Figures
Abstract
The ability to express phenotypically plastic responses to environmental cues might be adaptive in changing environments. We studied phenotypic plasticity in mating behaviour as a response to population density and adult sex ratio in a freshwater isopod (Asellus aquaticus). A. aquaticus has recently diverged into two distinct ecotypes, inhabiting different lake habitats (reed Phragmites australis and stonewort Chara tomentosa, respectively). In field surveys, we found that these habitats differ markedly in isopod population densities and adult sex ratios. These spatially and temporally demographic differences are likely to affect mating behaviour. We performed behavioural experiments using animals from both the ancestral ecotype (“reed” isopods) and from the novel ecotype (“stonewort” isopods) population. We found that neither ecotype adjusted their behaviour in response to population density. However, the reed ecotype had a higher intrinsic mating propensity across densities. In contrast to the effects of density, we found ecotype differences in plasticity in response to sex ratio. The stonewort ecotype show pronounced phenotypic plasticity in mating propensity to adult sex ratio, whereas the reed ecotype showed a more canalised behaviour with respect to this demographic factor. We suggest that the lower overall mating propensity and the phenotypic plasticity in response to sex ratio have evolved in the novel stonewort ecotype following invasion of the novel habitat. Plasticity in mating behaviour may in turn have effects on the direction and intensity of sexual selection in the stonewort habitat, which may fuel further ecotype divergence.
Citation: Karlsson K, Eroukhmanoff F, Svensson EI (2010) Phenotypic Plasticity in Response to the Social Environment: Effects of Density and Sex Ratio on Mating Behaviour Following Ecotype Divergence. PLoS ONE 5(9): e12755. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012755
Editor: Trine Bilde, Aarhus University, Denmark
Received: February 26, 2010; Accepted: August 4, 2010; Published: September 16, 2010
Copyright: © 2010 Karlsson et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: This study was funded by grants from the Swedish Research Council (VR) http://www.vr.se/ and FORMAS http://www.formas.se/ to E.I.S. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Phenotypic plasticity is the differential phenotypic expression of a genotype as a response to environmental cues [1], [2]. Phenotypic plasticity is widespread in nature [3] and the plasticity can be morphological, physiological or behavioural [2]. In heterogeneous environments, it is not likely that a single phenotype would achieve the highest fitness under all environmental conditions, and plasticity may thus be advantageous and favoured by selection [cf. 4,5]. Genotypes with more plastic traits may therefore do better when exposed to novel environmental conditions than less plastic genotypes [3], [5], and phenotypic plasticity might also reduce the effects of selection following environmental change [6]. Moreover, phenotypic plasticity has been suggested to play an important role when populations become exposed to abrupt and anthropogenic environmental changes [7], and plasticity might enhance population persistence in novel habitats and fuel evolutionary divergence [reviewed in 8]. Finally, phenotypic plasticity has also been suggested to be of importance during speciation [2], [3], both during subsequent species divergence [9] and in adaptive peak shifts during phenotypic evolution [3].
Behaviours are usually considered to be more flexible than morphological traits [2], [ but see e.g. 10]. Mating behaviour may, for example, be plastic in response to ecological and demographic factors [11]. In many animal species, plastic or flexible mating strategies exist, and plasticity in mating behaviours is usually an adaptive response to varying natural and social contexts [12]–[14]. Some recent examples of mating plasticity include sex role shift in two-spotted gobies, Gobiusculus flavescens [15], and fluctuations in female mate preferences for male collared flycatchers, Ficedula albicollis, [16]. Moreover, theoretical models have highlighted the importance of adaptive plasticity in mate choice [17].
