. These justifications were used to correct for misattributions if necessary.

Odor retrieval-cue evaluation. At the end of the retrieval session, the participants were asked to rate the odors in terms of pleasantness, intensity, and familiarity using non-graduated scales and to describe them if possible.

Data analysis

During retrieval, recognition memory performance was assessed using parameters from the signal detection theory [35]. Four response categories were defined: Hit and Miss corresponded to the accurate recognition and the inaccurate rejection of target odors, respectively, while correct rejection (CR) and false alarm (FA) corresponded to the accurate rejection and the inaccurate recognition of distractor odors, respectively. A memory score (d’L) reflecting the participant’s ability to discriminate between the target and distractor odors was calculated [36] (see S3 Fig for detailed calculations).

In the episodic retrieval test, we defined four types of responses depending on the accuracy of the memory triggered by accurate odor recognition (Hit). When the participants correctly recognized the target odors, they could accurately remember both the location and the context (WWW), the location only (WWhere), the context only (WWhich) or be mistaken about both dimensions (What). The theoretical proportions of these episodic combinations were 0.019, 0.037, 0.148 and 0.296, respectively (see S4 Fig for detailed calculations). The WWhere response occurred only once for one participant and was therefore excluded from the analyses. The number of R/K responses was calculated for the response categories (FA, Hit, WWW, WWhich and What), and the subscripts R or K were added to indicate the corresponding conditions (e.g., FAR, FAK, WWWR, WWWK). The recollection score (Rec; Jacoby, 1991) reflecting the proportion of accurate recollection was calculated as follows: where HitR and FAR represent the numbers of accurate and inaccurate recollections, respectively. With the probability of randomly giving an R, K or No response being equal, the calculation of theoretical proportions of the R/K episodic combinations was 0.006 for WWW (i.e., 0.019/3), 0.049 for WWhich (i.e., 0.148/3), and 0.099 for What (i.e., 0.296/3).

The confidence evaluations were a posteriori transformed into values ranging from 0 to 1. The confidence for the episodic retrieval responses was defined as the mean of the context and location confidences. The breathing parameters (i.e., the volume, amplitude and duration of the inspiratory cycles and the respiratory frequency) were extracted between the time at which the odor was delivered and the R/K responses. The means of these variables were determined for all R/K responses.

The odor retrieval-cue pleasantness, intensity, and familiarity ratings were a posteriori transformed into values ranging from 0 to 10. For familiarity, a value of 5 represented the boundary between unfamiliar and familiar odors. Odors rated as 5 were assigned to both groups. For pleasantness, values below 4 represented unpleasant odors, values between 4 and 6 represented neutral odors and values above 6 represented pleasant odors. The odor descriptions were transformed into scores of 1 and 0 based on whether the participants provided any description (e.g., medicine, spicy, sour, garden, soft, subtle, stinky cheese, oppressive) or not (there were no veridical labels because odors were uncommon odors).

Statistical analysis

The main effects of the factors and interactions were determined using repeated measures ANOVAs for the variable “Number of responses.” Confidence evaluations and breathing measures for odor and context were analyzed using two-way ANOVAs to allow for statistical comparisons even in the absence of some conditions for some participants. ANOVAs were followed by post-hoc bilateral Student’s t-tests if the main effects and/or interactions were significant. The “Proportions of responses” were compared to the respective theoretical proportions using Student’s t-tests. The effects were considered significant at p < 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed using Statistica (StatSoft®, Tulsa, OK, USA).

Results

Odor retrieval-cue evaluation

On average, the odorants were perceived as relatively neutral (4.83 ± 1.13, range: 3.36–6.74), moderately intense (6.22 ± 0.76, range: 4.14–7.24), with all odors being perceivable, moderately familiar (5.10 ± 1.25, range: 3.76–7.10), and moderately describable (0.63 ± 0.43).

Memory performance

Odor recognition.

