Figures
Abstract
Body dissatisfaction is a normative experience with the potential to impact women’s mental and physical health. It is closely related to self-objectification, where the body is viewed as an object for aesthetic evaluation rather than for its functional attributes. Self-objectification not only affects women’s body image but is associated with diminished subjective awareness of, and attention towards, internal bodily states (i.e., interoceptive sensibility). This study uses network analysis and network comparison to investigate the relationships between features of interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in women with high (N = 348) and low (N = 354) body dissatisfaction. The results firstly revealed significant differences in the connections between interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in women with high and low body dissatisfaction (p = 0.026). High body dissatisfaction in women was characterised by lower levels of body listening, self-regulation, and body trusting. Alternatively, high emotional awareness was most central to women with low body dissatisfaction. This study highlights the central role of interoceptive sensibility in body dissatisfaction and identifies key features of interoceptive sensibility to target for improving body dissatisfaction.
Citation: Naraindas AM, McInerney A, Deschênes S, Cooney SM (2025) Differences in the relationships between interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in women with high and low body dissatisfaction: A network analysis. PLoS One 20(5): e0323524. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323524
Editor: Silvana Mula,, University of Cagliari: Universita degli Studi Di Cagliari, ITALY
Received: December 2, 2024; Accepted: April 7, 2025; Published: May 28, 2025
Copyright: © 2025 Naraindas et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The data presented in this study are openly available via OSF: https://osf.io/25cjh
Funding: This project was funded by the University College Dublin Ad Astra research fellowship (ID: C776AAB4) to S.M.C. and associated Ad Astra PhD scholarship to A.M.N. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Body dissatisfaction refers to negative perceptions and attitudes about one’s external appearance [1], and is typically exemplified by shape and weight concerns [2]. While body dissatisfaction affects individuals of all ages, genders, and ethnicities, it is highly prevalent among women and is thus considered a normative discontent [3]. Affecting up to 72% of women [4], body dissatisfaction constitutes a burden at a global level, with higher levels being linked to a host of social, health (physical and mental), and economic consequences [5,6]. A recent study estimates the financial impact of body dissatisfaction between $226.4 billion and $506.5 billion in the United States alone [7]. Feminist theories suggest that body dissatisfaction and its negative outcomes stem from women’s routine encounters with objectification, which pushes them to perceive their bodies primarily as objects for external evaluation, rather than appreciating their functional capacities or internal sensations [8,9]. The ability to sense, interpret, and integrate sensations originating from within the body is known as interoception, and is vital to the maintenance of mental and physical health [10]. While current knowledge on the role of self-objectification in contributing to high levels of body dissatisfaction is considerable [11], there is a notable lack of studies investigating its relationship with interoception among women with varying degrees of body dissatisfaction, and its broader implications for health and well-being.
Interoception is the sense that allows the body to detect and respond to internal signals like hunger, thirst, body temperature, sleepiness, and emotional states such as joy, anxiety, or depression [12]. Atypical interoceptive processing contributes to increased vulnerability to psychological disorders [10], particularly in women, due to significant internal physical and hormonal changes they experience throughout development (See: Murphy et al. 2019 [13] for review). Dynamic changes in women’s reproductive physiology, such as puberty, menstruation, and pregnancy, create a variable body environment, decreasing the reliability of internal signals [13–15]. Self-objectification has also been found to influence the perception of inner body signals. Indeed, women with high body dissatisfaction consistently report increased self-objectification [11] and exhibit reduced awareness of internal bodily markers of wellbeing such as illness, fatigue, hunger, and coldness [16–19]. One explanation, the competition of cues hypothesis [20], suggests that because attention to the body is limited, that women who focus on their external appearance have less attention available for processing internal bodily signals. For example, Ainsley and Tsakiris (2013) conducted a heartbeat detection test where participants counted their perceived heartbeats during timed intervals, and these counts were compared to their actual heartbeats. They found that women with higher self-objectification had lower sensitivity to internal body signals and were less accurate at perceiving their heartbeats [21]. Therefore, self-objectification in women with high levels of body dissatisfaction might divert attention from important interoceptive signals, potentially contributing to the health and well-being consequences of high body dissatisfaction.
