Figures
Abstract
Patients with frontotemporal dementia have pervasive changes in emotion recognition and social cognition, yet the neural changes underlying these emotion processing deficits remain unclear. The multimodal system model of emotion proposes that basic emotions are dependent on distinct brain regions, which undergo significant pathological changes in frontotemporal dementia. As such, this syndrome may provide important insight into the impact of neural network degeneration upon the innate ability to recognise emotions. This study used voxel-based morphometry to identify discrete neural correlates involved in the recognition of basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, sadness, surprise and happiness) in frontotemporal dementia. Forty frontotemporal dementia patients (18 behavioural-variant, 11 semantic dementia, 11 progressive nonfluent aphasia) and 27 healthy controls were tested on two facial emotion recognition tasks: The Ekman 60 and Ekman Caricatures. Although each frontotemporal dementia group showed impaired recognition of negative emotions, distinct associations between emotion-specific task performance and changes in grey matter intensity emerged. Fear recognition was associated with the right amygdala; disgust recognition with the left insula; anger recognition with the left middle and superior temporal gyrus; and sadness recognition with the left subcallosal cingulate, indicating that discrete neural substrates are necessary for emotion recognition in frontotemporal dementia. The erosion of emotion-specific neural networks in neurodegenerative disorders may produce distinct profiles of performance that are relevant to understanding the neurobiological basis of emotion processing.
Citation: Kumfor F, Irish M, Hodges JR, Piguet O (2013) Discrete Neural Correlates for the Recognition of Negative Emotions: Insights from Frontotemporal Dementia. PLoS ONE 8(6): e67457. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067457
Editor: James Kilner, University College London, United Kingdom
Received: January 30, 2013; Accepted: May 17, 2013; Published: June 21, 2013
Copyright: © 2013 Kumfor et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: This work was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), of Australia project grant [510106]; the Australian Research Council (ARC) Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders [CE110001021]; an Australian Postgraduate Award (to FK); an ARC Federation Fellowship [FF0776229 to JRH]; and an NHMRC of Australia Career Development Fellowship [APP1022684 to OP]. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a progressive neurodegenerative brain disorder associated with marked changes in social and emotion processing [1]–[9]. FTD encompasses three clinical subtypes that present with either changes in behaviour (behavioural-variant FTD (bvFTD)), or changes in language (semantic dementia (SD) and progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA)) [10], [11]. The clinical features observed in FTD subtypes reflect their underlying focal brain atrophy in the initial stages of the disease, namely the orbitomesial frontal regions in bvFTD; the anterior temporal lobes in SD (greater on one side, more commonly the left hemisphere); and the left insula, superior temporal gyrus and inferior frontal regions surrounding the Sylvian fissure in PNFA [12]–[16].
Despite their differing clinical presentations and associated brain atrophy, all three phenotypes experience a degree of emotion processing impairment, which is present on tasks using facial, auditory and film stimuli [2], [5]–[7], [9]. Typically, recognition of negative emotions is affected, whereas recognition of positive emotions remains relatively intact [1], [17]. General emotion recognition deficits in FTD have been associated with atrophy of the amygdala [5], [9], orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex [5], [9], [18], [19], complementing previous reports in healthy adults [20]–[23]. Knowledge of neural correlates necessary for recognition of specific emotions in FTD subtypes remains, however, limited.
Some emotions, known as “basic emotions” (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise), are proposed to be innate [24], [25]. They have evolved over time across species and are thought to rely on, at least partly, dissociable brain regions [22], [25]–[28]. Accordingly, a neurobiological model of emotion processing has been developed, referred to as the multimodal system model of emotion [2], [29]. Converging research from animal and human (lesion and imaging) studies indicates that processing of some basic emotions maps onto discrete brain regions, irrespective of the type of stimulus presented [28]–[30].
Evidence for a discrete multimodal system model of emotion is strongest for two basic emotions: fear and disgust. The amygdala is consistently implicated during fear conditioning in animals [31] and shows increased activation in humans when viewing fearful faces [32], [33]. Further, selective deficits in fear recognition have been identified in patients with bilateral amygdala damage, supporting the view that this structure is crucial for processing of fear [30], [34]–[36]. In contrast, disgust recognition is associated with insula and basal ganglia integrity in humans [29], [37]–[40]. The central role of these structures in processing signals of disgust is demonstrated by the disproportionate impairment of disgust recognition observed in patients with Huntington’s disease, or following focal damage to the insula and putamen [29], [41]. Evidence supporting the existence of specialised brain regions for processing the other basic emotions is less well established. Medial and orbitofrontal regions appear to be involved in processing of anger [42], [43], although selective impairment of anger recognition following lesions involving these brain regions has not been reported to date. The involvement of discrete neural substrates for processing of sadness, surprise and happiness also remain poorly understood [28]. Importantly, these emotion-specific regions, namely the amygdala, insula and frontal cortex, are brain regions showing significant neuronal loss and abnormal protein deposition in FTD [15]. As such, in addition to providing insight into how specific lesions can cause impairment, studies of patients with neurodegenerative conditions can inform how specific brain regions form networks that are central to supporting different cognitive functions [44].