The participants were very proficient in recognizing old odors and rejecting new ones, as indicated by the high memory score (dL = 3.30 ± 1.37) and numbers of correct responses (Hit = 7.57 ± 1.44 of 9 target odors; CR = 7.52 ± 1.16 of 9 distractor odors), which were far above the chance level (t(22)’s > 10.21, ps < 0.001). This pattern of behavioral performance replicates our previous results [26,28] and indicates that the addition of the R/K procedure did not alter the recognition performance.

When recognizing an odor, the participants made a simultaneous R/K judgment (Fig 2A). A two-way R/K judgment x accuracy (Hit/FA) ANOVA revealed a significant effect of the R/K response [F(1, 22) = 42.07, p < 0.001] due to a higher number of R responses (6.83 ± 2.33) than K responses (2.65 ± 4.47). A significant R/K-by-accuracy interaction [F(1, 22) = 32.39, p < 0.001] revealed that the pattern of R/K responses was obtained for accurate recognitions (HitR vs. HitK; p < 0.001) but not for inaccurate recognitions (FAR vs. FAK; p > 0.35). Accurate recognitions were preferentially associated with R responses, whereas false memories were indifferently associated with R or K responses. Considering R responses only, the number of accurate recognitions (HitR) was higher than the number of inaccurate recognitions (FAR) (p < 0.001). These results were consistent with a recollection score higher than the chance value of zero (Rec = 0.55 ± 0.22; t(22) = 12.07, p < 0.001) and reflected that the accurate odor recognition was mainly related to recollective experience. Considering K responses only, the number of Hit was higher than the number of FA (HitK vs. FAK; p = 0.019), indicating that the feeling of knowing was sufficient to achieve odor retrieval-cue recognition.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Memory performance.

Mean numbers of R/K responses as a function of A) the accurate (Hit) and inaccurate (FA) odor recognition, and B) the episodic retrieval responses (WWW, WWhich and What). The dashed horizontal lines indicate the chance levels of the WWW, WWhich and What responses. Vertical bars represent the SD; in black, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; in white, *** p < 0.001 above chance level.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143767.g002

Episodic retrieval.

Following the accurate recognition of previously perceived odors (Hit), the participants were asked to retrieve the contextual and spatial dimensions of the episode associated with the odor. The numbers of accurate and inaccurate episodic retrieval responses (WWW: 3.09 ± 1.31; What: 3.09 ± 1.53) were significantly higher than the number of incomplete retrieval responses (WWhich: 1.35 ± 0.88) [F(2, 44) = 11.14, p < 0.001; post-hocs, ps < 0.001]. Moreover, the number of accurate episodic retrieval responses (WWW) was far above the chance level [t(22) = 10.68, p < 0.001], whereas the numbers of incomplete (WWhich) and inaccurate (What) episodic retrieval responses were not significantly different from chance [t(22)’s < 1.31, ps > 0.20]. Thus, either the participants retrieved complete episodes triggered by accurate odor recognition or they answered randomly.

The accuracy of the episodic retrieval triggered by odor recognitions associated with either R or K responses was examined (Fig 2B). A two-way R/K x Episodic ANOVA showed a significant interaction between both factors [F(2, 44) = 14.62, p < 0.001]. A higher number of R than K responses was observed in the three episodic conditions (WWW, p < 0.001; WWhich, p = 0.034; What, p < 0.001). Higher numbers of R responses for WWW and K responses for What were found compared to the other two episodic conditions (R responses: WWhich, p < 0.001 and What, p = 0.010; K responses: WWW, p = 0.010 and WWhich, p = 0.023). Additionally, the number of R responses was significantly higher than the number of theoretical random responses in the WWW condition (t(22) = 9.76, p < 0.001) but was not significantly different from chance in the WWhich and What conditions [t(22) = 0.39 and t(22) = 1.27, ps > 0.20]. The numbers of K responses were not significantly different or were significantly lower than the corresponding numbers of random responses in the WWW [t(22) = 1.66, p > 0.09], WWhich and What [t(22) = -4.09 and t(22) = -3.57, ps < 0.001] conditions. Briefly, the complete and accurate episodic retrieval was observed only when the participants accurately remembered the information (WWWR) but not when their responses were based on a feeling of knowing (WWWK).