McKinley and Hyde (1996) describe the process of self-objectification through the ‘body conscious variables’: body surveillance and body shame [8]. Body surveillance involves monitoring the body’s appearance based on anticipated evaluations by others [8,22]. This causes body shame by prompting women to measure their bodies against unattainable internalised beauty ideals [8]. Self-objectification theory suggests that women with elevated levels of self-surveillance not only emphasize their body’s external appearance but also prioritize it over its functionality or overall health [23]. Indeed, body surveillance has been linked to restrained eating [24,25], increased cigarette smoking [26], excessive indoor tanning [27], and willingness to undergo elective cosmetic surgery [28]. Furthermore, body surveillance is strongly associated with body dissatisfaction [11,29] and alterations to how attentional resources are allocated towards bodily processes [21,30,31]. For example, a study by Van De Veer et al. (2015) showed that induced self-objectification from brief mirror exposure or viewing model advertisements impaired participants’ real-time awareness of internal signals like hunger and satiety [32]. This aligns with the competition of cues hypothesis, highlighting an antagonism between interoceptive and exteroceptive signals.
One dimension of interoception that is a key area of focus in body image research is interoceptive sensibility: the self-reported awareness of one’s internal sensations [10]. Interoceptive sensibility is commonly measured using questionnaires such as the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-2), categorising interoceptive sensibility into several distinct subscales [33]. Recently, research has demonstrated a strong negative relationship between total MAIA-2 scores and self-objectification in women across adulthood [34] and in men [35], highlighting the consistency of this relationship across diverse demographic groups. However, due to the multidimensional nature of both interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification, approaches that employ a total score do not fully elucidate the interrelationships between their features in influencing body dissatisfaction. Interestingly, investigation into the MAIA-2 subscales has revealed relationships with both negative and positive body image [36]. Todd et al. (2019) found that the Noticing, Emotional Awareness and Trusting scales significantly predicted different facets of body dissatisfaction (appearance orientation and overweight preoccupation) [36]. The study also found that the Trusting, Emotional Awareness and Noticing scales predicted features of positive body image (body appreciation and body functionality). This indicates that different dimensions of interoceptive sensibility are related to both high and low body dissatisfaction.
Recently, research has begun to view the features of self-objectification theory and interoceptive sensibility as part of the same network of interacting influence [37]. Network analysis allows us to model these individual features (e.g., the MAIA-2 individual sub-scales and self-objectification (body shame and surveillance)) as an interactive network, examining which individual features exert the most influence on the network and which are most strongly connected to other features of a different construct. Networks consists of nodes, which symbolize variables (e.g., individual features of interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification), and edges, representing the links between them [38]. Through network analysis, it is possible to identify which components exert the greatest influence within the network (i.e., centrality) and to visualise relationships among these nodes [39].
Additionally, whilst body dissatisfaction ranges from non-existent to severe [40], much of the existing research has focused on the severe end, particularly high body dissatisfaction. Examining and comparing the relationships between interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in women with both high and low body dissatisfaction, could be useful for identifying protective factors and resilience mechanisms that help mitigate these disturbances. Therefore, this exploratory study aims to identify the most central (most highly connected) features of interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in women with high and low levels of body dissatisfaction. Based on our previous research, we predict that relationships between interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification will be stronger in women with high body dissatisfaction compared to those with low body dissatisfaction, with differences in central features between the groups [34].
Method
Participants
Participants aged 18–75 were recruited online via Prolific.com as part of a larger cross-sectional study, which included a survey investigating body image and interoceptive sensibility, as well as a cognitive task (see Naraindas and Cooney, 2023 [34], for detailed methods and data collection procedures). This study includes a secondary analysis of the survey portion of the study. The survey received N = 1372 responses, with the sample comprising women aged 18–75 years who self-reported having no current eating disorder (See S1 Table for demographic information of all participants). For this secondary analysis, participants were categorized into high body dissatisfaction (N = 347) and low body dissatisfaction (N = 357) groups based on their scores on the Body Shape Questionnaire. Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee (Humanities) at University College Dublin. All participants provided written informed consent, in line with the Declaration of Helsinki, and were informed of their right to withdraw from the study at any point. Participants were compensated at a rate of £8.50 per hour for their participation.
Materials
The survey was created using Qualtrics [41]. Participants were provided with relevant information, consent forms, and debriefing materials within the survey.
Measures
Body image.
Body dissatisfaction and self-objectification were measured using validated self-report questionnaires. See Table 1. For details.
Interoceptive sensibility.
Interoceptive sensibility was assessed using the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-2) [33]. The scale comprises 37 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from ‘Never’ to ‘Always’. The scale assesses eight distinct dimensions via its respective sub-scales (See Table 2. for details).