This study aimed to test the multimodal system model of emotion and investigate the contribution of dissociable neural substrates to the recognition of basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, sadness, happiness and surprise) in FTD, using voxel-based morphometry; an unbiased, automated, whole-brain structural analysis method. In order to identify emotion-specific neural correlates in FTD, this study used both a traditional facial emotion recognition task (Ekman 60) and a modified facial emotion recognition task (Ekman Caricatures). On the Ekman Caricatures task the intensity of the emotion expressed is enhanced, thereby reducing the attentional and perceptual demands of the task [45], [46]. Using the Ekman Caricatures task, a recent study showed that emotion recognition deficits observed in bvFTD and PNFA could be alleviated to some extent by increasing the intensity of the emotional expression [7]. In contrast, increasing the emotional intensity of facial expressions did not improve performance in SD, suggesting that, in these patients, reduced emotion recognition is due to a primary emotion processing impairment. In bvFTD and PNFA, however, poor emotion recognition performance may be in part mediated by attentional or perceptual demands of the task [7].
By using two tasks of facial emotion recognition, which vary in attentional and perceptual task demands, we aimed to identify common neural correlates associated with basic emotion identification, irrespective of task demands. Based on the multimodal system model we hypothesised that fear recognition would be dependent on amygdala integrity, and disgust recognition would be dependent on insula integrity, on both emotion recognition tasks. No specific hypotheses for neural substrates associated with processing the basic emotions anger, sadness, surprise or happiness were made, as evidence for specific neural substrates for processing of these emotions is mixed. In doing so, we aimed to determine the relative importance of specific neural structures for the recognition of basic human emotions in FTD, and establish the crucial role specific neural structures play in the recognition of basic emotions.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Forty FTD patients (18 bvFTD, 11 SD and 11 PNFA) were compared with 27 healthy controls. Patients were recruited from FRONTIER, the frontotemporal dementia clinical research group based at Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney. All patients underwent clinical assessment, cognitive neuropsychological assessment and structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). An experienced behavioural neurologist assessed all patients and diagnosis was established by consensus between the neurologist, neuropsychologist and occupational therapist, based on extensive clinical investigations, cognitive assessment, and evidence of atrophy on structural MRI brain scans. All patients met current clinical diagnostic criteria [10], [11]. In brief, the bvFTD group presented with changes in behaviour and personality, displaying emotional blunting, loss of insight, and reduced motivation. The SD group presented with reduced semantic knowledge, demonstrated by impaired naming, and comprehension, in the context of relatively preserved phonology and syntax. The PNFA group presented with impaired expressive language characterised by effortful speech, phonetic distortions and impaired grammatical structure, in the context of relatively intact comprehension.
Healthy control participants were recruited from the local area and included family members of the patients and individuals recruited from local community clubs. All control participants underwent neuropsychological assessment, and structural MRI. For all participants, exclusion criteria included: concurrent psychiatric disturbance, other types of dementia or other neurological disease, including cerebrovascular disease, history of substance abuse and/or use of medications with central nervous system side effects and for controls, an Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination – Revised (ACE-R) score below 88/100 [47].
The behavioural performance for 29 of the 40 FTD patients (10/18 bvFTD, 10/11 SD, 9/11 PNFA) has been previously reported in a study investigating the effects of increasing emotional intensity on emotion recognition in FTD [7]. The current study, however, sought to establish the neural correlates of recognition of specific basic emotions in FTD.
Ethics Statement
This research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethics approval was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committee of South Eastern Sydney/Illawarra Area Health Service (HREC 10/126) and the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Advisory panel D (Biomedical, ref. #10035). Participant or family written consent was obtained from each participant. Participants volunteered their time and were reimbursed for travel costs.
Behavioural testing
General Cognitive Tests.
Participants were assessed on the ACE-R [47] as a measure of general cognitive ability. In addition, all participants completed tests of attention (Digit Span Forwards subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale [48] and Trail Making Test A [49]), visuospatial ability (Rey Complex Figure [50]), confrontation naming (Sydney Language Battery, naming subscale [51]) and face perception (Face Matching Task [3]). These tests were administered as part of a larger cognitive assessment battery.
Facial Emotion Recognition Tests.
Integrity of facial emotion recognition was measured using the Ekman 60 [52], [53] and the Ekman Caricatures [52] tasks. On both tasks, participants viewed faces expressing one of six basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, sadness, surprise, happiness), on a computer screen, one at a time, in a pseudorandom order. Images were presented for 5 sec and participants were instructed to determine the emotional label (anger, disgust, fear, sadness, surprise or happiness) that best matched the facial emotion being displayed. Understanding of the emotional labels was confirmed prior to the task and participants unable to understand the labels were excluded from the study. Participants had unlimited time to respond. For the Ekman 60, the computer automatically recorded the participant’s response, whereas for the Caricatures task, the researcher recorded the response. For both tasks, participants responded by using the mouse, pointing or saying their response. The emotional labels remained on the screen until the participant responded. No feedback was provided.
Stimuli included in the Ekman 60 were photographs of natural facial expressions (100% emotional intensity), 10 exemplars per emotion. On the Caricatures task, the stimuli were expressions of two models (MO and JJ) from the Ekman 60 task that have been digitally manipulated to increase the emotional intensity of the natural expression by +15%, +30%, +50% or +75%, resulting in 8 exemplars per emotion [52]. For the purposes of this study, scores for the Caricatures task were derived by averaging across the four levels of intensity, for each basic emotion.
Image acquisition
All participants underwent whole brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) with a 3-Tesla (3-T) Phillips MRI scanner with standard quadrature head coil (8 channels). High resolution T1-images were obtained in the coronal plane using the following protocol: 256×256 matrix, 200 slices, 1 mm3 isotropic voxels, echo time/repetition time = 2.6/5.8 ms, flip angle α = 19°.