Confidence evaluations.

We examined the confidence of the odor recognition and of the visuospatial context retrieval and tested whether it differed according to the R/K and episodic retrieval (WWW, WWhich, What) responses. For the odor recognition, a two-way R/K x Episodic ANOVA revealed a significant effect of R/K responses [F(1, 83) = 8.67, p = 0.004] (Fig 3A, Odor) but no significant effect of Episodic responses [F(2, 83) = 1.54, p > 0.21] and no significant interaction between both factors [F(2, 83) = 0.89, p > 0.41]. For the visuospatial context retrieval, we observed a significant effect of the R/K factor [F(1, 83) = 24.85, p < 0.001] (Fig 3A, Context) but no significant effect of the Episodic factor [F(2, 83) = 2.25, p > 0.10] and no significant interaction between both factors [F(2, 83) = 0.16, p > 0.84]. Briefly, the confidence of participants in their responses during both odor recognition and visuospatial context retrieval was higher when they experienced recollection compared to a feeling of knowing.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Confidence ratings and episodic retrieval.

A) Mean levels of confidence of the odor recognition response (Odor) and the visuospatial context retrieval (Context) given the R/K responses. B) Mean numbers of R/K responses in the high confidence episodic retrieval. Vertical bars represent the SD; in black, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; in white, *** p < 0.001 above chance level.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143767.g003

High confidence responses.

To disentangle the R/K responses from the confidence judgments, we considered only the high-confidence R/K responses, in which odor confidence was equal or superior to the mean odor confidence (0.73 ± 0.19) (Fig 3B). First, we confirmed that proportions of high-confidence accurate odor recognitions (Hit) compared to the total number of recognitions (Hit + FA) were similar when considering R responses [HitR/(HitR+FAR) = 0.91 ±0.15] or K responses [HitK/(HitK+FAK) = 0.73 ± 0.44; F(1, 34) = 3.15, p > 0.08]. This result indicated that odor recognition accuracy was not related to R/K responses in high confidence responses. Second, a two-way R/K x Episodic ANOVA on the number of high-confidence responses revealed similar results to those obtained by including all confidence responses (see 3.2.2 Episodic Retrieval) [R/K: F(1, 22) = 43.45, p < 0.001; Episodic: F(2, 44) = 11.93, p < 0.001; R/K-by-Episodic: F(2, 44) = 14.78, p < 0.001]. Consistently, the number of WWWR was significantly above chance (t(22) = 10.46, p < 0.001) and the number of WWWK responses did not differ from the number of random responses (t(22) = 1.39, p > 0.19). In other words, a feeling of knowing, even when associated with a high level of confidence, was not sufficient to generate accurate episodic retrieval.

Retrieval-cue properties and breathing modulations.

Familiarity. We examined whether the R/K and the Episodic retrieval (WWW, WWhich, What) responses varied as a function of the familiarity of odor retrieval-cues (Unfamiliar, Familiar). A two-way ANOVA revealed that the familiarity of odors significantly influenced the R/K responses [F(1, 22) = 7.33, p = 0.002], but not the Episodic retrieval [F(2, 44) = 0.81, p > 0.44], and no significant interaction between these factors was found [F(2, 44) = 2.53, p > 0.08]. The familiar odors generated more recollective experience than the unfamiliar odors (Fig 4A). Furthermore, the odors’ familiarity was significantly positively correlated with the odors’ describability [r = 0.80, t(1, 18) = 5.35, p < 0.001, Pearson’s test] (Fig 4B). The more familiar the odors, the more they were described by the participants.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Odor familiarity and describability.