Data analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for all measures for the full sample (N = 1372). As relationships between interoceptive sensibility and body image total scores in the full sample have been analysed elsewhere (See: Naraindas and Cooney, 2023 [34]), this study will focus on the individual components of interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in those in the high and low body dissatisfaction groups. Participants were divided into the lowest (25th percentile) and highest (75th percentile) quartiles based on their BSQ scores. Independent sample t-tests examined group differences in all questionnaire measures (see Table 3). Due to the significant difference between the groups in age (see Table 3), we examined age’s influence on correlations between variables, similar to methods in other network analysis studies addressing demographic covariates as a sensitivity analysis [44] (see S2 Table. for raw dataset correlations and S3 Table. for correlations adjusted for age). The similarity between both tables led us to conduct the subsequent analyses on the raw dataset, without adjusting for age. Statistical analyses were conducted using R v4.3.1 [45].
Network analysis
To examine the network structures of the high and low body dissatisfaction groups, cross-sectional partial correlation networks were constructed using the R packages qgraph [46], bootnet [38], and glasso [47]. A network consists of ‘nodes,’ which are the key variables of interest (in this case, the subscales of the MAIA-2 and OBCS), and ‘edges,’ which are the connections between nodes (here represented by partial correlations). A Gaussian graphical model (GGM) was estimated for each network with extended Bayesian Information Criterion (EBIC) model selection, generating networks of nodes and edges. The networks were regularized using graphical LASSO (Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator) to reduce spurious edges [47]. Network stability and edge weight accuracy were assessed using a case-dropping subset bootstrapping method (1000 iterations) with the bootnet package [38]. The correlation stability coefficient indicates the highest proportion of excluded cases while maintaining a correlation above 0.7 between the centrality indices of the full sample and subset samples, with a 95% confidence level [38]. Finally, we evaluated the significance of each node within the network using centrality metrics. We chose to present strength centrality, as it is usually more stable than other indices like closeness or betweenness [38]. Strength centrality refers to the sum of absolute edge weights directly connecting a node to others in the network; higher centrality scores indicate more connections to the node.
Network comparison
To examine the differences between high and low body dissatisfaction subgroups, the network comparison test was performed using the R package Network Comparison Test (NCT) (version 2.2.1) [48]. We assessed global strength (the total sum of absolute connections between all node pairs), structure consistency (maximum difference between matrices of all edge strengths), and specific edge weight differences between the networks. Significance was determined using two-tailed permutation tests (p < 0.05) repeated 1000 times, comparing observed statistics with the reference distribution from the permutation procedure [48].
Results
Descriptive statistics
The mean scores and standard deviations of all scales/sub-scales for the full and grouped samples are reported in Table 3. Women in the high body dissatisfaction group had significantly higher scores on the BSQ, Body Surveillance, and body shame sub-scales compared to the low body dissatisfaction group. The high body dissatisfaction group also had significantly lower scores on all MAIA-2 sub-scales compared to the low body dissatisfaction group (see Table 3 for statistics).
Network estimation
We estimated separate Gaussian Graphical Models (partial-correlation networks) for the high and low body dissatisfaction groups (see Fig 1.)
The calculated total subscale scores of each MAIA-2 sub-scale and the OBC sub-scale for all cases are used as the “nodes” in the network. The “edges’‘ connecting these nodes represent the potential associations, with partial correlations ranging from -1 to 1. Positive edges are depicted by blue lines, while negative edges are shown in red. The thickness of an edge indicates the strength of the connection, as measured by the partial correlation.
Network stability and edge weight accuracy
The stability of strength centrality estimates exceeded cut-offs to be considered interpretable for both high (CS(cor = .75) =.67) and low (CS(cor = .70) =.67) body dissatisfaction samples. For edge weight accuracy, confidence interval widths were moderate and overlapped for some edges, indicating that while not all edge weights differ, a substantial number likely do (see S1 Fig for edge weight accuracy plots for high and low body dissatisfaction groups).
