Correction
26 Mar 2015: The PLOS ONE Staff (2015) Correction: Low Message Sensation Health Promotion Videos Are Better Remembered and Activate Areas of the Brain Associated with Memory Encoding. PLOS ONE 10(3): e0123143. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123143 View correction
Figures
Abstract
Greater sensory stimulation in advertising has been postulated to facilitate attention and persuasion. For this reason, video ads promoting health behaviors are often designed to be high in “message sensation value” (MSV), a standardized measure of sensory intensity of the audiovisual and content features of an ad. However, our previous functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) study showed that low MSV ads were better remembered and produced more prefrontal and temporal and less occipital cortex activation, suggesting that high MSV may divert cognitive resources from processing ad content. The present study aimed to determine whether these findings from anti-smoking ads generalize to other public health topics, such as safe sex. Thirty-nine healthy adults viewed high- and low MSV ads promoting safer sex through condom use, during an fMRI session. Recognition memory of the ads was tested immediately and 3 weeks after the session. We found that low MSV condom ads were better remembered than the high MSV ads at both time points and replicated the fMRI patterns previously reported for the anti-smoking ads. Occipital and superior temporal activation was negatively related to the attitudes favoring condom use (see Condom Attitudes Scale, Methods and Materials section). Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis of the relation between occipital and fronto-temporal (middle temporal and inferior frontal gyri) cortices revealed weaker negative interactions between occipital and fronto-temporal cortices during viewing of the low MSV that high MSV ads. These findings confirm that the low MSV video health messages are better remembered than the high MSV messages and that this effect generalizes across public health domains. The greater engagement of the prefrontal and fronto-temporal cortices by low MSV ads and the greater occipital activation by high MSV ads suggest that that the “attention-grabbing” high MSV format could impede the learning and retention of public health messages.
Citation: Seelig D, Wang A-L, Jaganathan K, Loughead JW, Blady SJ, Childress AR, et al. (2014) Low Message Sensation Health Promotion Videos Are Better Remembered and Activate Areas of the Brain Associated with Memory Encoding. PLoS ONE 9(11): e113256. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0113256
Editor: Martin Walter, Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Germany
Received: May 14, 2014; Accepted: October 21, 2014; Published: November 19, 2014
Copyright: © 2014 Seelig et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The authors confirm that, for approved reasons, some access restrictions apply to the data underlying the findings. Ethical and legal issues render data unsuitable for public deposition. Requests for data may be made to the University of Pennsylvania Office of Research Services, Franklin Building Annex, P-221 FB/6205, 3451 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6205, phone 215-573-6707 fax 215-898-9708, http://www.upenn.edu/researchservices.
Funding: This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse R03 DA035683 and National Institutes of Allergy and Infectious Diseases Penn Center for AIDS Research (CFAR) P30 AI 045008. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript” to the appropriate location in the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Health-related video public service announcements (PSAs) and commercial advertisements promoting health-related products and healthy behaviors, are an important component of public health campaigns to reduce the incidence of sexually transmitted infections (STI) and addictions [1], [2], [3]. To facilitate objective evaluation of health-related video advertisements (further “ads”), researchers have developed several standardized measures of video ad characteristics. “Message sensation value” (MSV), which quantifies video ad features that are arousing and engage attention, is one of the best validated of such measures [4], [5], [6], [7]. Data support MSV’s construct validity and value in predicting behavioral outcomes of video ads [8], [9], [10]. While there is no objective algorithm for development of effective video ads, a long held consensus has been that high MSV is likely to increase video ad effectiveness by capturing more attention [4]. This consensus was also in-line with the “distraction hypothesis” stating that diversion of attention from an ad core message to its other features (i.e., distraction), may actually enhance message effectiveness by impeding a viewer’s ability to argue against an ad’s arguments [11]. An alternative point of view was that attention-grabbing audio-visual features that confer higher salience may actually compete with ad’s content for limited cognitive resources, thus reducing the processing of an ad’s message [6], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16]. In line with the latter overstimulation interpretation, a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) study of anti-smoking video ads [17] found that compared to high MSV ads, low MSV ads were associated with better recognition of still frames extracted from these ads after a short delay and were more likely to activate the middle temporal gyrus during viewing, one of the brain regions associated with memory formation (i.e., encoding) [18], [19]. In addition, low MSV ads were more likely to activate areas of the prefrontal cortex associated with top-down attention and cognitive processing [20], [21], [22]. In contrast, high MSV ads were associated with greater activation of the occipital cortex, a region associated with initial processing of visual information [23], [24].
The above findings suggest that high MSV ads may interfere with memory encoding, perhaps due to the increased allocation of the limited cognitive resources to visual processing. Consistent with this hypothesis, medial temporal and prefrontal response to low MSV anti-tobacco video ads was positively correlated with subsequent frame recognition accuracy [17].
Despite the evidence that high MSV may disrupt the processing of ad arguments, campaign designers often include high MSV inputs to capture attention and without considering possible negative effects of these distractors [25]. The present research was designed to provide a further test of the “limited cognitive resources” explanation for the poorer recognition accuracy of high MSV ads. In particular, we investigated the relationship between regions associated with sensory stimulation during ad viewing (primarily occipital cortex) and the prefrontal and temporal regions associated with cognitive processing and memory encoding. If the “limited cognitive resources” explanation is valid, we would expect the connectivity between these regions to be weaker under high MSV than low MSV. The presence of such a system is supported by anatomical evidence of a direct occipito-temporal network [26], as well as functional demonstrations of an association between these regions during encoding of sensory information [27], [28], [29].