Data preprocessing
MRI data were analysed using FSL-voxel-based morphometry [54]–[56], part of the FMRIB software library package (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslvbm/index.html; [57].) Structural images were brain-extracted using BET, following which, tissue segmentation was conducted using FMRIB’s automatic segmentation tool (FAST) [58]. Grey matter partial volume maps were aligned to Montreal Neurological Institute standard space (MNI152) using non-linear registration (FNIRT) [59], [60], which uses a b-spline representation of the registration warp field [61]. A study-specific template was created and the native grey matter images were then non-linearly re-registered. Modulation of the registered partial volume maps was carried out (to correct for local expansion or contraction), by dividing them by the Jacobian of the warp field. Note that the modulation did not include the affine part of the registration so that participants were matched for brain size. The modulated, segmented images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a sigma of 3mm (FWHM: 8 mm).
Behavioural analyses
Data were analyzed using SPSS version 20.0 (IBM, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Between-group comparisons for each of the relevant demographic and neuropsychological variables were performed using univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) or, where appropriate, analyses of covariance for continuous measures, and chi-square tests for categorical measures. Recognition of each basic emotion was investigated for the two facial emotion recognition tasks. For each task, a 6×4 repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with Emotion (Anger, Disgust, Fear, Sadness, Surprise, Happiness) as the repeated, within subjects variable and Diagnosis as the between subjects variable (Control, bvFTD, SD, PNFA). Effect sizes using partial eta squared (ηp2) were calculated for main and interaction effects. Follow-up post hoc analyses (Bonferroni correction) were conducted to investigate differences across groups for each individual emotion.
Voxel-based morphometry analyses
A voxel-wise general linear model was applied to investigate grey matter intensity differences, using permutation-based, non-parametric statistics, with 5000 permutations per contrast [62]. Differences in cortical grey matter intensities between each patient group (bvFTD, SD, PNFA) and controls were assessed using t-tests. Age was included as a nuisance variable for all contrasts. For the atrophy analyses, the statistical threshold was set at p<.05, fully corrected for multiple comparisons [Family Wise Error (FWE)].
Next, correlations between behavioural performances, specific to each emotion type, on the two emotion recognition tasks were conducted. Two separate sets of identical contrasts were employed across both versions of the emotion recognition tasks (Ekman 60, Ekman Caricatures) for each emotion type (Anger, Disgust, Fear, Sadness, Surprise, Happiness). Firstly, all 6 emotions were entered simultaneously into the design matrix. Then, specific contrasts to investigate the neural correlates of each basic emotion independent of the others were run concurrently (e.g., for six emotions, neglecting nuisance variables the contrasts were: anger [1,0,0,0,0,0], disgust [0,1,0,0,0,0,], fear [0,0,1,0,0,0], sadness [0,0,0,1,0,0], surprise [0,0,0,0,1,0], happiness [0,0,0,0,0,1]). This approach allowed us to investigate the unique associations between grey-matter intensity and performance for each emotion of interest while covarying for performance on each of the other emotions (see [18]). Age was included as a nuisance variable, for all contrasts. Correlations between emotion recognition performance in each emotion subtype and regions of grey matter intensity were investigated in bvFTD, SD and PNFA subtypes and healthy controls combined. This method has been adopted in previous studies including FTD subtypes [63], [64] and serves to achieve greater variance in behavioural scores, thus increasing the statistical power to detect brain-behaviour relationships. For the behavioural analyses, no significant associations were present following correction for multiple comparisons using FWE. Therefore, for all behavioural analyses, the significance threshold was set at p<.001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons. An additional conservative cluster extent threshold of 100 contiguous voxels was used to reduce the likelihood of false positive voxels [65], [66]. This approach reduces the level of Type I errors while mitigating the risks of Type II errors [67].
Anatomical locations of significant results were overlaid on the MNI standard brain, with maximum coordinates provided in MNI stereotaxic space. Anatomical labels were determined with reference to the Harvard-Oxford probabilistic cortical and subcortical atlases.
Results
Demographic and Background Neuropsychological Data
Groups were matched for sex, age and education (all p values >.05) (Table 1). Disease duration differed across patient groups, with SD having longer disease duration than PNFA. This difference reflects the generally longer time to diagnosis in SD compared to other FTD subtypes. On the general cognitive screening measure ACE-R, an overall effect of diagnosis was present, with all patient groups performing worse than controls. The SD group scored lower than the bvFTD and PNFA groups, reflecting the semantic language demands on this task. Cognitive testing revealed neuropsychological profiles that were generally characteristic of each patient group. BvFTD showed poorer attention compared to controls on the Trail Making Test A and poor visuoconstruction on the Rey Complex Figure copy, indicating reduced planning and organisation on this task. BvFTD also showed some reductions in confrontation naming, although to a lesser extent than the SD group, and poorer simple face processing, compared to controls. The SD group was significantly impaired on the naming task, but performed within normal limits on tasks of attention and visuospatial functioning, consistent with their clinical presentation. The PNFA group performed poorly on tasks of auditory attention and naming, which reflects their reduced expressive language output. Processing speed, based on their performance on the Trail Making Test A, was also reduced compared to controls (Table 1).