Centrality indices
Centrality indices for the high and low body dissatisfaction networks are shown in Fig 2. The node highest in centrality in the high body dissatisfaction network was Listening, while in the low body dissatisfaction network it was Emotional Awareness. The least central node in both networks was Not Distracting. Fig 3. shows the bootstrapped difference tests and significant differences in strength centrality between nodes within the high and low body dissatisfaction networks. In the high body dissatisfaction network, Listening was significantly more central than the Not Distracting, Noticing, Not Worrying nodes of MAIA-2 and the Body Shame and Body Surveillance nodes of the objectified body consciousness scale. However, it did not show significantly greater centrality than Self-Regulation, Trusting, Emotional Awareness, and Attention Regulation nodes. In the low body dissatisfaction network, Emotional Awareness was significantly more central than Not Distracting, Noticing, and Trusting nodes of MAIA-2, and the Body Shame and Body Surveillance nodes of objectified body consciousness scale, but not significantly higher than Not Worrying, Attention Regulation, Self-Regulation, and Listening nodes.
Higher values indicate greater connections to the node.
Plot A shows significant differences between the strength centrality of the nodes in the high body dissatisfaction group. Plot B shows the same information for the low body dissatisfaction group. Grey squares represent nodes that did not show significant differences from each other, while black squares signify nodes that did differ significantly. White squares display the strength centrality values for each node.
Network comparison test
We observed network differences between the high and low body dissatisfaction groups. The high body dissatisfaction network was more connected (75%; 34/45 non-zero edges) than the low body dissatisfaction network (64%; 29/45 non-zero edges), indicating a denser network. The high body dissatisfaction network also had more negative relationships between interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification nodes than the low body dissatisfaction network. The NCT results showed a significant difference between the network structures (M = 0.21, p = 0.026), but global strength was comparable (S = 0.65, p = 0.10). Seven edges differed significantly between the networks. In the high body dissatisfaction group, there were stronger negative relationships between Body-Surveillance and Listening (p = 0.014), Trusting and Body Shame (p < 0.01), Not-distracting and Not-worrying (p = 0.01), and Not-worrying and Listening (p < 0.001). There were stronger positive relationships between Attention Regulation and Trusting (p = 0.024). In the low body dissatisfaction group, there were stronger negative relationships between Not-worrying and Body Shame (p = 0.01) and stronger positive relationships between Attention Regulation and Self-Regulation (p = 0.03).
Discussion
This study reveals significantly different relationships between features of interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification among women with high body dissatisfaction compared to women with low body dissatisfaction. Interestingly, the network analysis demonstrated that it was not self-objectification, but features of interoceptive sensibility that were most central to high and low body dissatisfaction.
Specifically, we found that relationships between interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification were stronger and more negatively related in the high body dissatisfaction group (i.e., a denser network) compared to the low body dissatisfaction group. This supports the research that indicates that women with higher levels of self-objectification tend to be less attuned to their bodily states [18,49,50]. According to network theory, the spreading of activation from one node to another is easier in denser networks [51]. Indeed, the high body dissatisfaction group exhibited significantly stronger negative relationships between Body surveillance and Listening, as well as between Body Shame and Trusting, compared to the low body dissatisfaction group. Additionally, low levels of Listening, Self-Regulation, and Trusting were central features of the high body dissatisfaction network. This means that these features are highly interconnected, creating a dynamic of interdependence where changes in one node are more likely to affect others connected to it [52]. Taken together, these findings align with the competition of cues hypothesis [20], indicating that due to limited attentional capacity, women who engage in self-objectification and focus strongly on their external body have fewer attentional resources to process interoceptive signals.
The finding of low Trusting being central to women with high body dissatisfaction in particular is an interesting finding. Research indicates that low body trust is strongly associated with poor mental health outcomes and shows a robust negative correlation with suicidal ideation and attempts [53,54]. It not only predicts the severity and longitudinal progression of suicidal ideation and self-harm [55] but also moderates the relationship between exercise dependence and suicidal thoughts [56]. Additionally, women with high body dissatisfaction often engage in increased dieting and restrictive eating, suggesting they may be less attuned to their internal bodily signals [57]. Furthermore, low body trust has been consistently associated with heightened concerns about shape and weight, making it the most reliable interoceptive sensibility predictor of body dissatisfaction [36,58,59]. Other network analysis studies have shown that experiencing body sensations as unsafe or untrustworthy significantly bridges eating disorder symptoms related to weight and shape dissatisfaction, suggesting that low body trust is a common feature of both body dissatisfaction and eating pathology [58].