A second goal of the research was to determine whether the superiority of low MSV ads previously demonstrated for anti-tobacco video ads [17] would extend to another important public health domain, namely safe sex. We anticipated that prior findings of superior encoding of low MSV ads would generalize to safe sex video ads promoting condom use. Generalization of the findings from anti-tobacco video ads to an entirely different topic would replicate the prior finding and suggest that it is relevant to a wider range of health topics. We hypothesized that low MSV ads will be recognized on subsequent tests of recognition memory better than high MSV ads. Furthermore we hypothesized that low MSV will preferentially activate temporal and prefrontal cortices, while high MSV ads would preferentially activate the occipital cortex. To explore our hypothesis that low MSV ads are associated with a greater degree of occipito-temporal connectivity than high MSV ads, we examined the psychophysiological interaction (PPI) between the occipital and the prefronto-temporal regions. Such a relationship would suggest that video ads with excessive visual stimulation prevent the efficient transfer of information from regions involved in visual processing to downstream regions associated with storage of this information in memory.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Thirty-nine healthy subjects were recruited through advertising and screened for psychiatric, neurological or medical illnesses. Exclusion criteria included presence of DSM-4-TR Axis 1 psychiatric disorder [30], chronic medical or neurological illness or treatment that could alter cerebral structure or metabolism, and safety-related contraindications for MRI scanning. All participants reported sexual experience. Data from 34 subjects (17 F), aged from 19 to 31 years (Mean 23.4, SD 2.9), with an average of 15.2 (SD 1.7) years of education, all right handed, were included in the final analysis. Among these participants, 11 (33%) self-identified as African American, 16 (47%) as European American and 7 (21%) as Asian American. All participants reported having been sexually active in the past three months with an average of 1.4 (SD 0.7) sexual partners. Six (2 M) reported having sexual partners of both genders and 10 (4 M) reported never using condoms. Each participant gave written informed consent to participate in the study. The study protocol and consent form were approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board.
Stimulus selection
We identified 71 ads that encouraged condom use by populations at-risk for unplanned pregnancy or sexually transmitted Diseases (STDs). Most of the ads were commercials produced by condom manufacturers (e.g., Trojan, Durex), who share a common goal with government-sponsored programs to encourage their target audiences to engage in safe-sex practices. Any references to particular manufacturer brand names were cut or digitally blurred. All 71 videos were scored for MSV by two raters who were trained on using the validated rating procedure [31], [32]. Briefly, MSV is an aggregate measure of audio and visual format features of video messages, derived from Zuckerman’s theory of sensation-seeking [33]. The rated features include cuts, special effects, intense graphic imagery, saturated colors, loud sounds, animation and so on. The specific MSV rating procedure used in this study has been described in Morgan et al [31] and validated in multiple previous studies [15], [34], including studies with neurophysiological correlates [17], [32], [35]. In the present study, the MSV of each ad was coded independently by two raters following the procedure described by Morgan et al. (2003). Rating are based on features described above, adjusted for ad length [31]. An ads’ total score is a composite of both qualitative (Present/Absent) and categorical (number of occurrences) ratings. The inter-rater correlation for the total score ratings in the present set of ads was very high (Pearson r = 0.96). The rare divergent ratings were reconciled by consensus between the two raters and a 3rd similarly trained individual.
We selected 32 complete ads (ranging between 26 to 66 [x¯ = 42.2] seconds in length) from the set of 71 drawn equally from above and below the collection’s median MSV score of 6 (mean 6.2, SD = 2.9). The length of the ads was not different between high and low MSV categories (high MSV: 40.81±12.06; low MSV: 40.75±13.64; t = 0.01, df = 30, p = 0.99). We created two sets of 16 ads, each consisting of 8 “high MSV” and 8 “low MSV” ads, with no difference in MSV between these two sets (t = 0.30, df = 30, p = 0.68). The mean MSV of the low subgroup was 3.6 (SD = 1.2, range = 1–5), and the mean MSV of the high subgroup was 8.8 (SD = 1.3, range = 7–10). MSV values were significantly different between low and high MSV ads (t = −11.3, df = 30, p<0.001). Four of the selected videos were produced by firms that sell condoms (e.g., Trojan, Durex), while the rest were developed by non-profit or government public health agencies (e.g., Centers for Disease Control) for network and cable broadcast (e.g. MTV).
Wang et al. [35] found an interaction between MSV and the persuasive strength of an ad’s argument (AS). As the goal of the current study was to compare the effects specifically associated with variation in MSV, we assessed whether AS was balanced between the two MSV conditions, using a previously employed questionnaire [32], [36] immediately prior to the end of their participation in the first session of the study. The mean AS scores of high MSV ads did not significantly differ from the mean AS of low MSV ads (t = −0.007, df = 30, p = 0.994).