Here, we report the results from a study on how mating behaviour responds to differences in population density and adult sex ratio in the freshwater isopod Asellus aquaticus. This crustacean species is of interest in terms of sexual selection because it exhibits precopulatory mate guarding, i.e. a male captures a female before she is receptive and carries her beneath him, in “precopula”, until she moults into sexual maturity and is ready to mate [18], [19]. Precopulatory mate guarding is a time investment strategy for males when female receptivity is limited to a short temporal window [20]. Precopulatory mate guarding has been suggested to be a trait that is subject to sexual conflict, because the optimal length of mate guarding might differ between males and females [21], [22]. Theory suggests that the initiation and length of mate guarding will be affected by demographic factors such as density and sex ratio [20], [21]. For example, when the encounter rate between the sexes is low, such as in a low density population, males might be selected for earlier investment in mate guarding [20]. Also in a male biased population, males' optimal guarding duration increases and they will attempt to form precopula earlier [21]. Density and sex ratio are important factors behind selection pressures in both sexual conflict dynamics [23], [24] and mating dynamics in general [25]–[27]. A. aquaticus therefore presents ideal opportunities to investigate the role of phenotypic plasticity with respect to these demographic factors and relate it to sexual selection. Previously, it has been found that A. aquaticus adjusts the length of mate guarding depending on adult sex ratios [28]. Here, we study differences in mating behaviours between two distinct, and recently diverged isopod ecotypes [see e.g. 29], and discuss the evolution and implications of such plasticity during ecotype divergence.
In Sweden, A. aquaticus is found in two distinct ecotypes, which have diverged in parallel in at least seven different lakes in southern Sweden [30], [31], [32]. Ecotype divergence took place after the isopods dispersed from the original habitat in the reed stands (Phragmites australis) along the shoreline, and colonised a new habitat, mainly consisting of stonewort (Chara tomentosa), in the lake centre [33]. As the new stonewort habitat was established only about 20 years ago, ecotype differentiation has occurred recently and rapidly [31], [33], [34]. Following the colonisation event, the ecotypes diverged in both morphology [32] and in behaviour [e.g. 35]. Molecular studies (mtDNA and AFLP) conducted on two ecotype pairs from two lakes suggest that these particular lakes have genetically independent stonewort populations, suggesting parallel origins of this novel ecotype [31]. Indirect inferences of selection, based on comparisons of Fst/Qst divergence [cf. 36], on these four ecotype populations have furthermore shown that differentiation between the ecotype populations is far greater than if driven by neutral processes like genetic drift alone [37] The main cause for ecotype divergence is suggested to be habitat-differences in predation regimes, either the number or types of predators, or a combination [31].
The ecotypes are known to differ in sexual behaviour, with the novel “stonewort” ecotype having lower propensity to initiate pair bonding (precopula) than the ancestral “reed” ecotype, both in terms of time taken until a precopula is formed and in the frequency of formed precopulas [31]. In the current study we address this issue further, by asking if mating propensity (measured as the frequency of formed precopulas) is affected by density and adult sex ratio (ASR), and if so, whether the ancestral (“reed”) and the novel (“stonewort”) ecotypes differ in their plastic responses. This study is performed on the ecotype populations from one lake (Lake Krankesjön) and therefore we do not address parallel evolution and general patterns of ecotype divergence. According to theory, we a priori expect mating propensity to increase in a lower population density and in a male biased population [cf. 20, 21], while we would expect mating propensity to decrease in a higher population density and in a female biased population [cf. 20,21]. This is because both low encounter rates between the sexes and an excess of males might favour an earlier male investment in mate guarding, while both a higher encounter rate and a female biased sex ratio will shorten the male optimal guarding duration and select for less male investment in precopulatory guarding [cf. 20,21]. We thus assume that an earlier investment in mate guarding will be reflected in a higher mating propensity as males will consider more females as suitable to guard. Furthermore, based on our knowledge from our previous studies on the A. aquaticus ecotypes we would expect the reed ecotype to in general have a higher mating propensity than the stonewort ecotype [31].
Here, we present field data from Lake Krankesjön on natural habitat-differences in sex ratio and population density. We demonstrate that the ancestral reed ecotype does not respond to differences in either density or sex ratio. In contrast, the novel stonewort ecotype responds by increasing its mating propensity in the male biased experimental population. Our results thus suggest that these specific ecotype populations have diverged in both mating behaviours and in phenotypic plasticity for mating propensity. We discuss the implication of changed demographic conditions on plasticity in mating behaviour.
