Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Paternal Care Decreases Foraging Activity and Body Condition, but Does Not Impose Survival Costs to Caring Males in a Neotropical Arachnid

  • Gustavo S. Requena ,

    billy.requena@gmail.com

    Affiliation Departamento de Ecologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP, Brazil

  • Bruno A. Buzatto,

    Affiliation Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Animal Biology, University of Western Australia - Crawley, WA, Australia

  • Eduardo G. Martins,

    Affiliations Institute of Environmental Science and Department of Biology, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada, Centre for Applied Conservation Research, Department of Forest Sciences, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

  • Glauco Machado

    Affiliation Departamento de Ecologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP, Brazil

Abstract

Exclusive paternal care is the rarest form of parental investment in nature and theory predicts that the maintenance of this behavior depends on the balance between costs and benefits to males. Our goal was to assess costs of paternal care in the harvestman Iporangaia pustulosa, for which the benefits of this behavior in terms of egg survival have already been demonstrated. We evaluated energetic costs and mortality risks associated to paternal egg-guarding in the field. We quantified foraging activity of males and estimated how their body condition is influenced by the duration of the caring period. Additionally, we conducted a one-year capture-mark-recapture study and estimated apparent survival probabilities of caring and non-caring males to assess potential survival costs of paternal care. Our results indicate that caring males forage less frequently than non-caring individuals (males and females) and that their body condition deteriorates over the course of the caring period. Thus, males willing to guard eggs may provide to females a fitness-enhancing gift of cost-free care of their offspring. Caring males, however, did not show lower survival probabilities when compared to both non-caring males and females. Reduction in mortality risks as a result of remaining stationary, combined with the benefits of improving egg survival, may have played an important and previously unsuspected role favoring the evolution of paternal care. Moreover, males exhibiting paternal care could also provide an honest signal of their quality as offspring defenders, and thus female preference for caring males could be responsible for maintaining the trait.

Introduction

Trivers' [1] classical definition of parental investment postulates that even simple forms of parental care, such as egg-guarding, should include both benefits (enjoyed by the offspring) and costs (directly paid by parental individuals). The benefits of parental care to the offspring include improving micro-climatic conditions, such as reducing risk of dehydration and/or increasing egg aeration, protection against predators, parasitoid or fungal attack, as well as provisioning water or food to juveniles [2]. The costs paid by parental individuals are generally classified into three main categories [2][3]: (a) energetic costs, as a consequence of either reduced feeding opportunities or increased metabolic expense while caring for the offspring; (b) survival costs, as an ultimate consequence of starvation or increased susceptibility of the tending parent(s) to predators, parasites, and parasitoids; and (c) reproductive costs, involving loss of additional mating opportunities.

Parental care generally prevents foraging activities of parental individuals and can also be associated with expensive behaviors, such as providing offspring with food [2]. Therefore, maternal care is energetically costly for females, especially in the case of iteroparous species [4], since it reduces the available energy to produce additional eggs, negatively affecting females' future reproduction and fecundity (e.g., [5][9]). Among arthropods exhibiting exclusive paternal care, the available data are equivocal. Studies with giant water bugs (Belostomatinae), whose males carry egg pads attached to their backs, demonstrated that paternal behavior carries energetic costs for parental individuals by both decreasing their foraging efficiency and food intake [10], and by increasing their muscular activity while promoting water flow and oxygen diffusion through the eggs' membrane [11][12]. On the other hand, studies with the sea spider Achelia simplissima [13] and the assassin bug Rhinocoris tristis [9] showed that, besides differences in movement and activity patterns between caring and non-caring individuals, paternal care does not negatively affect foraging efficiency or weight loss, respectively.

Conflicting evidence is also the case for the survival costs of exclusive paternal care, for which empirical data are restricted to a few insect species. Observational data on the thrips Hoplothrips karnyi showed that caring males remain near communal egg masses, and the protection of these eggs against attacking conspecific males increases their mortality as a consequence of fighting injuries [14]. On the other hand, a laboratory study with the giant water bug Belostoma flumineum showed that the mean lifespan of males that had their egg pads removed was not different from either virgin or brooding males [15]. Only two studies using mark-recapture techniques to compare apparent survival probabilities between caring and non-caring males in the field have been conducted so far. For the giant water bug Abedus breviceps, males with eggs on their backs paid no survival costs [16], whereas for the assassin bug R. tristis, there was evidence of survival costs for caring males [9].

Finally, the reduction of mating opportunities for males during parental care is often pointed out as the most important cost of paternal care in endotherms because a trade-off is expected to exist between parental effort and mating effort [1], [3], [17]. Among many fishes and arthropods, however, eggs laid by different females can be guarded simultaneously, greatly reducing the promiscuity costs for caring males. In fact, observational and experimental evidence for these animal groups clearly show that providing paternal care and acquiring new mates are non-mutually exclusive activities [18][20]. Moreover, theoretical studies have suggested that female preferences for caring males have played an important role in the evolution of paternal care [18], [21][22]. Experimental evidence supporting this suggestion has already been reported for several fishes (e.g., [23][26]) and at least two arthropod species [9], [27].

Although many theoretical models predict that the maintenance of parental care depends on the balance between costs and benefits of this behavior to the parents [2], [20], [22], empirical information among arthropods is restricted mostly to species exhibiting maternal care (e.g., [6][8], but see [9], [16]). In this paper, we used the Neotropical harvestman Iporangaia pustulosa (Arachnida: Opiliones) as a model organism to assess energetic and survival costs of male egg-guarding behavior under field conditions. Iporangaia females lay their eggs on the underside surface of leaves, secrete an abundant hygroscopic mucus coat on the clutch, and then abandon the eggs under males' protection [28][29] (Fig. 1A). During most part of the year, caring males remain on the eggs almost all the time [28], and their presence has a crucial protective role, given that unprotected eggs suffer intense predation in a few days [29]. Caring for the offspring and acquiring new mates are not mutually exclusive activities because males are able to sequentially copulate with several females and usually care for all their eggs simultaneously in a single multiple clutch [28] (Fig. 1B). Given that the clutches are acquired asynchronously by caring males, the total caring period may last up to four consecutive months [28], during which males are likely to experience both higher mortality probabilities and lower food intake when compared to non-caring individuals (males and females).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Paternal care in the harvestman Iporangaia pustulosa.