Video task
Subjects were given the task (see Figure 1) of viewing one of the sets of 16 videos presented in a random sequence. Each ad was preceded and followed by an 18-second interstimulus interval (ISI) during which a homogenous black background with a grey fixation point (“+”) was displayed. These ISI’s were used as baseline periods for the purpose of imaging data analysis. Stimuli were not repeated and total task duration was 13 min 28 s.
The actual task displays 16 video messages (8 high MSV and 8 low MSV) in pseudorandom order (the order presented above is one possible organization).
Frame Recognition Test
Five minutes after the video task, subjects were given the Frame Recognition Test (FRT), a forced-choice recognition memory task designed to probe episodic memory of the videos they had seen in the scanner. This task was adapted from an electroencephalography study of the retention of TV commercials [37]. The FRT used in the current study consisted of 3 still-frame targets extracted from each of the 16 videos seen by the subject and 3 still-frame foils from each of 16 videos not seen by the subject (96 frames in total). To construct the frame recognition task (FRT) we used the approach of Wang (2013), Langleben (2009), and Silberstein (2001). Briefly, six frames, sampled at equal time intervals from each other to ensure that they were representative of the full length of the ad, were extracted from each ad. Then we randomly chose 3 frames from each video for the initial and follow up recognition tests. Frame stimuli were presented for 3 second each, in an optimized pseudo-random order [38] that included a variable ISI (0.25 to 16.25 s) during which a fixation point was present (see previous section above). For each trial, participants were instructed to respond “Yes” or “No” to the question “Have you seen this ad?”, using a two-button keypad (FORP, Current Design Inc., Philadelphia, PA). The question intentionally implied the ad as a whole though only a single frame was displayed. Stimuli were not repeated, and the total task run time was 9 min 22 s.
Delayed Frame Recognition Test
Three weeks following the fMRI session, participants returned for a second Frame Recognition Test to examine decay and to assess whether initial differences between high MSV and low MSV ads remained. The FRT presented during this session (“delayed FRT”) used the same design as the FRT that immediately followed the ad-viewing task. However, each participant was presented with a different set of 96 still-frames (also extracted from the same viewed and unviewed videos as the first FRT).
Condom Attitude Scale (CAS)
Subjects were questioned about their attitudes towards condoms immediately before and after viewing the video task using an abridged version of the Condom Attitude Scale (CAS) [39]. The abridged CAS contains 17 items that query three factors: condom efficacy in preventing sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) and perceived risks of STD, interpersonal impact of condom use, and the effect of condoms on sexual experience. Participants were instructed to indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with each statement on a 1–6 scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 6 = Strongly Agree). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA assessed the main effect, on CAS, of the within-subjects variable (before vs. after task) and the between-subject variable (male vs. female), as well as their possible interaction.
Procedure
All visual tasks were programmed in the Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Albany, CA) stimulus presentation package and rear-projected to the center of the visual field using a PowerLite 7300 Video projector system (Epson America, Inc., Long Beach, CA) that was viewed through a mirror mounted on the scanner head coil. The video soundtrack was delivered through Silent Scan 2100 MRI-compatible headphones (Avotec Inc., Stuart, FL). Before the imaging session, participants were instructed to watch all the videos carefully, that their memory of the videos would be tested after a delay and that their performance on this test was an integral part of the experiment. Participants performed the FRT approximately five minutes after the video task. The FRT was repeated 3 weeks later at a second session.
Behavioral data analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using the IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (IBM SPSS version 21). Subjects’ performance on the FRT was evaluated using the Discrimination Index, Pr = ZCorrect target recognition–ZFalse alarms, which reflects how well subjects correctly distinguish targets from foils [40]. All subjects performed above chance level (Pr≥0), so none were excluded from analyses. Subject tendency to respond “Yes” or “No” under uncertainty was evaluated using the Response Bias measure, Br = ZCorrect target recognition+ZFalse alarms. Br = 0 indicates no bias, Br<0 indicates liberal bias (i.e. tendency of saying “Yes” when uncertain), Br>0 indicates conservative bias (i.e. tendency of saying “No” when uncertain). Paired t-tests were performed comparing subject performance on Session 1 vs. Session 2 FRT (immediate vs. delayed sessions), and high MSV vs. low MSV. Independent t-tests were performed comparing gender performance.
fMRI data acquisition
MRI data were collected on the University of Pennsylvania’s Siemens Trio (Erlangen, Germany) 3 Tesla whole body system using a 32-channel head coil. BOLD fMRI gradient-echo echo-planar sequence was acquired with the following parameters: TR = 3000 ms, TE = 32 ms, flip angle = 90, matrix = 64×64, FOV = 220 mm × 220 mm, slice thickness/gap = 3.4/0 mm, 46 slices. After BOLD fMRI, a 5-min magnetization prepared, rapid acquisition gradient echo T1-weighted image (MPRAGE, TR = 1620 ms, TE = 3.87 ms, FOV 250 mm, Matrix 192×256, effective voxel resolution of 1×1×1 mm) was acquired covering the whole brain for spatial normalization and anatomical overlay of functional data [41].
fMRI data preprocessing
fMRI data were preprocessed and analyzed using FEAT(fMRI Expert Analysis Tool), part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Images were slice time-corrected, motion-corrected to the median image using tri-linear interpolation with six degrees of freedom [42], high-pass filtered (100 s), spatially smoothed (5 mm FWHM, isotropic) and scaled using mean-based intensity normalization. The median functional and anatomical volumes were co-registered and then transformed into standard space (T1 MNI template) using tri-linear interpolation [42], [43]. BET (Brain Extraction Tool) [44] was used to remove non-brain areas. Data sets with motion or signal-to-noise ratio exceeding 2 standard deviations from the average were excluded (5 of 39 initial subjects). Coordinates were converted to Talairach space [45] for tables and figures.