Methods
Field work
Asellus aquaticus individuals were collected in the field from both the reed and the stonewort habitat in Lake Krankesjön (55° 42′N, 13° 28′E), southern Sweden, during spring 2007 and 2008. For both the density and the sex ratio experiments, we only used sexually active individuals in the experiments. This was achieved by using males and females that were captured in precopula during our field surveys. The isopods were transported to the lab and the copulating pairs were gently separated. The male and the female from each pair were then left to rest over night in individual boxes filled with lake water. Density and sex ratio experiment were always performed the following day, after one day of acclimatisation to the laboratory environment.
Habitat differences in population density and sex ratio
To estimate population density, we collected multiple samples consisting of a 15 *20 * 45 cm [height*width*length] box of the vegetation from each habitat. We counted all adult individuals found in each sample (single males and females, copulating pairs and pregnant females). After estimating the number of isopods, we weighed the dry habitat substrate. As density measurement we thus used the number of individuals/kg dry vegetation. Three samples from each habitat were taken at different locations during the breeding season (April-May 2007). Our data-points and units of study were thus the number of isopods in each sample (N = 6). The adult sex ratio of each habitat was estimated by counting all adult individuals (single males and females, copulating pairs and pregnant females) that were captured in five different locations, in each habitat, during the breeding season (April-May 2008). Our data-points and units of study were the number of males/all adult individuals in each sample (N = 10). As we counted all adult individuals we assessed the adult sex ratio (ASR) rather than the operational sex ratio (OSR) (for a discussion of ASR and OSR, see [27]).
Density and sex ratio experiments
To investigate how population density might influence mating propensity in the two different ecotypes, we measured the degree of pair formation in experimentally created high and low densities. In the high density treatment, we had 20 potential couples (i.e. 20 male and 20 females, 40 isopods in total), and in the low density population we had five potential couples (five isopods from each sex, ten individuals in total). The male and female isopods were placed in 15 *20 * 45 cm [height*width*length] boxes filled with lake water and small pieces (1–3 cm of length) of their origin habitat covering approximately one fifth of the bottom. These habitat pieces (and also for the sex ratio experiment, below) were added to make a more familiar surrounding for the animals, however, it was so little substrate that it did not infer with isopod movement or allowed for hiding etc. which could have affected the results. They were left for 30 minutes, whereafter the number of formed precopulatory pairs were counted and the experiment was terminated. The time span of 30 minutes should be sufficient for this type of experiment since pair formation usually occurs within 10 minutes [31]. Each density treatment was replicated three times for each habitat (N = 12). Each isopod was only used once.
Following the design of the density experiments, we compared the pair formation in a male biased (15 males, five females) and a female biased (five males, 15 females) experimental treatment. The isopods were placed in 15 *20 * 45 cm [height*width*length] boxes, filled with lake water and small pieces of their origin habitat, and left for 30 minutes. Thereafter, the number of formed pairs was counted and the experiment was ended. As in the density-experiment above, each sex ratio treatment was replicated three times for each habitat (N = 12). Each individual isopod was only used once in all these analyses, to avoid statistical dependence.
Statistical analyses
Differences in natural population density and adult sex ratio between the reed and the stonewort habitats were analysed using independent t-tests. As sex ratio cannot be normally distributed, we did this analysis with arcsine-squareroot transformed data. The transformed data was confirmed to be normally distributed with Shapiro Wilks test. In the analyses of the experimental results, we used ANOVAs to evaluate the effect of density (two treatments) and sex ratio (two treatments) on mating propensity. In each case, we compared the proportion of formed precopulas in each replicate between habitats. For the density experiment, an ANOVA was performed with density and ecotype as fixed factors. We also estimated the significance of the density * ecotype interaction effect. A similar approach was taken with the sex ratio experiment; here we used sex ratio and ecotype as fixed factors, and we also estimated the interaction effect, i.e. sex ratio * ecotype. We explored differences between the treatments with Tukey's post hoc test. Homogeneity of variances was confirmed with Levene's test and normal distribution of residuals was confirmed with Shapiro Wilks test. All analyses were performed in the software STATISTICA [38].