Emotion Recognition: Behavioural data
On the Ekman 60, a significant effect of diagnosis was present (F(3,63) = 17.6, p<.001, ηp2 = .456), and a significant main effect of emotion was also observed (F(1,63) = 50.0, p<.001, ηp2 = .443). Importantly, an interaction between emotion and diagnosis was present (F(13,263) = 3.0, p<.001, ηp2 = .126). Post hoc analyses revealed a significant effect of diagnosis for all negative emotions. A significant effect of diagnosis was also seen for happiness, although performance across all groups approached ceiling for this emotion (i.e., above 90% for patients and controls) (Table 2). Post hoc analyses investigating the performance of each diagnostic group for each basic emotion revealed that bvFTD and SD performed poorer than controls for all negative emotions, while PNFA showed significant deficits with respect to controls for the recognition of anger, fear and sadness. BvFTD, SD and PNFA did not perform significantly differently from each other for any of the specific emotions (Table 2).
On the Caricatures task, a significant effect of diagnosis was present (F(3,63) = 15.0, p<.001, ηp2 = .416) and a main effect of emotion was also observed (F(4,266) = 27.9, p<.001, ηp2 = .307). Again, an interaction between diagnosis and emotion was evident (F(13, 266) = 5.593, p<.001, ηp2 = .210). Post hoc analyses investigating the effect of diagnosis for each emotion, revealed a significant effect of diagnosis for each negative emotion only (Table 2). The bvFTD and SD group performed poorer than controls for all negative emotions, and worse than the PNFA group for recognition of fear. Recognition of disgust in SD was significantly worse than the PNFA group, and both bvFTD and SD performed poorer than PNFA for recognition of fear. No significant differences between PNFA and controls, or between bvFTD and SD were present for any of the specific emotions on this task (Table 2).
We also investigated the possible role of face perception and attention on emotion recognition performance, by repeating the same analyses for the Ekman 60 and Caricatures, including Face Matching and Digit Span Forwards (maximum span), as covariates. The interaction between Emotion and Diagnosis remained significant for both the Ekman 60 (F(15, 240) = 2.382, p = .005, ηp2 = .111) and the Caricatures (F(15, 233) = 4.389, p<.001, ηp2 = .188) tasks, although the size of the effect was smaller. Together, these results indicate that while impairments in attention and face perception ability contribute to poor emotion recognition performance in FTD, they do not account entirely for the emotion recognition deficits seen in these patients.
In summary, significant emotion recognition impairments were found for all negative emotions, across both emotion recognition tasks. Patients with bvFTD and SD displayed pervasive deficits across all negative emotions, whereas disgust was preserved in PNFA patients. In the bvFTD and SD groups, these deficits persisted even after increasing the emotional intensity of the stimuli, with both groups continuing to be significantly impaired across all negative emotions. In contrast, increasing the emotional intensity reduced the emotion processing difficulties in the PNFA group.
Voxel-based morphometry - Group analysis
Patterns of atrophy.
Patients with bvFTD showed grey matter intensity decrease in the frontal pole and orbitofrontal regions bilaterally, extending into the right parahippocampal and hippocampal regions, amygdala and thalamus, and the left medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate and paracingulate regions, compared to controls (Figure 1). Patients with SD showed grey matter intensity decrease, primarily in the left temporal pole region, extending to the left orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, amygdala, hippocampus, putamen, insula, middle and superior temporal gyrus, compared to controls. PNFA showed reduced grey matter intensity in the left inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus and insula, compared to controls.
Voxel-based morphometry analyses showing brain regions of decreased grey matter intensity in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD): x = 8, y = 28, z = –28, t>3.73 (top); semantic dementia (SD): x = –32, y = –10, z = –50, t>3.73 (middle); and progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA): x = – 54, y = –10, z = 8, t>3.21 (bottom) relative to controls. Coloured voxels show regions that were significant in the analyses with p<.05 corrected for multiple comparisons (FWE). Age included as a nuisance variable for all contrasts. Clusters are overlaid on the standard Montreal Neurological Institute brain. R = right, L = left, P = posterior, A = anterior.
Neural Correlates of Emotion Recognition.
Anger: Ekman 60 performance was associated with grey matter intensity of the left superior and middle temporal gyrus, right precuneus, and left posterior parahippocampal gyrus and lateral occipital cortex. On the Caricatures task, a significant cluster in the left middle temporal gyrus, extending into the superior temporal gyrus and insula was also identified, along with a more extensive network of regions that included the left temporal pole, fusiform cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, hippocampus and occipital pole (Table 3, Figure 2).
Voxel-based morphometry analyses showing common brain regions in which grey matter intensity in all participants combined correlated significantly with emotion recognition for each basic emotion on the Ekman 60 (Red) and Caricatures (Blue) tasks. MNI coordinates: Anger: x = –52, y = –44, z = –6; Disgust: x = –40, y = –6, z = –32; Fear: x = 28, y = –10, z = –10; and Sadness: x = –12, y = 30, z = –16. Coloured voxels show regions that were significant in the analyses with p<.001 uncorrected for all contrasts. Age included as a nuisance variable for all contrasts. All clusters reported t>3.95. Clusters are overlaid on the Montreal Neurological Institute standard brain. R = right, L = left, P = posterior, A = anterior.
Disgust: On the Ekman 60 task, a region in the left insula, extending into the orbitofrontal cortex was significantly associated with disgust recognition, along with regions in the right thalamus and the temporal poles bilaterally. On the Caricatures task, a region of grey matter intensity in the left insula was again associated with disgust recognition, as well as regions in the left temporal pole and orbitofrontal cortex. In addition, the left occipital cortex was also associated with disgust recognition on this task (Table 3, Figure 2).