Subject-level Analysis
Subject-level time series statistical analysis was carried out using FILM (FMRIB’s Improved Linear Model) with local autocorrelation correction [46]. High MSV and low MSV video messages were modeled using a canonical hemodynamic response function for all subjects based on the order of stimulus presentation for each subject. Six rigid body movement parameters from the motion correction were modeled as nuisance variables. Second level analysis was performed on the contrast images generated from the subject level analysis including: high MSV>low MSV; low MSV>high MSV; All videos>baseline. Age, gender, educational level and condom use status were entered as covariates of no interest. Group statistical maps were thresholded at z = 2.3. To control for Type 1 error, group maps were cluster corrected for multiple comparisons at p<0.05, using the Family-wise Error Rate based on Gaussian Random Fields (GRF) theory. Brain response to high and low MSV ads was compared between African American and Caucasian participants. Lastly, we analyzed the correlation between brain activation during ad viewing and the change in CAS after the fMRI session, calculated as ΔCAS = CAS_post – CAS_pre, which was entered as a covariate of interest for the All videos>baseline contrast, resulting in positive and negative correlation maps (z>2.3 p<0.05).
Psychophysiological Interaction (PPI) Analysis
The PPI analysis was designed to test the hypothesis that greater visual processing would interfere with memory encoding of the ads. Thus, brain regions most associated with visual stimulation (specified by the high MSV>low MSV contrast) would have a weaker or negative functional relationship with the brain regions required for successful memory encoding of the videos, specified by the low MSV>high MSV contrast in the High MSV condition than the low MSV condition. In the first step of this analysis, based on methods detailed by Gitelman et al. [47], we computed individual average time-series to modeled interactions (for the two MSV conditions separately) between each voxel in the brain and a seed region specified by a functional bilateral mask. This mask was created by using the voxels surviving cluster correction (z>2.3 p<0.05) in the high MSV>low MSV contrast. As seen by the blue-tinted voxels in the left half of Fig. 2, this seed mask was also nearly identical to the anatomically-defined occipital cortex. The seed time-courses for each subject were deconvolved based on the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) used by Gitelman et al [47] to construct a time series of neural activity in this region for each MSV condition. We then estimated a GLM with three regressors specifically during video-viewing segments: (1) A binary indicator function specifying low MSV vs. high MSV trials, (2) Average BOLD time-series for the seed (OCC mask) described above, and (3) An interaction between neural activity in the OCC seed and the indicator function described in #1. The second and third regressors were convolved with the HRF.
Middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and inferior frontal gyri (IFG) (red) have increased response for Low MSV>High MSV items. Occipital cortex (OCC) (blue) has increased response for High MSV>Low MSV ads. Statistical maps are displayed over the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain template and thresholded at Z = 2.3, cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons at p<0.05. Coordinates converted to Talairach space [45].
As our hypothesis was restricted to interactions between regional networks functionally defined by our task (i.e., brain areas associated with sensory stimulation vs. those associated with strong memory encoding), we utilized a second functional bilateral mask as a target region of interest (ROI). This seed mask was created by using the voxels surviving cluster correction (z>2.3 p<0.05) in the low MSV>high MSV contrast. As seen by the red-tinted voxels in Fig. 2, this mask primarily covers the medial temporal gyrus (MTG) with a smaller proportion of voxels covering the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Our subsequent analysis examined the magnitude and valence (positive or negative) of each voxel in the ROI target based on its interaction with the seed during our task (i.e., we did not investigate any seed-voxel interactions outside of the target ROI mask).
To compare the strength and direction of averaged interactions involving the OCC (seed) and the MTG/IFG (ROI target) between low MSV vs. high MSV conditions, we performed a second-level t-test analysis using a procedure modified from Hartley et al [48]. As each subject (N = 34) possessed a pair of average OCC – MTG/IFG interaction values for the low MSV and high MSV conditions respectively, we calculated a two-tailed paired t-test to look for significant interaction differences between the two conditions.
To further characterize differences between the two PPI distributions, we divided both conditions’ voxel distributions into two sets of 5 quintiles and used paired t-tests to compare the means within each pair of corresponding quintiles. This analysis involved the following procedure: Using the OCC mask as the seed region, a PPI z-score was determined for each voxel within the MTG/IFG (target region of interest, ROI) for each subject. Within the target ROI, the voxel/seed PPI distributions were broken into two sets of quintiles, one set for each condition.
The goal of this analysis was to determine whether differences in interaction between low MSV versus high MSV were driven by (1) a relative increase in positively interacting voxels during low MSV, (2) a relative increase in negatively interacting voxels during high MSV, or (3) a combination of these mechanisms. We also sought to determine if low MSV’s more positive PPI mean could be associated with a weaker overall associative relationship between the seed and target ROI relative to high MSV, a result which would disprove our hypothesis that low MSV videos improve the cooperation between regions involved in input of sensory information and downstream encoding of this information.