(A) Male guarding eggs on the undersurface of a leaf. (B) Egg-batch composed of multiples clutches in different stages of embryonic development (see text for definition of each stage). Note that the eggs are covered by a thick mucus coat.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.g001

In this study, we evaluated energetic costs by quantifying foraging activity and estimating how body condition changes over the course of the caring period. Given that Iporangaia individuals feed mainly on dead arthropods, actively searching for food on the vegetation (G.S. Requena, unpublished data), we predicted that: (a) caring males would have fewer feeding opportunities when compared to non-caring individuals in the population, and (b) body condition would deteriorate over the course of the caring period. To evaluate mortality risk, we conducted a capture-mark-recapture study to estimate apparent survival probabilities and dissociate them from recapture probabilities [30]. Because parental care may increase the susceptibility of caring males to natural enemies, we predicted that their survival would be lower than that of non-caring males and females. Our study, entirely conducted in the field, provides a comprehensive understanding of the main costs paid by Iporangaia caring males. Moreover, our results challenge some widespread ideas on the costs of paternal care and bring insightful implications for the maintenance of paternal care in arthropods as a sexually selected trait.

Methods

Study Site

We carried out this study in an Atlantic Forest fragment at Intervales State Park (24°14′S; 48°04′W), in the state of São Paulo, southeastern Brazil. The region has high precipitation levels, with an average annual rainfall of 2000 to 3000 mm/year, and mean annual temperature ranging from 12 to 20°C. There is a well-marked seasonality in the locality, with a wet and warm period from October to March, and a dry and cold period from April to September, when frosts are common (Fig. 2A). We collected our data along a stream nearly 5 m wide and flanked by abundant vegetation, which sometimes partially covers the stream bed. We established a 200 m transect along this stream and inspected the vegetation at a maximum distance of 1 m from the water in both margins. All procedures presented in the following sections were not conducted with endangered or protected species, and are in accordance with relevant national and international guidelines to ensure ethical appropriateness, for which we obtained all necessary permits from the authority responsible for Intervales State Park (COTEC-IF permit number: 40.625/05).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Climatic conditions and number of Iporangaia egg-batches observed in the sampled area.

(A) The solid line represents temperature; black bars represent the rainfall during the dry-cold season; white bars represent the rainfall during the wet-warm season. (B) Dark-gray bars represent the number of new egg-batches found in each month (i.e., those that were not present in the transect in the previous months); light-gray bars represent old egg-batches (i.e., that were already present in the transect in the previous months).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.g002

Capture-mark-recapture

We conducted a one-year capture-mark-recapture study on our 200 m transect between August 2003 and July 2004. We inspected the vegetation flanking the stream three times a day (08.30–12.00 h, 14.30–18.00 h and 20.30–00.00 h) during four consecutive days per month. In each survey, we captured Iporangaia adults using an active searching method, recording their sex, their location along the transect (to the nearest 1 m), whether they were feeding and, for individuals captured for the first time, individually marking them with enamel color paint (following protocol described in [31]). After marking, we released the individuals at the same place where we had captured them. We classified males according to their parental state as caring (i.e., those that were guarding an egg-batch) or non-caring (i.e., those that were not guarding an egg-batch).

The capture-mark-recapture procedure also allowed us to estimate how males (caring and non-caring) and females moved along the transect. Considering only individuals that were recaptured at least five times and in at least two different months, we observed that both females (median = 5 m; range = 1–31 m) and non-caring males (median = 4 m; range = 1–60 m) moved similar distances among different sampling occasions and that it was always longer than the distances moved by caring males (median = 0 m, range = 0–10 m) (analysis of deviance: Δ deviance = 105.5, df = 1, P<0.001, Fig. 3A). This information is important to understand the results of the mark-recapture study because movement patterns may influence both recapture and mortality rates (see Discussion). Moreover, given that females and non-caring males have similar movement patterns and do not care for the offspring, we collectively classified them as ‘non-caring’ individuals in some analyses described below.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Movement pattern and feeding activity of Iporangaia adults.

(A) Estimated distances that individuals in different parental states moved in consecutive capture occasions. (B) Observed proportion of individuals (females, caring and non-caring males) found in feeding activities in the field. Different letters represent differences among groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.g003

Energetic Costs

To investigate energetic costs of paternal care in Iporangaia, we first quantified if there was any decrease in feeding activity to caring males when compared to other individuals in the population. Iporangaia individuals feed mostly on the vegetation and we recorded the monthly number of caring and non-caring individuals (females and males) observed in feeding activities during the capture-mark-recapture period and the total number of individuals recorded monthly in each category during the same period. These data are not without bias given that small food items are quickly consumed and recorded less frequently than large items. However, harvestmen are generalist and opportunistic consumers [32] so that we have no reason to suppose that caring and non-caring individuals exhibit preference for either small or large items. Thus, the number of individuals in each category that we found consuming food in the field seems to be a good proxy of their feeding activities.

We used a model selection approach based on the maximum likelihood method [33][34] to compare alternative generalized linear mixed models, representing different biological hypotheses. We built alternative models considering that the monthly proportion of feeding individuals was: (a) affected by neither sex nor parental state, (b) affected by sex (‘males’ vs. ‘females’), (c) affected by parental state (‘caring males’ vs. ‘non-caring males and females’), and (d) affected both individual categories (‘females’ vs. ‘caring’ vs. ‘non-caring males’). We built all models considering a binomial distribution of errors. Furthermore, given that we were not specifically interested in evaluating seasonal variation on foraging activity, but on the potential differences among categories, we used the sampling month as a random effect to control for such variation.

Then, we used the small sample size bias-corrected version of the Akaike Information Criterion (AICc) to rank all models fitted to the data [34]. Then, we selected the model with the lowest AICc value as the most parsimonious model describing the data. We also computed the difference in the AICc value between the selected model and all other models in the ranking and the relative weight of all models. When this difference is larger than 2, there is strong support to conclude that the model selected is the best one among all candidate models [34]. We built, fit, and selected models using the packages ‘bbmle’ [35] and ‘lme4’ [36] in the software R 2.11.1 [37].