Results
We found highly significant differences between the reed and the stonewort ecotype populations in terms of both density and adult sex ratio (Fig. 1). The reed habitat had a mean density of 5.9±1.6 Asellus individuals/kg substrate, whereas the novel stonewort habitat had a much higher mean density of 141.7±20.3 individuals/kg habitat (t1,4 = 11.55, P = 0.0003). Thus, the population densities differ more than twenty-fold between these two habitats. The ASR was female biased in the reed habitat, with only 38% males in our surveys. In contrast, in the stonewort habitat, the sex ratio was more balanced (55% males). This suggests a marked difference in adult sex ratio between the ecotypes (t1,8 = 4.41, P = 0.002).
A. Density, measured as the number of individual isopods/kg dry substrate. B. Adult sex ratio, measured as the number of adult males per total number of individuals (males plus females) in each habitat. Error bars denote 95% confidence interval.
We found that the mating propensity was higher in the reed ecotype than in the stonewort ecotype in both the low and high density treatments (Fig. 2). These differences between the habitats in mating propensity are clearly concordant with our previous study which also revealed higher mating propensity in the reed [31]. The ecotype factor was the only significant effect in the two-way ANOVA (Table 1), with no effect of density-treatment. Therefore, population density does not seem to affect the intrinsic differences in mating propensity between these two different ecotypes.
Mating frequency does not differ between the high- and low densities in either ecotype. However, there is a significant intrinsic difference in mating frequency between the two ecotypes (Table 1). Across densities, the reed isopods show a higher mating frequency than the stonewort isopods. Error bars denote 95% confidence interval.
In the sex ratio experiment, the mating propensity was once again higher among the reed isopods than among the stonewort isopods (Fig. 3). From the two-way ANOVA, we found significant effects of all the three factors; sex ratio, ecotype and their interaction (Table 2). The reed ecotype had a mating propensity that was equally high in both the male biased and the female biased experimental treatment (Fig. 3), indicating that reed isopods are fairly canalised in terms of how mating behaviour responds to adult sex ratio. In contrast, the stonewort ecotype had a lower mating propensity in the female biased treatment. Tukey's post hoc test confirmed that it was only the female biased treatment for the stonewort ecotype which differed from the other groups (stonewort male biased treatment: P = 0.0012, reed male biased: P = 0.0007, reed female biased: P = 0.0012). Thus, adult sex ratio seems to influence mating propensity differently in the two ecotypes: only isopods belonging to the stonewort ecotype showed any evidence of mating plasticity (Fig. 3).
The stonewort ecotype, but not the reed ecotype, adjusts its mating propensity to the adult sex ratio (Ecotype * Sex Ratio treatment: F1,8 = 16.20 P = 0.0038) (Table 2). Letters denote significant different groups based on Tukey's post hoc test, the female biased treatment for the stonewort ecotype differs from all other treatments. Error bars denote 95% confidence interval.
Mating propensity for the stonewort population has previously been found to be 0.5–0.6 in a no-choice experiment [31], which is also the mating frequency seen in our density experiment (Table 1, Fig. 2). Assuming this is the natural mating propensity for the stonewort population under a balanced sex ratio, our results indicate that this ecotype adjusts mating propensity in both the male biased treatment (increasing mating propensity) and in the female biased treatment (decreasing mating propensity) (Fig. 3). Although it is only the female biased treatment which differs from the other groups (Fig. 3).
Discussion
Here we have demonstrated that in Asellus aquaticus, the adult sex ratio affects mating propensity in the novel stonewort ecotype but not in the ancestral reed ecotype. Thus, at least in Lake Krankesjön, these isopods seem to have developed a flexible and phenotypically plastic response to sex ratio following the colonisation of the novel stonewort habitat, which started only about two decades ago. Neither ecotype, however, adjusted their mating propensity to differences in adult population density.
The much higher population density in the stonewort (Fig. 1) suggests that this habitat is more favourable to A. aquaticus than the reed in Lake Krankesjön. Predation is a strong selective force in natural populations [39], [40] and differences in predation regimes are likely to have driven ecotype divergence in this system [31], [32]. The densities of invertebrate predators are higher in the reed, whereas visually hunting fish predators are more common in the stonewort [41]. It is, however, also quite possible that the habitats do not only differ in predation quality (invertebrate predators vs. fish) but also in predator quantity (high vs. low predation). The three-dimensional, net-like structure of the stonewort habitat creates a matrix that might actually provide the isopods with more protection from foraging fish [35].