Results
Behavioral data
A two-way repeated ANOVA revealed that memory performance was significantly better during the immediate post-task FRT session than the 3-week delayed session (F = 101.13, df = 1, p<0.001), and low MSV ads were bettered remembered than high MSV ads (F = 60.68, df = 1, p<0.001), but no interaction between Session and MSV (F = 2.20, df = 1, p = 0.15). Subjects responded significantly less conservatively during low MSV than high MSV ads (Br = −0.099 vs. 0.24, t = −7.045, df = 33, p<0.001). There were no significant bias differences between sessions (Br = 0.15 vs. 0.69, t = 1.36, df = 33, p = 0.183). An independent t-test on gender revealed no significant differences between males vs. females in discrimination or response bias (Pr = 2.05 vs 2.20, t = −0.775, df = 32, p = 0.444; Br = 0.033 vs. 0.18, t = −1.67, df = 32, p = 0.104). Examining the pre- and post-task CAS measure, there was no main effect of PSA exposure (F = 0.546, df = 1, p = 0.465) or interaction of task and gender (F = 0.076, df = 1, p = 0.784). There was a strong main effect of gender, with females exhibiting more positive condom attitudes than men (F = 10.3, df = 1, p<0.005).
fMRI data
Data were thresholded at Z = 2.3 and cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons at p<0.05. Compared to the high MSV videos, low MSV videos were associated with greater response in the prefrontal cortex, including the inferior and middle frontal gyri, and the temporal cortex, including the bilateral middle temporal gyrus (Table 1a, Figure 2 [red-yellow activation]). The high MSV videos were associated with greater activity in occipital cortex, including the cuneus and the fusiform and lingual gyri, when contrasted with low MSV videos (Table 1b, Figure 2 [blue-green activation]). In addition, changes in the Condom Attitude Scale (ΔCAS) in favor of condom use were negatively correlated with brain activation in the bilateral Fusiform and Superior Temporal Gyri (Table 2). There were no significant differences in between African American (N = 11) and Caucasian (N = 16) participants response to either high or low MSV ads.
Primary PPI analyses
Averaging the z-scores (interaction with OCC seed) across all MTG/IFG voxels revealed that both conditions shared a negative overall PPI, with low MSV less negative than high MSV (−0.46 vs. −0.95). This finding was marginally significant (p = 0.060, two-tailed, n = 34).
The analysis by quintiles revealed an asymmetrical voxel distribution, as reflected by the increasing asymmetry between low MSV and high MSV from Quintile 1 to Quintile 5. That is, while the MTG/IFG voxels with the most negative z-score interactions with OCC during the low MSV condition did not differ from that during high MSV (Quintile 1: −2.83 vs. −2.86, p = 0.94), the most positive quintile of voxels was significantly greater during low MSV than high MSV (Quintile 5∶1.96 vs. 1.12, p = 0.0089). As seen in Table 3, proceeding from Quintile 1 to Quintile 5, a gradual divergence between low MSV and high MSV ads was observed, with low MSV interactions increasingly becoming more positive than their high MSV counterparts.
Discussion
This study extends findings from anti-smoking PSAs [17] to videos promoting safe sex through condom use. First, we confirmed our hypothesis that videos low in MSV would be better recognized both immediately and 3 weeks after the viewing session. This finding confirms the observation that high MSV video ads are not as well remembered as low MSV video ads [17] and suggests that the empirical strategy of capturing attention, i.e., “buying the eyeballs” [49], [50] may come at the expense of learning the content and therefore the message of the ad. In line with our hypotheses regarding the brain systems-level mechanism of this phenomenon, we found that video ads low in MSV activated the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), a brain region engaged in episodic memory encoding [18], [51] as well as the dorsal prefrontal cortex, a region associated with working memory and attention [19], [20], [21], [22] and considered to be a predictor of long-term message efficacy in several studies [35], [52], [53], [54]. By contrast, video ads high in MSV activated the occipital cortex (OCC), a region involved in primary processing of visual information [23], [24].
The symmetrically bilateral occipitoparietal activation associated with high MSV ads, extends findings first reported in Langleben et al. [17] and confirmed by Wang et al. [35] in studies of anti-tobacco ads. The occipital activation evoked by high MSV videos could reallocate processing resources away from encoding of ad messages [55], reflected in the lower MTG activation and reduced recognition memory for the high MSV compared to the low MSV ads. In addition, the negative correlation between increased CAS in favor of condom use and activation in the bilateral Fusiform and Superior Temporal Gyri also suggests that visual input could interfere with the cognitive processing of video ads’ message.
Finally, we used PPI to explore the hypothesis that relative to high MSV videos, low MSV videos allow for more efficient transfer of visual information inputs processed in the OCC and subsequently encoded in the MTG (also facilitated by co-activating attentional networks based in the IFG). By contrast, we hypothesized that high MSV videos should be associated with reduced interaction between these regions by reallocating brain processing resources away from the regions involved in processing and remembering ad messages [17], [56]. In support of the overstimulation hypothesis, we found that despite an overall negative level of interaction between these regions, the distribution of interactions was progressively more positive while viewing low MSV ads.