Our second approach to evaluate energetic costs of paternal care was to quantify how males' body condition changes over the course of the caring period. Between January and March 2009, we sampled 93 Iporangaia males in the field and, using an electronic caliper (precision of 0.01 mm) and an electronic scale (precision of 0.001 g), we took the following measurements from each individual: (a) dorsal scute length (DSL), (b) total body length (TBL), (c) body width (BW) at the widest portion of the opisthosoma, (d) body height (BH) at the highest portion of the opisthosoma, and (e) total body mass (TBM). The dorsal scute (or carapace) is a rigid structure that does not change in size with food acquisition and can be classified as a structural body size measure (sensu [38]). The last five opisthosomal tergites, on the other hand, are not fused, but rather connected by a highly elastic membrane, allowing body expansion after a meal.

Although body dimensions are presumably correlated with current nutrient storage and have been broadly used to estimate body condition, they actually have the disadvantage of being simultaneously correlated with structural body size [38]. To remove the effect of the latter, we used two different proxies to assess males' body condition (as suggested by [39]): (a) body volume (V) controlled by a structural body size measure (i.e. DSL), and (b) body mass (TBM) controlled by body volume (V). In our case, we estimated V as an approximation of the ellipsoid according to the following formula: .

Given that Iporangaia eggs change in size and coloration over the course of the embryonic development [28], we estimated the time males had already invested in paternal activities based on features of their egg-batches. During the same summer that we collected data on the body condition of males, we also photographed 20 egg-batches on a daily basis and used the temporal sequence of photos to estimate the duration of each stage of embryonic development, creating an egg development schedule that was used as a proxy for the caring period (Table 1, Fig. 1B). For non-caring males, we attributed the value zero to the duration of their caring period. For caring males, we considered the oldest eggs in their egg-batches as the time invested in paternal activities (Table 1). The first stage corresponds to recently laid eggs, which are completely white and have not yet absorbed water from the environment. Eggs in the second stage have the same color as those of the first stage, but are larger because of water absorption. In the second stage it is also possible to identify clearly the embryonic formation of legs. Eggs in the third stage are larger than those of the second stage and are generally opaque or milky, with the legs not so clearly identifiable. In the following three categories there is no more difference in egg size. Eggs in the fourth stage are similar in color to those of the third stage, but it is possible to clearly identify two black spots corresponding to the eyes of the embryo. Eggs in the fifth stage are dark yellow or brownish, which corresponds to the beginning of tegumentary pigmentation of the embryo. In the sixth and last stage, immediately before hatching, eggs are almost black (modified from [28]).

thumbnail
Table 1. Duration of each stage of embryonic development in Iporangaia eggs during the wet and warm season and the corresponding estimated caring period.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.t001

Considering body volume as a proxy for body condition, we conducted the model selection procedure in two steps. First, to control for the effect of body size, we built models in which DSL: (a) does not affect V, (b) affects only the mean of V, (c) affects only the variance of V, and (d) affects both the mean and the variance of V. We used a linear function to model the influence on the mean parameter of V and a power function to model the variance parameter of V (as recommended by [40]). Using the best model selected in this first step, we then incorporated the effect of paternal care as: (a) the effect of caring period on the mean of V (controlled by body size), (b) caring period on the variance, (c) caring period on both parameters, (d) parental state on the mean, (e) parental state on the variance, and (f) parental state on both parameters. Using total body mass (TBM) as another proxy for body condition, we used the same two analytical steps described above to control for the effect of V and to evaluate the effect of paternal care on the mean and variance parameters of the TBM distribution. We conducted all these analyses and the model selection using the packages ‘bbmle’ [35] and ‘lme4’ [36] in the software R 2.11.1 [37], using AICc to rank the models fitted to the data [34], as described above.

Mortality Risk

To quantify the potential survival costs of male egg-guarding behavior, we estimated apparent survival (Φ) and recapture probabilities of females and males (caring and non-caring), using a statistical modeling approach [31] implemented in the software MARK [41]. We analyzed the data of all individuals together to be able to explicitly address models that consider the same Φ or among individuals of the same sex or individuals performing similar behaviors (parental state). Moreover, we pooled the capture-recapture data obtained during the three periods of the same day to generate a single sampling occasion per day. Therefore, our capture-recapture data set comprises 12 primary occasions (sampling months) and 46 secondary occasions (due to heavy rains, two months had only three days sampled). This so-called robust design model assumes that the population is open during the intervals between primary occasions, during which individuals may migrate, die or molt to the adult stage in the sampling area, but it is considered closed within each primary occasion because secondary occasions are so close together in time [42]. Therefore, it combines the advantages of closed capture models to estimate within primary occasions, at the ith month, with the advantages of the Cormack-Jolly-Seber live recapture model to estimate ψ between consecutive primary occasions, at the interval between the ith and the (i+1)th months (the model is described in details by [42]).

Male parental state is a varying condition in Iporangaia because caring males become non-caring males when nymphs hatch and disperse, while non-caring males may copulate and obtain a first clutch, thus becoming caring males. Therefore, we used multi-state models to estimate transition probabilities (ψ) between caring and non-caring states, at the interval between the ith and the (i+1)th months [43][45]. We used a ‘Huggins closed robust design multi-state model’, which does not include the abundance of individuals as a parameter of the model [42]. Furthermore, we also assumed the same probability to capture individuals for the first time and to recapture them within each primary occasion, and fixed ψ between males and females as zero.

We first assessed the fit of the global model to the capture-recapture history data. This global model considered that Φ and p were a function of time and individual categories (‘caring males’ vs. ‘non-caring males’ vs. ‘females’). Furthermore, the global model also considered that ψ between male states (‘caring’ vs. ‘non-caring’) was a function of time and parental state of males at the ith month. Our global model did not include interactions between time and individual categories (or parental state) because such a model did not reach convergence. We used the ratio of the model deviance by the model degrees of freedom, obtained by the goodness-of fit test for multi-state models performed in the software U-CARE [46], to estimate the overdispersion parameter of the global model (ĉ). The global model was considered to fit the data adequately if the estimated value of ĉ fell between 1 and 3, though the closer the value of ĉ is to 1, the better the fit of the model [41].