Fear: Ekman 60 performance was associated with grey matter intensity in the right amygdala, hippocampus, and the left anterior cingulate, extending into the paracingulate gyrus. Regions in the right inferior temporal gyrus, right temporal pole, orbitofrontal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus, together with a region in the left temporal fusiform cortex was also associated with fear recognition on this task. Similarly on the Caricatures task, the right amygdala, hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex were associated with fear recognition, along with a more extensive network of regions that included the planum temporale, Heschl’s gyrus, frontal cortex, including the subcallosal cortex, medial and superior frontal cortex and occipital regions including the occipital pole and the left lingual gyrus (Table 3, Figure 2).
Sadness: Ekman 60 performance was associated with the left subcallosal cingulate cortex. No clusters larger than 100 contiguous voxels were associated with sadness recognition on the Caricatures task (Table 3, Figure 2). A summary of the distinct brain regions correlating with recognition of anger, disgust, fear and sadness is presented in Figure 3.
Voxel-based morphometry analyses showing dissociable brain areas in which grey matter intensity in all participants combined, correlated significantly with emotion recognition irrespective of task for the emotions: Anger (Red), Disgust (Green), Fear (Blue) and Sadness (Yellow). Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates: x = –6, y = –6, z = –28; Coloured voxels show regions that were significant in the analyses with p<.001 uncorrected for all contrasts. Age included as a nuisance variable for all contrasts. All clusters reported t>3.95. Clusters are overlaid on the Montreal Neurological Institute standard brain. P = posterior, A = anterior, R = right, L = left.
Recognition of positive emotions, happiness and surprise, were included in the design matrix, in order to take into account task performance across all basic emotions. No significant clusters were identified for recognition of surprise, which is consistent with the lack of group differences on performance for this emotion across both tasks. Performance for recognition of happiness was at ceiling in almost all participants, regardless of group membership. While small significant clusters were identified in the VBM analyses associated with happiness (Table S1), these findings are likely to have limited clinical meaning, given the small number of participants who committed any errors on this task.
Discussion
This study has successfully established the existence of dissociable neural regions supporting recognition of the four basic negative emotions: fear, disgust, anger and sadness, emotions for which neural correlates have not been clearly identified in FTD. In keeping with our hypotheses, fear recognition was associated with the right amygdala, hippocampus, and anterior cingulate, whereas, disgust recognition was associated with the left insula and left temporal pole. In addition, this study also uncovered associations between anger recognition and the region surrounding the left superior temporal sulcus, and between sadness recognition and the subcallosal cingulate. Together these results provide strong support for the applicability of the multimodal system model of emotion for understanding the emotion processing deficits seen in FTD.
The right amygdala and anterior cingulate were the only regions involved in fear recognition, irrespective of task. The association between the right amygdala and fear recognition in FTD converges strongly with previous findings and demonstrates the integral nature of this structure for processing fear [31], [34], [35], [68]–[71]. The anterior cingulate cortex has previously been shown to activate during recognition of fearful expressions in healthy adults [70]. Our results provide additional support for the critical role of the anterior cingulate for processing fear signals. The concurrent involvement of these structures for processing fear-related stimuli likely reflects their strong bidirectional connectivity [72]–[74].
The ability to correctly identify facial stimuli conveying the emotion disgust was associated with the integrity of the insula, resonating with previous reports in the literature [37], [38], [40], [75]. Specialisation within the insula likely explains the relatively intact disgust recognition in PNFA despite the marked insula atrophy observed in this group. The anteroventral portion of the insula is specialised for emotion processing, whereas dorsal regions are predominantly associated with language expression [76], reflecting the language deficits associated with the burden of atrophy observed in PNFA. In contrast, the well-documented deficits in disgust recognition in Huntington’s disease indicate an involvement of ventral, emotion-processing, insular regions. Detailed investigations will be required to clarify the nature and functions of the insula subdivisions in cognitive and emotion processing [76], [77].
The left superior temporal sulcus was the single brain region identified by both tasks during processing of angry facial stimuli. In healthy individuals, regions surrounding the superior temporal sulcus activate when viewing angry faces, and when detecting anger from meaningless speech, providing converging support for the specialisation of this region in anger recognition [75], [78]. In contrast, sadness recognition was associated with subcallosal cingulate cortex integrity. This association mirrors functional imaging findings in healthy adults [70], [79], [80] and in depressed individuals [81], suggesting that abnormal changes in this region may not only affect facial emotion recognition, but may also contribute to depressed mood in some FTD patients. Evidence supporting the existence of specialised regions for recognition of sadness and anger is less well established than for fear and disgust. Our results provide important evidence for the role of the superior temporal sulcus and the subcallosal cingulate for the recognition of anger and sadness respectively.
The novel findings of discrete neural correlates for the emotions fear, disgust, anger and sadness, in FTD, are consistent with the multimodal system model of emotion [2] (Figure 4). Consistent with previous research in FTD, our study also identified structures generally implicated in emotion recognition, including the orbitofrontal cortex and temporal regions [5], [9], [18], [19]. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex for recognition of multiple basic emotions is in keeping with its central role in emotion processing [82]–[86], while the temporal pole and the fusiform gyrus have been shown to play an important role in processing of social and emotional stimuli, and face perception [87], [88]. Together, our results demonstrate the presence of dissociable neural substrates specialised for the recognition of specific negative emotions, as well as the existence of structures important for general aspects of emotion processing. These findings demonstrate the utility of neurodegenerative disorders in testing models of complex behaviour, such as emotion recognition.