Also the differences in adult sex ratio might have been caused by predation, since male isopods are more vulnerable to predation during their mate search [42]. Invertebrate predation in particular is likely to cause differences in mortality between the sexes [43]. The female-biased sex ratio in the reed habitat (Fig. 1) is thus consistent with a higher predation pressure for males in that habitat perhaps because of its higher density of invertebrate predators (mainly odonate larvae) [32]. Another explanation for these sex ratio differences between the habitats might be the possibility of Wolbachia infections, which is widespread among arthropods and also present in A. aquaticus [44]. Wolbachia bacteria have the capacity to cause feminisation in their host and are probable sex ratio distorters [44]. Thus, the demographic differences between the two populations, which affect mating behaviour and phenotypic plasticity, likely stem from different ecological features of these two distinct habitats.
Adjustment of mating behaviour to sex ratio has previously been demonstrated in e.g. guppies, Poecilia reticulata, [45], [46], in the two-spotted goby, Gobiusculus flavescens (Forsgren et al. 2004), and in the water strider Aquarius remigis [47]. Of particular interest here is the fact that different isopod species are also known to respond to sex ratio in terms of mating behaviour [28], [48], probably because sex ratio affects initiation of precopula [20]. The novel finding in this study is that the ancestral ecotype does not exhibit any phenotypic plasticity in response to adult sex ratio (Table 2, Fig. 3). Thus, this phenotypic plasticity with respect to the social and demographic environment has apparently emerged in the stonewort isopods following the colonisation of this novel habitat. Interestingly, our recent work on evolution of behavioural syndromes in this system also suggests that phenotypic plasticity in behaviour in general is more pronounced in the stonewort ecotype [35].
For adaptive phenotypic plasticity to evolve, there must be variable environments where selection favours different phenotypes, and no phenotype should be beneficial across all environments [reviewed in 5]. In several natural populations of A. aquaticus, changes in the sex ratio has been documented over the mating season [19], [49]. The adult sex ratio becomes increasingly female biased over the season, presumably because of higher male mortality during the breeding season [19], [49]. Thus, if different phenotypes are beneficial under different sex ratios, such temporal variation in sex ratio may be part of the explanation for plasticity in our stonewort population. Temporal variation in sex ratio is common also in other species, e.g. the guppy Poecilia reticulata [50]. However, our general knowledge about how sex ratios varies under natural conditions and their ecological effects are quite limited [50].
For the stonewort population, our results are certainly in line both with previous empirical studies [28], [48] and theoretical models which predict that, in males, it is more advantageous to initiate a precopula when male-male competition is intense (i.e. in a male biased sex ratio) but instead wait for a preferred female when adult sex ratio is female-biased [20], [21]. To always have a high mating propensity may be costly if it lessens the opportunity for male mate choice. Male isopods may choose females based on maturity [48] presumably because this shorten the time spend in precopula. A long precopula may be costly for both sexes as it may affect the reproductive output fecundity (Karlsson K., Eroukhmanoff F., Härdling R., & Svensson E.I., submitted). Thus, different mating phenotypes might be favoured under different environmental conditions, i.e. our adult sex ratios, which might have selected for phenotypic plasticity in male mating behaviours [cf. 5]. Moreover, the environment must produce reliable cues for the individuals to assess for plasticity to evolve [5]. In the isopod Lirceus fontinalis, males are shown to choose females based on the level of moulting hormone that they release [51]. Such moulting hormones are likely to play an important role also in the reproductive development of A. aquaticus, and could potentially be the physiological mediator of the environmental cue that affect mating propensity.
That neither ecotype responded to differences in density (Table 1, Fig. 2) might suggest that density does not fluctuate over the season. However, density is an important factor in mating dynamics [26] and density-differences might explain the differences in mating propensity between the habitats (this study: Table 1, Fig. 2, [31]). The extreme differences in adult population densities that we observed in Lake Krankesjön (Fig. 1), might also indirectly have affected the ecotype differences in plasticity with respect to adult sex ratio. The low population density in the reed, and the resulting low encounter rates between the sexes, is likely to create a strong selection pressure on males to mate with the first females they encounter [20]. Also from the female perspective it might be highly beneficial to initiate a precopula quickly, even if it may be far in advance of her sexual moult and even if a long mate guarding might lower her fecundity (Karlsson K., Eroukhmanoff F., Härdling R., & Svensson E.I., submitted). Phenotypic plasticity might actually be selected against in the reed habitat because flexible individuals might run in to the risk of never finding a mate. In contrast, in the stonewort, habitat selection to mate immediately is likely to have become relaxed, due to a population explosion following the colonisation of this novel habitat (Fig. 1).