We compared the global model to three other general models incorporating different surrogates for time-dependent parameters. In the first model, we divided the study period into two seasons, corresponding to the wet-warm season (between October and March) and the dry-cold season (between April and September) (Fig. 2A). In the second and third models, we incorporated either temperature or rainfall as covariates representing time variation over the course of the study period, since both variables are strongly correlated with the reproductive activity of Iporangaia (Fig. 2B). For Φ and ψ estimates, we used the mean values of temperature and rainfall recorded for the days between two consecutive sampling occasions. For p estimates, we used the mean values of temperature and rainfall recorded during the four sampling days of each month. All additional models included interactions between time surrogates and individual categories (or parental state). We compared these four general models and selected the most parsimonious one using the small-sample size Akaike information criterion in the same way described in the Energetic Costs above, but corrected for overdispersion (QAICc).

After the selection of the general model, we built models in the following way. First, we fixed the global structure for Φ and p as dependent on the additive effect between the time-related variable and individual categories, and built alternative models that considered ψ between caring and non-caring males as being: (a) constant and not affected by male state at the ith month, (b) affected by the selected time variable, (c) affected by male state at the ith month (‘caring’ vs. ‘non-caring’), and (d) affected by the additive effect of the selected time-related variable and the male state at the ith month. We also built two additional models in which (e) ψ from caring to non-caring state was constant, but ψ from non-caring to caring state was dependent of the selected time variable, and (f) ψ from non-caring to caring state was constant, but ψ from caring to non-caring state dependent of the selected time variable. We compared all alternative models and selected the best one using the QAICc.

With the best selected structure for ψ, and with the structure for Φ still fixed as the additive effect between the time-related variable and individual categories, we built a new set of alternative models that considered p as being: (a) constant and not affected by individual categories, (b) affected by the selected time-related variable, (c) affected by individual categories, (d) affected by the additive effect of the time-related variable and individual categories, (e) constant for caring and non-caring males, but affected by the time-related variable for females, (f) constant for caring males and females, but affected by the time-related variable for non-caring males, (g) constant for non-caring males and females, but affected by the time-related variable for caring males, (h) affected by sex (‘all males together’ vs. ‘females’); (i) affected by the additive effect of the time-related variable and sex, (j) constant for males, but affected by the time-related variable for females, (k) constant for females, but affected by the time-related variable for males, (l) affected by parental state (‘non-caring males and females together’ vs. ‘caring males’), (m) affected by the additive effect of the time-related variable and parental state, (n) constant for caring individuals, but affected by the time-related variable for non-caring individuals, and (o) constant for non-caring individuals, but affected by the time-related variable for caring individuals, Finally, we built the same last 15 alternative models for Φ and repeated the model selection procedure.

We used the ‘step-down’ approach described above (and first presented by [30]) to avoid the comparison of all possible models in a single analysis, i.e., (4 structures for the general model)×(6 structures for ψ)×(15 structures for p)×(15 structures for Φ) = 5,400 models, which would be a prohibitive, time consuming procedure and would greatly increase the possibility of spurious results [34], [47]. However, it is still not clear if the order in which the structure of parameters is fixed or modeled affects the convergence of different approaches to the same best selected model [30], [47]. In an attempt to avoid biased results due to our specific analytical implementation, we also performed the model selection procedure starting with a general model in which all parameters were considered constant. Both procedures converged to the same best supported model given the capture history data observed. Therefore, for the sake of simplicity, we will focus our results on the first step-down model selection procedure, in which we started with all parameters as dependent of the additive effect between the time-related variable and individual categories. Furthermore, since the estimates of p and ψ are not the main goal of our study, we will focus here on the results on estimates of Φ. The results of the second model selection procedure are presented in the Supporting Information S1 and the values of all additional probabilities included in the best supported model can be found in the Supporting Information S2.

Results

Energetic Costs

We found 501 males and 349 females of Iporangaia, recording a total of 3,503 captures and recaptures between August 2003 and July 2004. Of all males captured in the study area, 66.4% were recorded only in the non-parental state (n = 333), 12.4% were recorded only in the parental state (n = 62), and 21.2% were recorded in both parental states (n = 106). During this period, we observed 60 individuals feeding on the vegetation and most of them were recorded in the afternoon (14.30–18.00 h; 40%) and at night (20.30–00.00 h; 42%). From all individuals found while feeding, 35 were females, 24 were non-caring males, and only one was a caring male. Therefore, there was a clear effect of individual categories on feeding activity: the frequency of caring males feeding in the field was significantly lower than the frequency of non-caring males, and females were more often found in feeding activities than males in general (Table 2; Fig. 3B).

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary of the model selection statistics for the analysis that evaluated the monthly feeding activity of Iporangaia individuals between August 2003 and July 2004.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.t002

The best model to describe the relationship between males' structural body size (DSL) and males' body volume (V) was the one considering that DSL affects only the mean parameter of V distribution (Table 3). Then, using this model to control for the effect of body size, the most supported model taking into account the effect of paternal care considers that caring period negatively affects both the mean and the variance of V (Table 3). This means that males caring for eggs during longer periods have more homogenous and smaller body volumes than non-caring males or males that have just started to care (Fig. 4A–B). Therefore, we showed that our first proxy of body condition (body volume controlled by structural body size) decreases and is more homogeneous among Iporangaia males as caring period increases.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Energetic costs paid by Iporangaia caring males.