Proposed neural correlates associated with processing of basic emotions based on current and prior results from studies including frontotemporal dementia (FTD) patients. Anger (Red), Fear (Blue), Sadness (Yellow). Structures implicated in general emotion processing are shown in orange. 1: Left orbitofrontal cortex; 2: Left temporal pole; 3: Left superior temporal sulcus; 4: Anterior cingulate; 5: Subcallosal cingulate; 6: Right amygdala; 7: Right temporal pole, 8: Right orbitofrontal cortex. Note. Insula, which is implicated in the processing of disgust stimuli, is not visible on lateral or sagittal views. Neuroanatomical regions are approximate.
By combining two facial emotion recognition tasks that varied in the emotional intensity of the facial expression, we were able to determine the contribution of attentional and perceptual deficits to emotion recognition performance in FTD. Consistent with previous results, the behavioural analyses indicated that impairments in early cognitive processes (attention, visuospatial) do not explain fully, the profound emotion processing deficits observed in some FTD patients [7], [89]. Importantly, while this study successfully linked the recognition of basic negative emotions to specific brain structures, neural correlates underlying recognition of positive emotions remain elusive. The facts that severe deficits for the recognition of positive emotions are rare and that relatively few positive emotions can be portrayed via different facial expressions compared to negative emotions, probably account for this lack of evidence. Stimuli that can measure various facets of positive emotions and emotion recognition tasks of greater difficulty that will avoid ceiling effects are needed in order to detect high-level deficits in recognition of positive emotions [90], [91].
This study used FTD as a model to examine brain-behaviour relationships of emotion processing. While the number of patients may appear small, the sample size compares favourably with other studies investigating clinical features of neurodegenerative disorders. Results, however, need to be interpreted with this caveat in mind; for example, VBM analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons (although a minimum cluster size of at least 100 voxels was applied to minimise spurious findings). This caution notwithstanding, the brain regions we identified are remarkably consistent with previous studies using fMRI, patient lesion and animal models, providing additional support for our results.
In summary, we have demonstrated the existence of emotion-specific brain regions necessary for the recognition of negative basic emotions. Damage to these structures produces marked impairments in primary emotion processing in FTD patients. Although previous studies in FTD have suggested that frontal and temporal lobe structures are important for emotion processing, this study is the first to confirm that dissociable brain regions are necessary for recognition of different basic emotions in FTD patients. In particular, associations between the amygdala and fear recognition and the insula and disgust recognition confirm that these brain regions are specialised for processing these emotion signal types. Importantly, by using two tasks of facial emotion recognition that varied in perceptual and attentional demands, we also successfully identified discrete neural regions associated with anger and sadness recognition, supporting the hypothesis that specialised brain regions also exist for processing these negative emotions. The ability to recognise emotions in others is a complex process. Here, we have shown that specialised neural regions across the frontal and temporal lobes are necessary for recognising different emotions, providing further support for the existence of “basic emotions” and improving our understanding of the neurobiological basis of emotion processing.
Supporting Information
Table S1.
Voxel-based morphometry analyses showing significant correlations between grey matter intensity and recognition of happiness on the Ekman 60 and Ekman Caricatures tasks. Note. All results uncorrected at p<.001; only clusters with at least 100 contiguous voxels are reported. All clusters reported t>3.95. MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute. No clusters greater than 100 voxels were significantly associated with surprise recognition on the Ekman 60 or Ekman Caricatures task. Age included as a nuisance variable for all contrasts.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067457.s001
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: FK MI JRH OP. Analyzed the data: FK MI OP. Wrote the paper: FK MI JRH OP.
References
- 1. Lavenu I, Pasquier F, Lebert F, Petit H, Van der Linden M (1999) Perception of emotion in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders 13: 96–101.
- 2. Kumfor F, Piguet O (2012) Disturbance of emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia: A synthesis of cognitive and neuroimaging findings. Neuropsychology Review 22: 280–297.
- 3. Miller L, Hsieh S, Lah S, Savage S, Hodges JR, et al. (2012) One size does not fit all: Face emotion processing impairments in semantic dementia, behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease are mediated by distinct cognitive deficits. Behavioural Neurology 25: 53–60.
- 4. Rosen HJ, Pace-Savitsky K, Perry RJ, Kramer JH, Miller BL, et al. (2004) Recognition of emotion in the frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 17: 277–281.
- 5. Rosen HJ, Perry RJ, Murphy J, Kramer JH, Mychack P, et al. (2002) Emotion comprehension in the temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 125: 2286–2295.
- 6. Rohrer JD, Sauter DA, Scott SK, Rossor MN, Warren JD (2010) Receptive prosody in nonfluent primary progressive aphasias. Cortex 48: 308–316.
- 7. Kumfor F, Miller L, Lah S, Hsieh S, Savage S, et al. (2011) Are you really angry? The effect of intensity on emotion recognition in frontotemporal dementia. Social Neuroscience 6: 502–514.
- 8. Snowden JS, Austin NA, Sembi S, Thompson JC, Craufurd D, et al. (2008) Emotion recognition in Huntington's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 46: 2638–2649.
- 9. Kipps CM, Nestor PJ, Acosta-Cabronero J, Arnold R, Hodges JR (2009) Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. The role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain 132: 592–603.