The occurrence of eager males that are willing to initiate a pre-copula quickly has previously been interpreted as male mate choice, and has been shown to be influenced by female size, sexual maturity and adult sex ratio [18], [19], [28], [48], [52]. The differences in mating propensity under different sex ratios we have seen here might thus partly reflect flexible male mate choice. Recently, the importance of male mate choice in invertebrates has been highlighted and there has been a call for more investigations of male mate choice and its consequences in natural populations [53]. Variation in mate choice might also influence the intensity and form of sexual selection [11], [14]. Mate choice plasticity has for example been suggested to affect ornament evolution [13]. For such research questions, A. aquaticus may be a suitable study system. However, from our experimental set up alone, we cannot distinguish whether the plastic increase in mating propensity in the stonewort (Fig. 3) is due to changes in the male behaviour, the female behaviour or both. In other isopod species, female resistance affect the time it takes until the pair formation is completed [54], [55].The results on female resistance to male mate guarding attempts for A. aquaticus is inconclusive [18], [19], [54], and further studies addressing female resistance in A. aquaticus are clearly needed.
In summary, we have documented striking differences in how the two different isopod ecotypes of Lake Krankesjön respond to adult sex ratio in terms of mating propensity. Our results strongly suggest that phenotypic plasticity in response to sex ratio fluctuations has evolved after the isopods colonised the novel stonewort habitat. Lack of phenotypic plasticity in response to adult sex ratio in the reed ecotype might either indicate static sex ratio or strong selection to always express high mating propensity in this habitat. Our results are in line with recent suggestions that plasticity may be particularly important during profound environmental changes, e. g. by facilitating population persistence shortly after colonisation of novel habitats [3], [6], [7]. Colonisation of the stonewort habitat has occurred recently, and for the isopods, the new environmental conditions are likely to have exposed the stonewort invaders to intense and novel selection pressures. Phenotypic plasticity may also play an important initial role in evolutionary divergence and speciation [3], [8] and may potentially fuel continued ecotype differentiation, perhaps by influencing both the direction and the intensity of sexual selection [11], [14]. However, whether our results are general for other ecotype populations as well or specific for the Lake Krankesjön populations is still to be confirmed and may be subject for further research. Future research should also address the consequences of such mating flexibility on sexual selection dynamics as well as the potential role for sexual conflict in this isopod system.
Acknowledgments
We thank Sanna Harris, Lars Pettersson and two reviewers for constructive comments on a previous draft of this manuscript. We are grateful to Maria von Post, Sanna Harris, Sita Ollek and Thomas Bourdier for field and lab assistance.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: KK FE EIS. Performed the experiments: KK FE. Analyzed the data: KK FE EIS. Wrote the paper: KK. Discussed the results and improved the text: FE EIS.
References
- 1.
West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.. 794 p.
- 2. West-Eberhard MJ (1989) Phenotypic Plasticity and the Origins of Diversity. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 20: 249–278.
- 3. Price TD, Qvarnström A, Irwin DE (2003) The role of phenotypic plasticity in driving genetic evolution. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 270: 1433–1440.
- 4. Via S, Gomulkiewicz R, Dejong G, Scheine SM, Schlichting CD, et al. (1995) Adaptive Phenotypic Plasticity - Consensus and Controversy. Trends Ecol Evol 10: 212–217.
- 5. Ghalambor CK, Mckay JK, Carroll SP, Reznick DN (2007) Adaptive versus non-adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the potential for contemporary adaptation in new environments. Funct Ecol 21: 394–407.
- 6. Chevin L-M, Lande R (2009) When do adaptive plasticity and genetic evolution prevent extinction of a density-regulated population? Evolution.