Effect of caring period on the mean (A) and the variance (B) of males' body volume controlled by structural body size, i.e., dorsal scute length (DSL). Effect of parental state on the mean (C) and the variance (D) of males' weight controlled by body size, i.e., body volume (V). Filled circles and solid lines represent the predicted values for non-caring males, and open circles and dashed lines represent predicted values for caring males.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.g004

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary of the model selection statistics for the analysis that evaluated the relationship between paternal care and the mean and the variance of body condition proxies (controlled by structural body size) of Iporangaia males.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.t003

Using total body mass (TBM) as another proxy for males' body condition, we found two equally plausible models to describe its relationship with the body size of males: considering the effect of males' body volume on the mean and variance of TBM distribution, and considering the effect of V only on the mean parameter of TBM (Table 3). Therefore, we used both model structures in the second analytical step, which revealed that the best supported model fitted to the observed data takes also into account the influence of parental state on the mean and variance of TBM (Table 3). For males of the same size, caring individuals were always lighter than non-caring males (Fig. 4C), although the variance in their body mass was higher (Fig. 4D). Therefore, our second proxy for body condition (body mass controlled by body volume) also negatively responded to variation in the caring period.

Mortality Risk

The global model considering time dependence of all parameters fitted satisfactorily to the capture-recapture history data (goodness-of fit test for the JMV model: χ2 = 535.266, df = 485, P = 0.057), resulting in a ĉ-value of 1.1036. The global model including the additive effect of time and individual categories was by far the best supported by the data when compared to the models considering rainfall, temperature or seasons as time-related covariates (all had Δ QAICc>50). The summary of the step-down model selection starting from the global model is shown in Table 4. At the end, the most supported model to explain the observed capture-recapture history data considered: (a) ψ as being influenced by the additive effect between time and the state of males; (b) p as being influenced by the additive effect between time and the individual categories in each sampled month; and (c) Φ as being influenced by parental state of individuals, with estimates for females and non-caring males depending on time, and estimates for caring males constant over the sampling period (Table 4). In this sense, non-caring males and females, which share similar behaviors, also showed similar apparent survival probabilities throughout the year. However, the best supported model revealed a general pattern that does not corroborate our initial hypothesis: caring males did not show lower survival than non-caring individuals. In fact, in almost all sampling months the survival estimates for parental males were higher than or at least similar to those of non-caring individuals (Fig. 5).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Apparent survival probability estimates for Iporangaia individuals according to their parental state.

Vertical lines represent 95%CI of the monthly estimates, solid squares and dotted lines represent estimates for non-caring individuals (males and females), while solid circles and solid lines represent estimates for caring males.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.g005

thumbnail
Table 4. Summary of the step-down model selection procedure for the capture-recapture analysis that investigated the influence of time, sex, parental state, and individual category on the transition probabilities between male parental states, as well as their influence on apparent survival and recapture probabilities of Iporangaia individuals between August 2003 and July 2004.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.t004

Discussion

Our results indicate that parental behavior imposes energetic costs to Iporangaia caring males given that they feed less frequently than other individuals in the population and that their body condition deteriorates over the course of the caring period. However, the deterioration of body condition while guarding eggs does not seem to negatively affect the survival of caring males. Contrary to our initial hypothesis, survival estimates of males during the caring period were consistently higher than (or at least similar to) those obtained during the period in which they were not caring for the offspring. In the following sections, we will discuss these results in details and integrate them to explore the implications of our main findings for sexual selection and parental care theory.

Energetic Costs

Both body volume (controlled by structural body size) and body mass (controlled by body volume) of caring males decreased as the time invested in egg-guarding increased. Given that the only parental activity exhibited by Iporangaia males is egg-guarding [28], the deterioration of body condition over the course of the caring period is probably the result of reduced food intake, rather than increased metabolic expense while caring for the offspring. Indeed, our field data indicate that caring males feed much less frequently, if at all, than caring males, a result similar to that obtained for another harvestman species with exclusive paternal care, Magnispina neptunus ( = Pseudopucrolia sp.) under laboratory conditions [27].

Most arthropod species exhibiting exclusive paternal care are predators or detritivorous [18], and males have developed strategies to maintain their body condition during the caring period. For example, sea spider males can carry egg-masses and seek food during brooding [13], while caring males of the assassin bug R. tristis usually perform filial cannibalism [48]. Instead of cannibalizing eggs, a rare behavior that we have observed only twice during more than 3,000 h of field observations, Iporangaia caring males may temporarily abandon the offspring to search for food [28]. However, given that body volume and mass of caring males clearly decreases over the course of the caring period, it seems that Iporangaia's foraging behavior is not as efficient in maintaining caring males' body condition as the strategies reported for sea spiders [13] and assassin bugs [9] — probably because foraging is limited to a small area on the vegetation around the egg-batch where dead arthropods, an unpredictable food source, are likely to be scarce. Our results suggest, therefore, that the energetic costs of male care in this harvestman species are probably higher than the ones paid by the other two arthropod species in which the costs of paternal care have been measured.

Paternal care not only erodes Iporangaia males' body condition, but also results in a homogeneous body condition in caring individuals after a month of parental activities (Fig. 4B). Due to the increased attractiveness of males caring for recently laid egg-batches [49], they may copulate with several females at different moments, potentially prolonging the total caring period to more than three months [28] and, consequently, intensifying the cumulative energetic costs of caring. These costs associated with the extended period of care could explain why only a small fraction of males in the population (33.6%) were found caring for the offspring during one year of intensive sampling and why females avoid mating with males guarding egg-batches containing old eggs [49]. Poorly-fed males or males infested by larvae of phorid parasitoids [50] probably have limited endogenous energy reserves and are unable to pay the energetic costs of paternal care. Furthermore, depleted energy reserves may negatively affect the expected future quality of paternal care, measured as both the ability of males to protect the eggs against potential predators and the frequency and/or the duration of their temporary desertions to seek for food. These males, therefore, should be avoided by ovigerous females, just like has been reported for some fishes with paternal care (e.g. [51][52], but see [53]).

Survival Costs

Although parental activities negatively affect body condition of Iporangaia males, egg-guarding per se does not seem to impose survival costs upon caring males. Here, we considered that the observed differences in apparent survival probability estimates among adults do represent real mortality, although the effects of permanent emigration and mortality are still confounded, even using a robust design model approach ([30], [44], but see detailed discussion in the Supporting Information S3). Two other field studies using a mark-recapture approach showed completely distinct patterns for arthropods. For the giant water bug Abedus breviceps, there was no difference in the apparent survival probability between males in caring and non-caring states [16]. The authors argued that males in both parental states are equally exposed to predators (mainly birds) during similar time periods, such as when water bugs go to the surface to take air. For the assassin bug R. tristis, on the other hand, the apparent survival probability of caring males was lower than that of non-guarding males [9]. Apparently, suppression of escape behavior in caring males, rather than their conspicuousness on the host plant, accounts for their lower survival. Due to the paucity of empirical evidence and the controversial results found by the available studies, the effect of paternal care on the survival of caring males certainly deserves further investigation in other arthropod groups. It seems clear, however, that the so-called effect of increased visibility of parental individuals to natural enemies [54] cannot account for all the empirical results reported so far.