For the most part, the distribution of PPI z-scores was negative between the regions of interest during viewing of either high MSV or low MSV videos. Studies disagree to what extent the negative interactions between the occipital and rostral cortices are part of normal brain activity. There is some evidence that negative interactions may imply a dysfunctional inhibitory process. For example, relative to healthy control subjects, hyperactivation of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in patients with schizophrenia has been found to diminish top-down cognitive control normally mediated by the anterior cingulate cortex [57]. However, other fMRI studies consider negative interactions to be evidence of a more functional or efficient system. For example, a recent study, which examined the connectivity between the lateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, was characterized by primarily negative interactions, which the authors interpreted to be evidence of a complex dynamic system involving reciprocal interactions [58]. These findings are consistent with a hypothesis [59] that reduced negative interaction between cortical and limbic systems is a marker of major depression.
Despite the controversy about whether to interpret negative interactions as evidence of increased or decreased functional connectivity, our PPI analysis shows the negative interaction between the OCC and MTG/IFG regions to be weaker during the low MSV than high MSV at a marginally significant (p<0.06) level. This was especially prominent at the upper end of the PPI distribution (Table 3), suggesting that the former’s smaller mean (closer-to-zero) is a result of a shift towards more positive interactions during low MSV ads (rather than more negative interactions during high MSV). These findings provide support for the hypothesis that high MSV inhibits memory encoding as a result of increased activation of the primary sensory cortices, while low MSV videos may facilitate more efficient transfer of sensory information processed by OCC to downstream brain regions involved in encoding and attention (MTG, IFG). These results are consistent with theories modeling persuasion as a step-wise process that begins with attracting audience’s attention, followed by comprehension [25], where an excess of attention-grabbing devices could interfere with comprehension. They are also in-line with theories of dual-processing of information, such as the Elaboration Likelihood Model [6] and Limited Capacity theories, which postulate that sensory input could compete for the limited cognitive resources available to process various types of information in the message and interfere with subsequent processing that leads to successful encoding [60]. This conclusion also weighs on an earlier debate between the proponents and critics of the “distraction hypothesis” suggesting that distracting elements in an ad improve persuasiveness by reducing the resistance to persuasive messages [11], [61]. Our results complement the largely inconclusive behavioral studies of comparative effectiveness of the high and low MSV safe-sex video messages [3], [7], [62].
Although our results suggest that low MSV ads are more efficacious than the high MSV ads, they come with a number of caveats and limitations. Similarly to prior neuroimaging studies [37] [17], we used immediate and delayed recognition memory as a proxy outcome variable for the video ads’ behavioral impact. As the ultimate goal of neuroimaging research in the context of communications is to elucidate the mechanisms by which video ads effect sustained behavioral change and apply this knowledge towards improving video ad effectiveness, future research is necessary to confirm the link between remembering an ad and actually changing behavior in accordance with an ad’s message. The present study did not include in-depth standardized evaluation of sexual behaviors and participants’ emotional reaction of the video ads, both of which should be taken into account in future neuroimaging studies of safe-sex ad processing. While both ad and audience characteristics are important for the overall ad effect, the present study focused on ad characteristics, i.e. the MSV and accounted for the socio-demographic variables [63], [64], [65], [66], [67], [68] in the analysis. Future studies should investigate the influence of audience variables on ad effectiveness. Third, to preserve ecological validity, we used authentic previously aired ads of varying lengths, in their entirety. Though the high and low MSV categories did not differ on ad length, there remains a possibility that ad length variability could have confounded the Frame Recognition Test performance. Future research could consider alternative approaches, such as producing ads specifically for the purpose of research study or editing existing ads to standard size. Finally, since CAS is only a proxy outcome variable, longitudinal assessment of actual condom use following ad exposure would be required to determine the translational significance of our findings.
In conclusion, we confirmed prior reports [17] that high MSV video ads are more likely to interfere with and be less effective at conveying their messages than low MSV ads. Our neuroimaging findings suggest that the potential mechanism for this advantage may be that low MSV ads enable more efficient cross-brain communication between the brain regions involved in early processing of visual information and regions involved in downstream processing and encoding of this information into long-term memory. The fact that these behavioral and neuroimaging findings generalize across message genres justifies further efforts to incorporate neuroimaging in the evaluation and design of public health communications.
Acknowledgments
Authors thank Dr. Rick Zimmerman for assistance in acquiring safe sex video ads and Dr. Lewis Donohew for comments on an earlier version of the paper.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: JWL DR DDL. Performed the experiments: SJB DR DDL. Analyzed the data: DS ALW KJ ARC DDL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: DS ALW KJ ARC DR DDL. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: DS ALW KJ DR DDL.
References
- 1. Emery SL, Szczypka G, Powell LM, Chaloupka FJ (2007) Public health obesity-related TV advertising: lessons learned from tobacco. Am J Prev Med 33: S257–263.
- 2. Biener L, McCallum-Keeler G, Nyman AL (2000) Adults’ response to Massachusetts anti-tobacco television advertisements: impact of viewer and advertisement characteristics. Tob Control 9: 401–407.
- 3. Zimmerman RS, Palmgreen PM, Noar SM, Lustria ML, Lu HY, et al. (2007) Effects of a televised two-city safer sex mass media campaign targeting high-sensation-seeking and impulsive-decision-making young adults. Health Educ Behav 34: 810–826.