- 7. Hendry AP, Farrugia TJ, Kinnison MT (2008) Human influences on rates of phenotypic change in wild animal populations. Mol Ecol 17: 20–29.
- 8. Agrawal AA (2001) Ecology - Phenotypic plasticity in the interactions and evolution of species. Science 294: 321–326.
- 9. Losos JB Creer DA, Glossip D, Goellner R, Hampton A, et al. (2000) Evolutionary implications of phenotypic plasticity in the hindlimb of the lizard Anolis sagrei. Evolution 54: 301–305.
- 10. Duckworth RA (2009) The role of behavior in evolution: a search for mechanism. Evol Ecol 23: 513–531.
- 11. Jennions MD, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences. Biol Rev 72: 283–327.
- 12. Svensson E, Sinervo B, Comendant T (2001) Condition, genotype-by-environment interaction, and correlational selection in lizard life-history morphs. Evolution 55: 2053–2069.
- 13. Chaine AS, Lyon BE (2008) Adaptive plasticity in female mate choice dampens sexual selection on male ornaments in the lark bunting. Science (Washington D C) 319: 459–462.
- 14. Cornwallis CK, Uller T (2010) Towards an evolutionary ecology of sexual traits. Trends Ecol Evol 25: 145–152.
- 15. Forsgren E, Amundsen T, Borg Å, Bjelvenmark J (2004) Unusually dynamic sex roles in a fish. Nature (London) 429: 551–554.
- 16. Qvarnström A, Part T, Sheldon , BC (2000) Adaptive plasticity in mate preference linked to differences in reproductive effort. Nature (London) 405: 344–347.
- 17. Alonzo SH, Sinervo B (2001) Mate choice games, context-dependent good genes, and genetic cycles in the side-blotched lizard, Uta stansburiana. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49: 176–186.
- 18. Manning JT (1975) Male Discrimination and Investment in Asellus-Aquaticus and Asellus-Meridianus Crustacea Isopoda. Behaviour 55: 1–14.
- 19. Ridley M, Thompson DJ (1979) Size and Mating in Asellus-Aquaticus Crustacea Isopoda. Z Tierpsychol 51: 380–397.
- 20. Parker GA (1974) Courtship Persistence and Female Guarding As Male Time Investment Strategies. Behaviour 48: 157–184.
- 21. Jormalainen V, Tuomi J, Yamamura N (1994) Intersexual conflict over precopula duration in mate guarding Crustacea. Behav Process 32: 265–284.
- 22. Jormalainen V (1998) Precopulatory mate guarding in crustaceans: Male competitive strategy and intersexual conflict. Quart Rev Biol 73: 275–304.
- 23. Gavrilets S (2000) Rapid evolution of reproductive barriers driven by sexual conflict. Nature (London) 403: 886–889.
- 24. Gosden TP, Svensson EI (2009) Density-Dependent Male Mating Harassment, Female Resistance, and Male Mimicry. Am Nat 173: 709–721.
- 25. Emlen ST, Oring LW (1977) Ecology Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Mating Systems. Science (Washington D C) 197: 215–223.
- 26. Kokko H, Rankin DJ (2006) Lonely hearts or sex in the city? Density-dependent effects in mating systems. Philos T Roy Soc B 361: 319–334.
- 27. Kokko H, Jennions MD (2008) Parental investment, sexual selection and sex ratios. J Evol Biol 21: 919–948.
- 28. Manning JT (1980) Sex Ratio and Optimal Male Time Investment Strategies in Asellus aquaticus (L.) and A. meridianus Racovitza. Behaviour 74: 264–273.
- 29.
Eroukhmanoff F (2009) The interplay between selection and constraints on adaptive divergence and phenotypic evolution. PhD thesis Lund: Lund University. 241 p.
- 30. Hargeby A, Stoltz J, Johansson J (2005) Locally differentiated cryptic pigmentation in the freshwater isopod Asellus aquaticus. J Evol Biol 18: 713–721.
- 31. Eroukhmanoff F Hargeby A, Nowshiravani N, Hellgren O, Bensch S, et al. (2009) Parallelism and historical contingency in ecotype divergence of a freshwater isopod. J Evol Biol 22: 1098–1110.