Studies with Namib Desert beetles [55] and milkweed beetles [56] have reported that individuals that were more active during the reproductive period (males) were more frequently captured by ambush predators than sedentary individuals (females). In Iporangaia, females and non-caring males are constantly searching for mates and/or food inside home-range areas bigger than those of caring individuals, as seems evident by measurements of individual movements (Fig. 3A). Like the abovementioned beetles, it is plausible that females and non-caring males should be under stronger predation pressure than caring males, which remain close to their egg-batches for long periods. The natural predators recorded for Neotropical harvestmen in southeastern Brazil are frogs, mammals, insects, and spiders [50]. Half of the 18 known predatory species are active hunters (all vertebrate species, one assassin bug, and one ant species), and 66.6% of them forage primarily at the ground level. Thus, they are unlikely to prey on Iporangaia, which lives exclusively on the vegetation, mostly between 50 and 250 cm from the ground (G. S. Requena unpub. data). Conversely, the predators that adopt an ambush hunting strategy are spiders (nine different species) that catch their prey on the vegetation. Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that individuals of Iporangaia are more likely to be at risk of predation by ambush predators than by active hunters. Interestingly, the only predation event we witnessed in the field was by a corinnid spider that ambushed a female on the foliage (see Fig. 9.3 in [50]).

Implications for Sexual Selection

Post-ovipositional maternal care in arthropods is a costly behavior because it reduces foraging opportunities for guarding females during long periods of care and, consequently, their lifetime fecundity (e.g. [7], [9], [57]). Given that the production of sperm and other seminal products generally requires fewer nutrients than does the production of eggs [58], care-related reductions in feeding activities are likely to be less costly for males than they are for females [1]. In species in which post-zygotic uniparental care is crucial for offspring survival, females leaving eggs under male protection are allowed to forage immediately after oviposition without sacrificing offspring survivorship. Here we demonstrated that, under field conditions, the foraging rate of Iporangaia females is also much higher than that of caring males, and similar to that of non-caring males. In this context, males willing to guard eggs may provide to females a fitness-enhancing gift of cost-free care of their offspring [17]. Under the male's perspective, reductions in the mortality risks as a result of remaining stationary, combined with the benefits of improving egg survival, may have selected originally for male parental care. Thus, contrary to current theoretical models, which assume that parental care increases male mortality [59], reductions in the mortality risks during the caring period may have played and important and previously unsuspected role favoring the evolution of paternal care.

Males exhibiting paternal care could also provide an honest signal of their quality as offspring defenders, and thus female preference for caring males could be responsible for maintaining the trait [18], [22]. Indeed, results from another field study with Iporangaia show that female choice seems to be influenced by the presence of eggs, and also by the age of the guarded offspring: caring males are preferred when they are guarding recently laid egg-batches and avoided when they are guarding old egg-batches in which nymphs have already hatched [49]. As we showed here, the longer the caring period, the worse the body condition of the male, which may negatively affect the quality of paternal care (see Energetic Costs above). Therefore, female rejection, mediated by poor male body condition and/or his low frequency of egg attendance, may prevent an indefinite increase in the number of eggs in an egg-batch. It is worth noticing, however, that the mucus coat secreted by Iporangaia females after oviposition may be viewed as a naturally-selected trait that confers protection to the eggs when starving males temporarily abandon their clutches to forage [28][29]. As a result, if the frequency of egg attendance decreases over the course of the caring period in response to the energetic costs imposed by prolonged male care, females are expected to invest more in the mucus coat when ovipositing in old egg-batches, whose males are probably food deprived. This is a testable hypothesis and Iporangaia offers the opportunity to investigate this putative conflict between sexes over the relative parental investment.

Concluding remarks

Most models about life-history theory predict that parental care could evolve only when the benefits in terms of offspring survival outweigh the costs to the parents [2]. Furthermore, classic models usually assume that a trade-off does exist between parental and mating effort (see discussion in [20]). However, recent theoretical studies propose new benefits for males resulting from egg-guarding (as increased attractiveness and paternity for caring males), and point out that paternal care does not necessarily conflict with males' mating effort [18][20], [22]. Previous results from our research group indicate that paternal care in Iporangaia has an important protective role for the offspring, significantly decreasing egg predation [29], at the same time as it increases the attractiveness of caring males [49]. The findings we report here clearly show that food intake and body condition decline during the caring period, but this energetic cost does not reduce the survival of caring males. We conclude, therefore, that paternal care in this arthropod species incurs relatively low costs in relation to great benefits for caring males. Since the male egg-guarding behavior observed in Iporangaia is a simple form of parental assistance, further investigations in arthropod species in which males heavily invest in nest defense (e.g. [27], [60]) or carry large masses of eggs attached to their own body [11][12], would contribute to a more general understanding of the relationship among the intensity of paternal investment, the costs of caring, and the strength of sexual selection.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Summary of the first step-down model selection procedure for the capture-recapture analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.s001

(DOC)

Figure S1.

Transition probabilities, recapture probabilities, and population size estimates for the harvestman Iporangaia pustulosa. (A) Transition probability estimates between male parental states. Solid diamonds represent estimates for the transition from non-caring to caring states, while filled diamonds represent estimates for the transition from caring to non-caring states. (B) Recapture probability and (C) population size estimates. Solid squares represent estimates for females; solid triangles represent estimates for non-caring males; and solid circles represent estimates for caring males. Vertical lines in all graphs represent 95%CI of the estimates in corresponding periods.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.s002

(DOC)

Figure S2.