- 4. Harrington N, Lane DR, Donohew L, Zimmerman RS, Norling GR, et al. (2003) Persuasive strategies for effective anti-drug messages. Communication Monographs 70: 16–38.
- 5. Palmgreen P, Donohew L, Lorch E, Rogus M, Helm D, et al. (1991) Sensation seeking, message sensation value, and drug use as mediators of PSA effectiveness. Health Communication 3: 217–227.
- 6.
Petty R, Cacioppo J (1986) Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change New York: Springer Verlag. 262 p.
- 7. Stephenson M, Southwell B (2006) Sensation Seeking, the activation model, and mass media health campaigns: Current findings and future directions for cancer communication. Journal of Communication 56: S38–S56.
- 8. D’Silva MU, Palmgreen P (2007) Individual differences and context: factors mediating recall of anti-drug public service announcements. Health Commun 21: 65–71.
- 9. Helme DW, Donohew RL, Baier M, Zittleman L (2007) A classroom-administered simulation of a television campaign on adolescent smoking: testing an activation model of information exposure. J Health Commun 12: 399–415.
- 10. Van Stee SK, Noar SM, Allard S, Zimmerman R, Palmgreen P, et al. (2012) Reactions to safer-sex public service announcement message features: attention, perceptions of realism, and cognitive responses. Qualitative health research 22: 1568–1579.
- 11. Festinger L, Maccoby N (1964) On Resistance to Persuasive Communications. Journal of abnormal psychology 68: 359–366.
- 12. Lang A (2006) Using the Limited Capacity Model of Motivated Mediated Message Processing to Design Effective Cancer Communication Messages. Journal of Communication 56: S57–S80.
- 13. Bolls PD, Muehling DD, Yoon K (2003) The effects of television commercial pacing on viewers’ attention and memory. Journal of Marketing Communications 9: 17–28.
- 14. Indovina I, Macaluso E (2007) Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of visuospatial attention. Cereb Cortex 17: 1701–1711.
- 15. Kang Y, Cappella JN, Fishbein M (2006) The attentional mechanism of message sensation value: Interaction between message sensation value and argument quality on message effectiveness. Communication Monographs 73: 351–378.
- 16. Romer D (1979) Distraction, couterarguing and the internalization of attitude change. European Journal of Social Psychology 9: 1–17.
- 17. Langleben DD, Loughead JW, Ruparel K, Hakun JG, Busch-Winokur S, et al. (2009) Reduced prefrontal and temporal processing and recall of high “sensation value” ads. NeuroImage 46: 219–225.
- 18. Hannula DE, Ranganath C (2008) Medial temporal lobe activity predicts successful relational memory binding. J Neurosci 28: 116–124.
- 19. Buschman TJ, Miller EK (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315: 1860–1862.
- 20. D’Esposito M (2007) From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 362: 761–772.
- 21. Knudsen EI (2007) Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 30: 57–78.
- 22. Murray LJ, Ranganath C (2007) The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contributes to successful relational memory encoding. J Neurosci 27: 5515–5522.
- 23. Johnson JA, Zatorre RJ (2006) Neural substrates for dividing and focusing attention between simultaneous auditory and visual events. Neuroimage 31: 1673–1681.
- 24. Indovina I, Macaluso E (2004) Occipital-parietal interactions during shifts of exogenous visuospatial attention: trial-dependent changes of effective connectivity. Magn Reson Imaging 22: 1477–1486.
- 25.
McGuire WJ (1989) Theoretical foundations of campaigns. In: Rice RE, CK A, editors. Public communication campaigns. 2 ed. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. 43–65.
- 26. Guhn A, Dresler T, Hahn T, Muhlberger A, Strohle A, et al. (2012) Medial prefrontal cortex activity during the extinction of conditioned fear: an investigation using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuropsychobiology 65: 173–182.
- 27. Plaud JJ (2003) Pavlov and the foundation of behavior therapy. Span J Psychol 6: 147–154.
- 28. Wolpe J (1963) Quantitative relationships in the systematic desensitization of phobias. The American journal of psychiatry 119: 1062–1068.
- 29. Milad MR, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ (2006) Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biol Psychol 73: 61–71.
- 30.
First M, editor (2002) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
- 31. Morgan SE (2003) Associations between message features and subjective evaluations of the sensation value of antidrug public service announcements. Journal of Communication 53: 512–526.
- 32. Strasser AA, Cappella JN, Jepson C, Fishbein M, Tang KZ, et al. (2009) Experimental evaluation of antitobacco PSAs: effects of message content and format on physiological and behavioral outcomes. Nicotine Tob Res 11: 293–302.
- 33.
Zuckerman M (1979) Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 449 p.
- 34. Everett M, Palmgreen P (1995) Influences of sensation seeking, message sensation value, and program context on effectiveness of anticocaine public service announcements. Health Communication 7: 225–248.
- 35. Wang AL, Ruparel K, Loughead JW, Strasser AA, Blady SJ, et al. (2013) Content matters: neuroimaging investigation of brain and behavioral impact of televised anti-tobacco public service announcements. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33: 7420–7427.