A) Mean numbers of R/K responses as a function of familiarity of odors. Fam, Familiar; Unfam, Unfamiliar; Vertical bars represent the SD; ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. B) Correlation between familiarity and describability of odors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143767.g004

Pleasantness. A two-way R/K x Episodic ANOVA showed that the pleasantness of the odor retrieval-cues (Pleasant, Neutral, Unpleasant) had no significant effect on R/K [F(2, 44) = 0.04, p > 0.95] and Episodic retrieval [F(4, 88) = 0.62, p > 0.64] responses. No significant interaction between these factors [F(4, 88) = 0.41, p > 0.79] was found.

Breathing. We explored whether the volumes, durations, amplitudes and frequencies of the inspirations measured during retrieval varied as a function of R/K and Episodic responses (WWW, WWhich, What). Two-way ANOVAs revealed that the inspiration measures did not vary as a function of R/K responses and Episodic retrieval responses [F(1, 83)’s < 2.15, ps > 0.13 and F(2, 83)’s < 1.50, ps > 0.22, respectively], and no significant interactions between these factors were found [F(2, 83)’s < 0.36, ps > 0.69].

Discussion

The present study examined the involvement of recollection and feeling of knowing in complex episodic memory retrieval by investigating the odor-triggered retrieval of rich and close-to-real-life episodes. Although these two states of awareness supported accurate recognition memory of the odor, the retrieval of the full episode overwhelmingly required recollection. The feeling of knowing was insufficient, even when only high-level confidence responses were concerned. Interestingly, recollective experience was promoted by odor familiarity and describability. Higher semantic knowledge about odor retrieval-cues promoted the conscious recollection that was required for the retrieval of accurate episodic memories.

Only two studies have already investigated the requirement of recollection in cued-retrieval of complex episodic memories. First, Holland and Smulders [37] developed a “What-Where-When” memory task, in which the participants had to remember the locations of a room in which they hid coins of various values on two consecutive days. No R/K procedure was conducted, but the participants reported using a mental time travel strategy to recall the spatial locations that suggested a recollective experience. Second, Easton et al. [15] created a task in which the participants were questioned about two episodes immediately after their explicit encoding. By choosing one of two answer options, the participants answered questions about either the order of the power-point slide (When) or the visual background (Which) associated with an abstract symbol (What) located in a particular spot on the screen (Where). In contrast to the “What-Where-When” memory task, in which familiarity (i.e., the strength of the memory trace) was sufficient, the “What-Where-Which” memory task was accurately performed only using recollection. Our experiment confirmed the necessity of recollection in the accurate retrieval of complex episodes and validated the Remember/Know procedure in an ecological setting. Importantly, when the participants accurately recognized the odors, either they retrieved complete and accurate episodes or they answered randomly. This result is in agreement with the idea we previously proposed that when the binding between the odors and the spatio-contextual dimensions of the episodes is successful, the odor recognition and the episodic retrieval process collapsed into a unique memory process [28]. Together, these findings suggest that when accurate the retrieval of complex episodes is underpinned by one unique memory process involving recollection.

The cross-modal nature and the low semantic content of the episodes could explain why their retrieval was based on conscious recollection. In our task, the participants were asked to retrieve specific episodes comprised of dimensions of various modalities. The associations were arbitrary and included unfamiliar and hard-to-identify odors, limiting the use of semantic links to bind episode dimensions together. In associative recognition studies, the contribution of recollection and familiarity depends on the semantic relationships between items and on the cross-modality of the associations. Recollection is required to recognize arbitrary associations between items [12,38]. On the one hand, between-item and between-domain associations, that are associations respectively formed between various types of items or modalities (e.g., faces and voices), rely on recollection more than intra-item or within-domain associations [39,40]. On the other hand, familiarity is greater for intra-item associations (e.g., paired words forming a compound word) than for within-domain associations (e.g., unrelated words) [41]. Overall, in associative recognition studies, the more distant the items of the associations in terms of modality or semantic relationships, the more their recognition requires recollection. Our episodic memory study extended these observations by demonstrating that the more distant the dimensions of the episodes in terms of modality or semantic relationships, the more their retrieval requires recollection.