Schematic representation of the study site.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046701.s003

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We thank T.M. Del Corso, T.M. Nazareth, C. Zatz, and R.M. Werneck for help in the fieldwork; C.Y. Mandai, P.I.K.L. Prado, A.A. Oliveira for statistical advices to implement optimization routines in R language; L.E. Costa-Schmidt, J. Gilbert, S. Kudo, R. Macías-Ordóñez, A. Manica, R. Munguía-Steyer, L.S. Rayor, T.B. Quental, P.R. Guimarães-Jr., and R.H. Willemart for helpful discussions and comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. We are especially grateful to the staff of the Intervales State Park for logistical support and to R.L. Paiva for help in the fieldwork.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: GSR EGM GM. Performed the experiments: GSR BAB GM. Analyzed the data: GSR EGM. Wrote the paper: GSR BAB EGM GM.

References

  1. 1. Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B, editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man. Chicago: Aldine Press. pp 136–179.
  2. 2. Clutton-Brock TH (1991) The evolution of parental care. Princeton: Princeton University Press. 368 p.
  3. 3. Gross MR, Sargent RC (1985) The evolution of male and female parental care in fishes. Am Zool 25: 807–822 (doi: 10.1093/icb/25.3.807).
  4. 4. Tallamy DW, Brown WP (1999) Semelparity and the evolution of maternal care in insects. Anim Behav 57: 727–730 (doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.1008).
  5. 5. Tallamy DW, Denno RF (1982) Life history trade-offs in Gargaphia solani (Hemiptera: Tingidae); the cost of reproduction. Ecology 63: 616–620 (doi: https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1936779).
  6. 6. Zink AG (2003) Quantifying the costs and benefits of parental care in female treehoppers. Behav Ecol 14: 687–693 (doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg044).
  7. 7. Buzatto BA, Requena GS, Martins EG, Machado G (2007) Effects of maternal care on the lifetime reproductive success of females in a Neotropical harvestman. J Anim Ecol 76: 937–945 (doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01273.x).
  8. 8. Kölliker M (2007) Benefits and costs of earwig (Forficula auricularia) family life. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61: 1489–1497 (doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0381-7).
  9. 9. Gilbert JDJ, Thomas LK, Manica A (2010) Quantifying the benefits and costs of parental care in assassin bugs. Ecol Entomol 35: 639–651 (doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2311.2010.01221.x).
  10. 10. Crowl TA, Alexander JE (1989) Parental care and foraging ability in male water bugs (Belostoma flumineum). Can J Zool 67: 513–515 (doi: 10.1139/z89-074).
  11. 11. Smith RL (1976) Brooding behavior of a male water bug Belostoma flumineum (Hemiptera: Belostomatidae). J Kansas Ent Soc 49: 333–343.
  12. 12. Munguía-Steyer RE, Favila ME, Macías-Ordóñez R (2008) Brood pumping modulation and the benefits of paternal care in Abedus breviceps (Hemiptera: Belostomatidae). Ethology 114: 693–700 (doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01507.x).
  13. 13. Burris ZP (2010) Costs of exclusive male parental care in the sea spider Achelia simplissima (Arthropoda: Pycnogonida). Mar Biol 158: 381–390 (doi: 10.1007/s00227-010-1566-6).
  14. 14. Crespi BJ (1988) Risks and benefits of lethal male fighting in the colonial, polygynous thrips Hoplothrips karnyi (Insecta: Thysanoptera). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22: 293–301 (doi: 10.1007/bf00299845).
  15. 15. Gilg MR, Kruse K (2003) Reproduction decrease life span in the giant waterbug (Belostoma flumineum). Am Mid Nat 149: 306–319.
  16. 16. Munguía-Steyer RE, Macías-Ordóñez R (2007) Is it risky to be a father? Survival assessment depending on sex and parental status in the waterbug Abedus breviceps (Hemiptera: Belostomatidae) using multistate modeling. Can J Zool 85: 49–55 (doi: 10.1139/Z06-196).
  17. 17. Maynard-Smith J (1977) Parental investment: a prospective analysis. Anim Behav 25: 1–9.
  18. 18. Tallamy DW (2001) Evolution of exclusive paternal care in arthropods. Ann Rev Entomol 46: 139–165 (doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.139).
  19. 19. Manica A, Johnstone R (2004) The evolution of male care with overlapping broods. Am Nat 164: 517–530 (doi: 0003-0147/16404-40280).
  20. 20. Stiver KA, Alonzo SH (2009) Parental and mating effort: is there necessarily a trade-off? Ethology 115: 1101–1126 (doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01707.x).
  21. 21. Hoelzer GA (1989) The good parent process of sexual selection. Anim Behav 38: 1067–1078 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80146-0).
  22. 22. Alonzo SH (2012) Sexual selection favours male parental care, when females can choose. Proc R Soc B 279: 1784–1790 (doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.2237).
  23. 23. Ridley M, Rechten C (1981) Female sticklebacks prefer to spawn with males whose nests contain eggs. Behaviour 76: 152–161 (doi: https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853981X00059).
  24. 24. Jamieson I (1995) Do female fish prefer to spawn in nests with eggs for reasons of mate choice copying or egg survival? Am Nat 145: 824–832 (doi: 10.1086/285770).
  25. 25. Forsgren E, Karlsson A, Kvarnemo C (1996) Female sand gobies gain direct benefits by choosing males with eggs in their nests. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39: 91–96 (doi: 10.1007/s002650050270).
  26. 26. Lindström K, St Mary CM, Pampoulie C (2006) Sexual selection for male parental care in the sand goby, Pomatoschistus minutes. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60: 46–51 (doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0138-0).
  27. 27. Nazareth TM, Machado G (2010) Mating system and exclusive postzygotic paternal care in a Neotropical harvestman (Arachnida: Opiliones). Anim Behav 79: 547–554 (doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.026).