- 32. Eroukhmanoff F, Svensson EI (2009) Contemporary parallel diversification, antipredator adaptations and phenotypic integration in an aquatic isopod. PLoS ONE 4: e6173.
- 33. Hargeby A, Johansson J, Ahnesjö J (2004) Habitat-specific pigmentation in a freshwater isopod: Adaptive evolution over a small spatiotemporal scale. Evolution 58: 81–94.
- 34. Hargeby A, Blindow I, Andersso G (2007) Long-term patterns of shifts between clear and turbid states in Lake Krankesjön and Lake Tåkern. Ecosystems 10: 28–35.
- 35.
Harris S (2010) Behaviour under predation risk - antipredator strategies, behavioural syndromes and sex-specific responses in aquatic prey. PhD thesis Lund: Lund University. 122 p.
- 36. Whitlock MC (2008) Evolutionary inference from Q(ST). Mol Ecol 17: 1885–1896.
- 37. Eroukhmanoff F, Hargeby A, Svensson EI (2009) Rapid adaptive divergence between ecotypes of an aquatic isopod inferred from FST–QST analysis. Mol Ecol 18: 4912–4923.
- 38.
Statsoft, Inc (2004) STATISTICA [7] Statsoft, I. Tulsa.
- 39. Svensson EI, Friberg M (2007) Selective predation on wing morphology in sympatric damselflies. Am Nat 170: 101–112.
- 40. Reznick DN, Shaw FH, Rodd F, Shaw RG (1997) Evaluation of the rate of evolution in natural populations of guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Science (Washington D C) 275: 1934–1937.
- 41. Wagner BM, Hansson LA (1998) Food competition and niche separation between fish and the Red-necked Grebe Podiceps grisegena (Boddaert, 1783). Hydrobiologia 368: 75–81.
- 42. Jormalainen V, Merilaita S, Tuomi J (1995) Differential Predation on Sexes Affects Color Polymorphism of the Isopod Idotea-Baltica (Pallas). Biol J Linn Soc 55: 45–68.
- 43. Cothran RD (2004) Precopulatory mate guarding affects predation risk in two freshwater amphipod species. Anim Behav 68: 1133–1138.
- 44. Bouchon D, Rigaud T, Juchault P (1998) Evidence for widespread Wolbachia infection in isopod crustaceans: Molecular identification and host feminization. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 265: 1081–1090.
- 45. Jirotkul M (1999) Operational sex ratio influences female preference and male-male competition in guppies. Anim Behav 58: 287–294.
- 46. Evans JP, Magurran AE (1999) Male mating behaviour and sperm production under varying sperm competition risk in guppies. Anim Behav 58: 1001–1006.
- 47. Krupa JJ, Sih A (1993) Experimental Studies on Water Strider Mating Dynamics - Spatial Variation in Density and Sex-Ratio. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 33: 107–120.
- 48. Jormalainen V, Merilaita S, Tuomi J (1994) Male choice and male-male competition in Idotea baltica (Crustacea, Isopoda). Ethology 96: 46–57.
- 49. Økland KA (1978) Life History and Growth of Asellus-Aquaticus in Relation to Environment in A Eutrophic Lake in Norway. Hydrobiologia 59: 243–260.
- 50. Ramnarine IW, Pettersson LB, Becher SA, Mahabir R, Magurran AE (2004) Sex ratio dynamics and fluctuating selection pressures in natural populations of the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55: 461–468.
- 51. Sparkes TC, Keogh DP, Haskins KE (2000) Female resistance and male preference in a stream-dwelling isopod: effects of female molt characteristics. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 47: 145–155.
- 52. Jormalainen V, Tuomi J, Merilaita S (1992) Mate choice for male and female size in aquatic isopod Idotea balthica. Ann Zool Fenn 29: 161–167.
- 53. Bonduriansky R (2009) Reappraising sexual coevolution and the sex roles. PLoS Biology 7: e1000255.
- 54. Jormalainen V, Merilaita S (1995) Female resistance and duration of mate-guarding in three aquatic peracarids (Crustacea). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36: 43–48.
- 55. Jormalainen V, Merilaita S (1993) Female resistance and precopulatory guarding in the isopod Idotea baltica (Pallas). Behaviour 125: 219–231.