Even if odors were hardly identifiable, our results revealed that the semantic knowledge about the odor retrieval-cues favored the conscious recollection of the entire complex episode. The semantic knowledge was figured by the odor familiarity and describability. Familiar odors have been shown to evoke semantic information that enables their identification [42] and to generate greater recollective experience in recognition memory [43]. Here, odor familiarity, which was intrinsically related to the verbal describability of the odor, triggered more episodic retrieval based on remembering than on the feeling of knowing. Our findings confirmed the widespread hypothesis that semantic knowledge increases conscious recollection but not the feeling of knowing in the remembrance of episodic memories (for a review [10], but [44]). How does semantic knowledge promote recollection? First, when retrieving experienced events, we piece together our memory of the items (persons, objects) and the context in which we encoded these items. The context of the events is part of a lifetime period and contains semantic and conceptual information (e.g., locations, dates, relationships) [45,46]. Here, our results argued for the idea that the feeling of familiarity enhances the description of the odors even if they were mostly limited to an adjective or an olfactory note (e.g., minty, spicy). Associating an odor with prior semantic knowledge seems to promote its recognition and the recollection of contextual details. Second, processing information in relation to the self is also known to increase the recollective processes during memory retrieval [47,48]. We could suppose that the familiarity evoked by previously encountered odors enhances self-reference processes during encoding and therefore increases recollection during episodic retrieval.

Finally, the influence of the confidence level accompanying recollection and familiarity requires further discussion. Many studies examining the subjective processes accompanying recognition memory have shown that, on average, recollection is associated with high confidence responses, whereas a feeling of knowing is associated with low confidence responses [4952]. During odor recognition and episodic retrieval, our results were consistent with these observations and corroborated the association of recollection with a higher level of confidence than with the feeling of knowing. Thus, it could be claimed that the impossibility of the feeling of knowing to support accurate episodic retrieval is related to the low confidence the participants had in their responses rather than to their state of awareness. However, even in restricting our analysis to responses with high confidence values, our observation of same results argued for the requirement of recollection in retrieving episodic memories.

Conclusion

Briefly, our results showed that cross-modal “What-Where-Which” accurate odor-evoked episodic retrieval overwhelmingly relied on recollective processes. Additionally, the feeling of familiarity evoked by odor retrieval-cues increased the recollective experience, leading to the accurate remembering of the odor and its associated dimensions. Familiar odors benefited from greater semantic knowledge than unfamiliar odors, which induced a stronger episodic memory trace and a greater recollection during episodic retrieval. Together, our study suggested that semantic access to episodic memory promoted by odor retrieval-cue familiarity increased recollection, which is the state of awareness required for accurately retrieving complex episodic memories.

Supporting Information

S4 Fig. Calculation of theoretical proportions of episodic combinations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143767.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS) and research grants from the Région Rhône-Alpes (CIBLE 10 015 772 01). It was performed within the framework of the LABEX Cortex (NR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon, within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR). Anne-Lise Saive was funded by the Roudnitska Foundation.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ALS JP. Performed the experiments: ALS JP. Analyzed the data: ALS JPR SG. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SG MT. Wrote the paper: ALS JPR JP.