  28. 28. Machado G, Requena GS, Buzatto BA, Osses F, Rossetto LM (2004) Five new cases of paternal care in harvestmen (Arachnida: Opiliones): implications for the evolution of male guarding in the Neotropical family Gonyleptidae. Sociobiology 44: 577–598.
  29. 29. Requena GS, Buzatto BA, Munguía-Steyer R, Machado G (2009) Efficiency of uniparental male and female care against egg predators in two closely related syntopic harvestmen. Anim Behav 78: 1169–1176 (doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.035).
  30. 30. Lebreton JD, Burnham KP, Clobert J, Anderson DR (1992) Modeling survival and testing biological hypothesis using marked animals: a unified approach with case studies. Ecol Monog 62: 67–118.
  31. 31. Machado G, Oliveira PS (1998) Reproductive biology of the neotropical harvestman (Goniosoma longipes) (Arachnida, Opiliones: Gonyleptidae): mating and oviposition behaviour, brood mortality, and parental care. J Zool Lond 246: 359–367.
  32. 32. Acosta LE, Machado G (2007) Diet and foraging. In: Pinto-da-Rocha R, Machado G, Giribet G, editors. Harvestmen: the biology of Opiliones. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. pp. 309–338.
  33. 33. Johnson JB, Omland KS (2004) Model selection in ecology and evolution. TREE 19: 101–108 (doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2003.10.013).
  34. 34. Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York: Spinger-Verlag. 514 p.
  35. 35. Bolker BM (2011) bbmle: Tools for general maximum likelihood estimation. R package version 1.0.3. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/bbmle/bbmle.pdf. Accessed 2012 September 11.
  36. 36. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B (2011) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-42. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/lme4.pdf. Accessed 2012 September 11.
  37. 37. R Development Core Team (2011) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org. Accessed 2012 September 11.
  38. 38. Moya-Laraño J, Macías-Ordóñez R, Blanckenhorn WU, Fernández-Montraveta C (2008) Analysing body condition: mass, volume or density? J Anim Ecol 77: 1099–1108 (doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01433.x).
  39. 39. Jakob EM, Marshall SD, Uetz GW (1996) Estimating fitness: a comparison of body condition indices. Oikos 77: 61–67 (doi: 10.2307/3545585).
  40. 40. Bolker BM (2008) Ecological models and data in R. Princeton: Princeton University Press. 408 p.
  41. 41. White GC, Burnham KP (1999) Program MARK: survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46: 20–138 (doi: 10.1080/00063659909477239).
  42. 42. Kendall W (2012) The ‘robust design’. In: Cooch E, White G, editors. Program: MARK – A gentle introduction (11th edition). Available: http://www.phidot.org/software/mark/docs/book/pdf/chap15.pdf. Accessed 2012 September 11.
  43. 43. Brownie C, Hines JE, Nichols JD, Pollock KH, Hestbeck JB (1993) Capture–recapture studies for multiple strata including non-Markovian transition probabilities. Biometrics 49: 1173–1187.
  44. 44. Williams BK, Nichols JD, Conroy MJ (2002) Analysis and management of animal populations. San Diego: Academic Press.
  45. 45. Lebreton JD, Pradel R (2002) Multistate recapture models: modeling incomplete individual histories. J Appl Statist 29: 353–369 (doi: 10.1080/02664760120108638).
  46. 46. Choquet R, Lebreton JD, Gimenez O, Reboulet AM, Pradel R (2009) U-CARE: Utilities for performing goodness of fit tests and manipulating CApture–REcapture data. Ecography 32: 1071–1074 (doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0587.2009.05968.x).
  47. 47. Doherty PF, White GC, Burnham KP (2010) Comparison of model building and selection strategies. J Ornithol (doi: 10.1007/s10336-010-0598-5).
  48. 48. Thomas LK, Manica A (2003) Filial cannibalism in an assassin bug. Anim Behav 66: 205–210.
  49. 49. Requena GS, Machado G (2012) Paternal care increases male attractiveness in a Neotropical arachnid (Arachnida: Opiliones). In: Requena GS. Investimento parental e papéis sexuais em opiliões com cuidado paternal exclusive. PhD dissertation, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. pp. 154–182.
  50. 50. Cokendolpher JC, Mitov PG (2007) Natural enemies. In: Pinto-da-Rocha R, Machado G, Giribet G, editors. Harvestmen: the biology of Opiliones. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. pp. 339–373.
  51. 51. Mackereth RW, Noakes DLG, Ridgway MS (1999) Size-based variation in somatic energy reserves and parental expenditure by male smallmouth bass, Micropterus dolomieu. Environ Biol Fishes 56: 263–275 (doi: 10.1023/A:1007580323816).
  52. 52. Cooke SJ, Philipp DP, Wahl DH, Weatherhead PJ (2006) Parental care energetics of six syntopic centrarchid fishes. Oecologia 148: 235–249 (doi: 10.1007/s00442-006-0375-6).
  53. 53. Hanson KC, Cooke SJ (2009) Why does size matter? A test of the benefits of female mate choice in a teleost fish based on morphological and physiological indicators of male quality. Physiol Biochem Zool 82: 617–624 (doi: 10.1086/605934).
  54. 54. Shine R (1980) ‘Costs’ of reproduction in reptiles. Oecologia 46: 92–100 (doi: 10.1007/BF00346972).
  55. 55. Polis GA, Barnes JD, Seely KK, Henschel JR, Enders MN (1998) Predation as a major cost of reproduction in Namib desert tenebrionid beetles. Ecology 79: 2560–2566 (doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(1998)079[2560:PAAMCO]2.0.CO;2).
  56. 56. McCauley DE, Lawson EC (1986) Mating reduces predation on male milkweed beetles. Am Nat 127: 112–117.
  57. 57. Elkin CM, Reid ML (2005) Low energy reserves and energy allocation decisions affect reproduction by mountain pine beetles, Dendroctonus ponderosae. Ecology 19: 102–109.
  58. 58. Parker GA, Baker RR, Smith VGF (1972) The origin and evolution of gamete dimorphism and the male-female phenomenon. J Theor Biol 36: 29–553.
  59. 59. Kokko H, Jennions MD (2008) Parental investment, sexual selection and sex ratios. J Evol Biol 21: 919–948 (doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01540.x).
  60. 60. Mora G (1990) Parental care in a neotropical harvestman, Zygopachylus albomarginis (Arachnida: Gonyleptidae). Anim Behav 39: 582–593.