- 36. Fishbein M, Hall-Jamieson K, Zimmer E, von Haeften I, Nabi R (2002) Avoiding the boomerang: Testing the relative effectiveness of antidrug public service announcements before a national campaign. American Journal of Public Health 92: 238–245.
- 37.
Rossiter JR, Silberstein RB (2001) Brain-imaging detection of visual scene encoding in long-term memory for TV commercials. Journal of Advertising Research 41: Mar-Apr 2001.
- 38. Dale AM (1999) Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 8: 109–114.
- 39. Sacco WP, Levine B, Reed DL, Thompson K (1991) Attitudes about condom use as an AIDS-relevant behavior: Their factor structure and relation to condom use. Psychological Assessment 3: 265–272.
- 40. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J (1988) Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of experimental psychology General 117: 34–50.
- 41. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, et al. (2000) Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp 10: 120–131.
- 42. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17: 825–841.
- 43. Jenkinson M, Smith S (2001) A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal 5: 143–156.
- 44. Smith SM (2002) Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 17: 143–155.
- 45.
Talairach J, Tournoux P. (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers.
- 46. Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady M, Smith SM (2001) Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. NeuroImage 14: 1370–1386.
- 47. Erlich JC, Bush DE, Ledoux JE (2012) The role of the lateral amygdala in the retrieval and maintenance of fear-memories formed by repeated probabilistic reinforcement. Front Behav Neurosci 6: 16.
- 48. Hartley CA, Fischl B, Phelps EA (2011) Brain structure correlates of individual differences in the acquisition and inhibition of conditioned fear. Cereb Cortex 21: 1954–1962.
- 49. Chattopadhyay A, Laborie J-L (2005) Managing Brand Experience: The Market Contact Audit (TM). Journal of Advertising Research 45: 9–16.
- 50. Russell CA, Belch M (2005) A Managerial Investigation into the Product Placement Industry. Journal of Advertising Research 45: 73–92.
- 51. Thrasher JF, Rousu MC, Anaya-Ocampo R, Reynales-Shigematsu LM, Arillo-Santillan E, et al. (2007) Estimating the impact of different cigarette package warning label policies: the auction method. Addictive behaviors 32: 2916–2925.
- 52. Chua HF, Ho SS, Jasinska AJ, Polk TA, Welsh RC, et al. (2011) Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting. Nature neuroscience 14: 426–427.
- 53. Chua HF, Liberzon I, Welsh RC, Strecher VJ (2009) Neural correlates of message tailoring and self-relatedness in smoking cessation programming. Biol Psychiatry 65: 165–168.
- 54. Miettunen J, Lauronen E, Kantojarvi L, Veijola J, Joukamaa M (2008) Inter-correlations between Cloninger’s temperament dimensions– a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 160: 106–114.
- 55. Akyurek EG, Vallines I, Lin EJ, Schubo A (2010) Distraction and target selection in the brain: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 48: 3335–3342.
- 56. Lang A, Bradley SD, Park B, Shin M, Chung Y (2006) Parsing the Resource Pie: Using STRTs to measure attention to mediated messages. Media Psychology 8: 369–394.
- 57. Schlosser RG, Koch K, Wagner G, Nenadic I, Roebel M, et al. (2008) Inefficient executive cognitive control in schizophrenia is preceded by altered functional activation during information encoding: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 46: 336–347.
- 58. Longe O, Senior C, Rippon G (2009) The lateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex work as a dynamic integrated system: evidence from FMRI connectivity analysis. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 21: 141–154.
- 59. Mayberg HS (1997) Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences 9: 471–481.
- 60. Lang A (2000) The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication 50: 46–70.
- 61. McGuire WJ (1966) Attitudes and opinions. Annual review of psychology 17: 475–514.
- 62. Noar SM, Palmgreen P, Chabot M, Dobransky N, Zimmerman RS (2009) A 10-Year Systematic Review of HIV/AIDS Mass Communication Campaigns: Have We Made Progress? J Health Commun 14: 15–42.
- 63. Reitman D, St Lawrence JS, Jefferson KW, Alleyne E, Brasfield TL, et al. (1996) Predictors of African American adolescents’ condom use and HIV risk behavior. AIDS Educ Prev 8: 499–515.
- 64. St Lawrence JS, Eldridge GD, Reitman D, Little CE, Shelby MC, et al. (1998) Factors influencing condom use among African American women: implications for risk reduction interventions. American journal of community psychology 26: 7–28.
- 65.
Johnson WD, Diaz RM, Flanders WD, Goodman M, Hill AN, et al. (2008) Behavioral interventions to reduce risk for sexual transmission of HIV among men who have sex with men. Cochrane database of systematic reviews: CD001230.
- 66. Protogerou C, Johnson BT (2014) Factors Underlying the Success of Behavioral HIV-Prevention Interventions for Adolescents: A Meta-Review. AIDS Behav 18: 1847–1863.
- 67. Thurston IB, Dietrich J, Bogart LM, Otwombe KN, Sikkema KJ, et al. (2014) Correlates of sexual risk among sexual minority and heterosexual South African youths. American journal of public health 104: 1265–1269.
- 68. Widman L, Noar SM, Choukas-Bradley S, Francis DB (2014) Adolescent sexual health communication and condom use: A meta-analysis. Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association 33: 1113–1124.