References

  1. 1. Tulving E. Episodic and semantic memory. Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press; 1972. pp. 381–405.
  2. 2. Tulving E. Episodic memory and common sense: how far apart? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356: 1505–1515. pmid:11571040
  3. 3. Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53: 1–25. pmid:11752477
  4. 4. Tulving E. Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol Can. 1985;26: 1–12.
  5. 5. Gardiner JM, Java RI. Recognition memory and awareness: An experiential approach. Eur J Cogn Psychol. 1993;5: 337–346.
  6. 6. Yonelinas AP. Components of episodic memory: the contribution of recollection and familiarity. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2001;356: 1363–1374.
  7. 7. Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22: 425–444; discussion 444–489. pmid:11301518
  8. 8. Duarte A, Ranganath C, Winward L, Hayward D, Knight RT. Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004;18: 255–272. pmid:14741312
  9. 9. Rissman J, Greely HT, Wagner AD. Detecting individual memories through the neural decoding of memory states and past experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107: 9849–9854. pmid:20457911
  10. 10. Yonelinas AP. The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research. J Mem Lang. 2002;46: 441–517.
  11. 11. Mandler G. Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurence. Psychol Rev. 1980;87: 252–271.
  12. 12. Hockley WE, Consoli A. Familiarity and recollection in item and associative recognition. Mem Cognit. 1999;27: 657–664. pmid:10479824
  13. 13. Yonelinas AP. The contribution of recollection and familiarity to recognition and source-memory judgments: a formal dual-process model and an analysis of receiver operating characteristics. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1999;25: 1415–1434. pmid:10605829
  14. 14. Holland SM, Smulders TV. Do humans use episodic memory to solve a What-Where-When memory task? Anim Cogn. 2011;14: 95–102. pmid:20661614
  15. 15. Easton A, Webster LAD, Eacott MJ. The episodic nature of episodic-like memories. Learn Mem. 2012;19: 146–150. pmid:22411421
  16. 16. Fink GR, Markowitsch HJ, Reinkemeier M, Bruckbauer T, Kessler J, Heiss WD. Cerebral representation of one’s own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J Neurosci. 1996;16: 4275–4282. pmid:8753888
  17. 17. Levine B, Turner GR, Tisserand D, Hevenor SJ, Graham SJ, McIntosh AR. The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering: a prospective functional MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16: 1633–1646. pmid:15601525
  18. 18. Piolino P, Giffard-Quillon G, Desgranges B, Chételat G, Baron J-C, Eustache F. Re-experiencing old memories via hippocampus: a PET study of autobiographical memory. NeuroImage. 2004;22: 1371–83. pmid:15219608
  19. 19. Nadel L, Campbell J, Ryan L. Autobiographical memory retrieval and hippocampal activation as a function of repetition and the passage of time. Neural Plast. 2007;2007: 90472. pmid:18274617
  20. 20. Janata P. The neural architecture of music-evoked autobiographical memories. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19: 2579–2594. pmid:19240137
  21. 21. Konishi S, Wheeler ME, Donaldson DI, Buckner RL. Neural correlates of episodic retrieval success. NeuroImage. 2000;12: 276–286. pmid:10944410
  22. 22. Daselaar SM, Veltman DJ, Rombouts SARB, Raaijmakers JGW, Jonker C. Neuroanatomical correlates of episodic encoding and retrieval in young and elderly subjects. Brain. 2003;126: 43–56. pmid:12477696
  23. 23. Donaldson DI, Wheeler ME, Petersen SE. Remember the source: dissociating frontal and parietal contributions to episodic memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22: 377–391. pmid:19400677
  24. 24. Royet JP, Morin-Audebrand L, Cerf-Ducastel B, Haase L, Issanchou S, Murphy C, et al. True and false recognition memories of odors induce distinct neural signatures. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5: 65. pmid:21811450
  25. 25. Herholz SC, Halpern AR, Zatorre RJ. Neuronal Correlates of Perception, Imagery, and Memory for Familiar Tunes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24: 1382–1397. pmid:22360595
  26. 26. Saive AL, Ravel N, Thévenet M, Royet JP, Plailly J. A novel experimental approach to episodic memory in humans based on the privileged access of odors to memories. J Neurosci Methods. 2013;213: 22–31. pmid:23201236