- 10. Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis A, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, et al. (2011) Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 76: 1006–1014.
- 11. Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman DS, Mendez MF, Kramer JH, et al. (2011) Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134: 2456–2477.
- 12. Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Goldman WP, Perry RJ, Schuff N, et al. (2002) Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Neurology 58: 198–208.
- 13. Kril JJ, Macdonald V, Patel S, Png F, Halliday G (2005) Distribution of brain atrophy in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 232: 83–90.
- 14. Perry RJ, Graham AJ, Williams GB, Rosen H, Erzinçlioglu S, et al. (2006) Patterns of frontal lobe atrophy in frontotemporal dementia: A volumetric MRI study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 22: 278–287.
- 15. Seeley WW, Crawford R, Rascovsky K, Kramer JH, Weiner M, et al. (2008) Frontal paralimbic network atrophy in very mild behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Archives of Neurology 65: 249–255.
- 16. Brambati SM, Rankin KP, Narvid J, Seeley WW, Dean D, et al. (2009) Atrophy progression in semantic dementia with assymmetric temporal involvement: A tensor-based morphometry study. Neurobiology of Aging 30: 103–111.
- 17. Fernandez-Duque D, Black S (2005) Impaired recognition of negative facial emotions in patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 43: 1673–1687.
- 18. Rosen HJ, Wilson MR, Schauer GF, Allison S, Gorno-Tempini ML, et al. (2006) Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired recognition of emotion in dementia. Neuropsychologia 44: 365–373.
- 19. Bertoux M, Volle E, Funkiewiez A, de Souza LC, Leclercq D, et al. (2012) Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment (SEA) is a marker of medial and orbital frontal functions: A voxel-based morphometry study in behavioral variant of frontotemporal degeneration. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 18: 972–985.
- 20. LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23: 155–184.
- 21. Whalen PJ (1998) Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science 7: 177–188.
- 22. Adolphs R (2002) Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 12: 169–177.
- 23. Adolphs R (2002) Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 1: 21–62.
- 24. Izard CE (1992) Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review 99: 561–565.
- 25. Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion 6: 169–200.
- 26. Calder AJ, Lawrence AD, Young AW (2001) Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 352–363.
- 27.
Darwin C (1872) The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.
- 28. Hennenlotter A, Schroeder U (2006) Partly dissociable neural substrates for recognizing basic emotions: A critical review. Progress in Brain Research 156: 443–456.
- 29. Calder AJ, Keane J, Manes F, Antoun N, Young AW (2000) Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nature Neuroscience 3: 1077–1079.
- 30. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A (1994) Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 372: 669–672.
- 31. Hitchcock J, Davis M (1986) Lesions of the amygdala, but not of the cerebellum or red nucleus, block conditioned fear as measured with the potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioural Neuroscience 100: 11–22.
- 32. Morris JS, Friston KJ, Buchel C, Frith CD, Young AW, et al. (1998) A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain 121: 47–57.
- 33. Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, et al. (1996) A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 383: 812–815.
- 34. Calder AJ, Young AW, Rowland D, Perrett DI, Hodges JR, et al. (1996) Facial emotion recognition after bilateral amygdala damage: Differentially severe impairment of fear. Cognitive Neuropsychology 13: 699–745.
- 35. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A (1995) Fear and the human amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience 15: 5879–5891.
- 36. Sprengelmeyer R, Young AW, Schroeder U, Grossenbacher PG, Federlein J, et al. (1999) Knowing no fear. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B 266: 2451–2456.
- 37. Kipps CM, Duggins AJ, McCusker EA, Calder AJ (2007) Disgust and happiness recognition correlate with anteroventral insula and amygdala volume respectively in preclinical Huntington's disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19: 1206–1217.
- 38. Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, et al. (1997) A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389: 495–498.
- 39. Phillips ML, Young AW, Scott SK, Calder AJ, Andrew C, et al. (1998) Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365: 1809–1817.
- 40. Wright P, He G, Shapira NA, Goodman WK, Liu Y (2004) Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination. Neuroreport 15: 2347–2351.
- 41. Sprengelmeyer R, Young AW, Calder AJ, Karnat A, Lange H, et al. (1996) Loss of disgust: Perception of faces and emotions in Huntington's disease. Brain 119: 1647–1665.
- 42. Blair RJ, Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Dolan RJ (1999) Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain 122: 883–893.
- 43. Harmer CJ, Thilo KV, Rithwell JC, Goodwin GM (2001) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of medial-frontal cortex impairs the processing of angry facial expressions. Nature Neuroscience 4: 17–18.
- 44. Irish M, Piguet O, Hodges JR (2012) Self-projection and the default network in frontotemporal dementia. Nature Reviews Neurology 8: 152–161.
- 45. Calder AJ, Young AW, Rowland D, Perrett D (1997) Computer-enhanced emotion in facial expressions. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B 264: 919–925.
- 46. Calder AJ, Rowland D, Young AW, Nimmo-Smith I, Keane J, et al. (2000) Caricaturing facial expressions. Cognition 76: 105–146.
- 47. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR (2006) The Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21: 1078–1085.
- 48.
Wechsler D (1997) WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San Antonio: TX: The Psychological Corporation.
- 49. Tombaugh TN (2004) Trail making test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19: 203–214.
- 50.
Meyers J, Meyers K (1995) The Meyers scoring system for the Rey Complex Figure and the recognition trial: Professional manual. Odessa: FL: Psychological Assessment Resourses.
- 51.
Savage S, Hsieh S, Leslie F, Foxe D, Piguet O, et al. (in press) Distinguishing subtypes in primary progressive aphasia: Application of the Sydney Language Battery. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.
- 52.
Young AW, Perrett DI, Calder AJ, Sprengelmeyer R, Ekman P (2002) Facial Expressions of Emotion - Stimuli and Tests (FEEST). Bury St Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company.
- 53.
Ekman P, Friesen WV (1976) Pictures of Facial Affect. San Francisco: CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
- 54. Ashburner J, Friston KJ (2000) Voxel-based morphometry - the methods. Neuroimage 11: 805–821.
- 55. Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, et al. (2009) Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45: S173–186.
- 56. Mechelli A, Price CJ, Friston KJ, Ashburner J (2005) Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: Methods and applications. Current Medical Imaging Reviews 1: 105–113.
- 57. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, et al. (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23: 208–219.
- 58. Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S (2001) Segmentation of brain MR images though a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximisation algorithm. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 20: 45–57.
- 59.
Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S (2007) Non-linear optimisation. FMRIB Technical report TR07JA1. Oxford: University of Oxford FMRIB Centre.
- 60.
Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S (2007) Non-linear registration, aka spatial normalisation. FMRIB Technical report TR07JA2. Oxford: University of Oxford FMRIB Centre.
- 61. Rueckert D, Sonoda LI, Hayes C, Hill DL, Leach MO, et al. (1999) Nonrigid registration using free-form deformations: Application to breast MR images. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 18: 712–721.
- 62. Nichols TE, Holmes AP (2002) Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Human Brain Mapping 15: 1–25.
- 63. Sollberger M, Stanley CM, Wilson SM, Gyurak A, Beckman V, et al. (2009) Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia 47: 2817–2827.
- 64. Irish M, Addis DR, Hodges JR, Piguet O (2012) Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: evidence from semantic dementia. Brain 135: 2178–2191.
- 65. Wu X, Yu C, Fan F, Zhang K, Zhu C, et al. (2011) Correlation between progressive changes in piriform cortex and olfactory performance in early Parkinson's disease. European Neurology 66: 98–105.
- 66. Kubota M, Miyata J, Hirao K, Fujiwara H, Kawada R, et al. (2011) Alexithymia and regional gray matter alterations in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Research 70: 206–213.
- 67. Lieberman MD, Cunningham WA (2009) Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4: 423–428.
- 68. Phan KL, Wager TD, Taylor SF, Liberzon I (2004) Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions. Neuroimage 9: 258–266.
- 69. Fusar-Poli P, Pacentino A, Carletti F, Landi P, Allen P, et al. (2009) Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: A voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 34: 418–432.
- 70. Vytal K, Hamann SB (2010) Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22: 2864–2885.
- 71. Broks P, Young AW, Maratos EJ, Coffey PJ, Calder AJ, et al. (1998) Face processing impairments after encephalitis: Amygdala damage and recognition of fear. Neuropsychologia 36: 59–70.
- 72. Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Fera F, Weinberger DR (2003) Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biological Psychiatry 53: 494–501.
- 73. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences 4: 215–222.
- 74. Hariri AR, Bookheimer SY, Mazziotta JC (2000) Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport 11: 43–48.
- 75. Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Przuntek H (1998) Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B 265: 1927–1931.
- 76. Mutschler I, Wieckhorst B, Kowalevski S, Derix J, Wentlandt J, et al. (2009) Functional organisation of the human anterior insular cortex. Neuroscience Letters 457: 66–70.
- 77. Craig AD (2009) How do you feel - now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10: 59–70.
- 78. Grandjean D, Sander D, Pourtois G, Schwartz S, Seghier ML, et al. (2005) The voices of wrath: brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech. Nature Neuroscience 8: 145–146.
- 79. Murphy FC, Nimmo-Smith I, Lawrence AD (2003) Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioural Neuroscience 3: 207–233.
- 80. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I (2002) Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 16: 331–348.
- 81. Hamani C, Mayberg H, Stone S, Laxton A, Haber S, et al. (2011) The subcallosal cingulate gyrus in the context of major depression. Biological Psychiatry 69: 301–308.
- 82. Goodkind MS, Sollberger M, Gyurak A, Rosen HJ, Rankin KP, et al. (2011) Tracking emotional valence: The role of the orbitofrontal cortex. Human Brain Mapping 33: 753–762.
- 83. Heberlein AS, Padon AA, Gillihan SJ, Farah MJ, Fellows LK (2008) Ventromedial frontal lobe plays a critical role in facial emotion recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20: 721–733.
- 84. Hornak J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, O'Doherty J, et al. (2003) Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain 126: 1691–1712.
- 85. Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET (2004) The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology 72: 341–372.
- 86. Rolls ET (2004) The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition 55: 11–29.
- 87. Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y (2007) The enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130: 1718–1731.
- 88. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (1997) The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialised for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience 17: 4302–4311.
- 89.
Kumfor F, Piguet O (in press) Emotion recognition in the dementias: Brain correlates and patient implications. Neurodegenerative Disease Management.
- 90. Aviezer H, Trope Y, Todorov A (2012) Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science 338: 1225–1229.
- 91. Hsieh S, Foxe D, Leslie F, Savage S, Piguet O, et al. (2012) Grief and joy: Emotion word comprehension in the dementias. Neuropsychology 26: 624–630.