  27. 27. Saive AL, Royet JP, Plailly J. A review on the neural bases of episodic odor memory: from laboratory-based to autobiographical approaches. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8: 240. pmid:25071494
  28. 28. Saive AL, Royet JP, Ravel N, Thévenet M, Garcia S, Plailly J. A unique memory process modulated by emotion underpins successful odor recognition and episodic retrieval in humans. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8: 203. pmid:24936176
  29. 29. Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science. 2001;294: 1048–52. pmid:11691982
  30. 30. Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature. 2005;437: 1272–8. pmid:16251952
  31. 31. Jiang T, Soussignan R, Rigaud D, Martin S, Royet JP, Brondel L, et al. Alliesthesia to food cues: heterogeneity across stimuli and sensory modalities. Physiol Behav. 2008;95: 464–470. pmid:18675834
  32. 32. Plailly J, Luangraj N, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S, Royet JP, Sulmont-Rossé C. Alliesthesia is greater for odors of fatty foods than of non-fat foods. Appetite. 2011;57: 615–622. pmid:21801771
  33. 33. Ingram KM, Mickes L, Wixted JT. Recollection can be weak and familiarity can be strong. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2012;38: 325–339. pmid:21967320
  34. 34. Migo EM, Mayes AR, Montaldi D. Measuring recollection and familiarity: Improving the remember/know procedure. Conscious Cogn. 2012;21: 1435–1455. pmid:22846231
  35. 35. Lockhart R, Murdock B. Memory and the theory of signal detection. Psychol Bull. 1970;74: 100–9.
  36. 36. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1988;117: 34–50. pmid:2966230
  37. 37. Holland SM, Smulders TV. Do humans use episodic memory to solve a What-Where-When memory task? Anim Cogn. 2011;14: 95–102. pmid:20661614
  38. 38. Donaldson DI, Rugg MD. Recognition memory for new associations: electrophysiological evidence for the role of recollection. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36: 377–395. pmid:9699947
  39. 39. Mayes A, Montaldi D, Migo E. Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11: 126–135. pmid:17270487
  40. 40. Tibon R, Gronau N, Scheuplein A-L, Mecklinger A, Levy DA. Associative recognition processes are modulated by the semantic unitizability of memoranda. Brain Cogn. 2014;92C: 19–31. pmid:25463136
  41. 41. Giovanello KS, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. The contribution of familiarity to associative memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44: 1859–1865. pmid:16643967
  42. 42. Royet JP, Paugam-Moisy H, Rouby C, Zighed D, Nicoloyannis N, Amghar S, et al. Is short-term odour recognition predictable from odour profile? Chem Senses. 1996;21: 553–66. pmid:8902284
  43. 43. Larsson M, Oberg C, Backman L. Recollective experience in odor recognition: influences of adult age and familiarity. Psychol Res. 2006;70: 68–75. pmid:15480757
  44. 44. Greve A, van Rossum MCW, Donaldson DI. Investigating the functional interaction between semantic and episodic memory: Convergent behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for the role of familiarity. NeuroImage. 2007;34: 801–814. pmid:17112741
  45. 45. Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev. 2000;107: 261–288. pmid:10789197
  46. 46. Conway MA. Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47: 2305–2313. pmid:19524094
  47. 47. Conway MA, Dewhurst SA. The self and recollective experience. Appl Cogn Psychol. 1995;9: 1–19.
  48. 48. Lalanne J, Rozenberg J, Grolleau P, Piolino P. The self-reference effect on episodic memory recollection in young and older adults and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2013;10: 1107–1117. pmid:24156261
  49. 49. Yonelinas AP. Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: evidence for a dual-process model. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20: 1341–54. pmid:7983467
  50. 50. Dunn JC. Remember-know: a matter of confidence. Psychol Rev. 2004;111: 524–542. pmid:15065921
  51. 51. Dunn JC. The dimensionality of the remember-know task: a state-trace analysis. Psychol Rev. 2008;115: 426–446. pmid:18426296
  52. 52. Yonelinas AP, Aly M, Wang W-C, Koen JD. Recollection and familiarity: examining controversial assumptions and new directions. Hippocampus. 2010;20: 1178–1194. pmid:20848606