Additionally, the stronger association between low Trusting and Body Shame in the high body dissatisfaction network may result from these individuals distancing themselves from their bodies due to the shame experienced from it. This distancing can lead to perceiving internal bodily signals as unsafe [60]. For instance, women with high body dissatisfaction may ignore hunger signals or misinterpret them as untrustworthy due to perceived associations of these signals with weight gain [58]. Therefore, enhancing body trust may be crucial for improving the health and well-being of women with high body dissatisfaction.
Alternatively, high levels of Emotional Awareness, Listening, and Self-Regulation were central features of the low body dissatisfaction network. There were also stronger negative relationships between the Not-Worrying and Body Shame subscales in the low group compared to the high group, indicating that women with lower body shame were less likely to worry and catastrophise about bodily discomfort. Indeed, research suggests that women with healthier body image are more attuned to their physical needs [61] and more likely to participate in self-care and mindfulness behaviours [62]. This could underly the high levels of body Listening and Self-Regulation that were central to the low body dissatisfaction group. Also, considering that the most central node in the low body dissatisfaction group was high emotional awareness, this suggests that better management of emotional states toward the body might be key to lower levels of body dissatisfaction. This aligns with findings from Todd et al. (2019), who found that the MAIA-2 Emotional Awareness subscale positively predicted body appreciation, indicating that improved emotional awareness regarding bodily sensations may contribute to a positive body image [36].
There were also differences in the way features of interoceptive sensibility connected to each other between the high and low body dissatisfaction groups. In the high body dissatisfaction group, the relationships in interoceptive sensibility were less consistent than in the low body dissatisfaction group. For example, whilst some features were negatively related (e.g., Not-Distracting and Not-worrying, Not-worrying and Listening), others were positively related (e.g., Attention Regulation and Trusting). In contrast, the low body dissatisfaction group and general population samples from other studies predominantly show positive relationships among the MAIA-2 subscales [33,63]. However, it is important to highlight that the Not-Distracting and Not-Worrying sub-scales of the MAIA-2 have demonstrated low reliability and internal consistency across various studies [33,36,64]. In this study, the Not-Worrying sub-scale showed the lowest reliability among all MAIA-2 scales, while the Not-Distracting sub-scale appeared to be the least central to the high and low body dissatisfaction networks. Consequently, future research may consider excluding these sub-scales, particularly in body image studies. Despite this, the current study provides preliminary evidence of altered connectivity patterns in interoceptive sensibility among women with high body dissatisfaction. Both altered interoception and body image are often considered nonspecific risk factors for negative physical and mental health outcomes in women [13,65,66]. Indeed, women report poorer body image during periods of turbulent and altering inner body experiences (e.g., menopause [67], menstruation [68] and pregnancy [15,69]. Therefore, future research should investigate how altered interoception may contribute to poor body image during changes in women’s health across the lifespan.
Conclusions and limitations
Overall, the network analysis revealed distinct connectivity patterns between features of interoceptive sensibility and self-objectification in women with high and low body dissatisfaction. However, a limitation of this study is that we only focused on one aspect of interoception, namely self-report interoceptive sensibility. Interoception is a multidimensional construct comprising interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive sensibility, and interoceptive awareness, according to the tripartite model of interoception [70]. However, these different constructs are not proxies for one another, and numerous studies have demonstrated independence between subjective self-report measures of interoception (i.e., interoceptive sensibility) and objective measurements of interoception (e.g., interoceptive accuracy) [71–73]. Therefore, future research should investigate relationships between self-objectification and other interoception dimensions to better understand these relationships within the context of body dissatisfaction.. Furthermore, the present findings were based on cross-sectional data; thus, we cannot infer that the differences noted above between the high and low body dissatisfaction groups are due to body dissatisfaction alone.
For example, given the diverse nature of the sample, cultural differences in body dissatisfaction and interoceptive sensibility may have influenced the findings. For instance, individuals from non-Western cultures demonstrate higher interoceptive sensibility [74] and may express body dissatisfaction in culturally specific ways, such as concerns with facial features [75] or skin tone [76]. Nonetheless, cross-cultural research suggests that the antagonistic relationship between body image and interoception is observed across diverse ethnic groups [37,77]. Additionally, the wide age range of the sample may have introduced age-related effects, as younger individuals often report greater appearance-based body dissatisfaction due to societal pressures [78], while older individuals may experience more dissatisfaction with their body functionality as opposed to appearance [79].
Given the complex nature of body image and interoceptive sensibility, future longitudinal studies should consider using network analysis to examine their dynamic relationships throughout the lifespan. This is particularly important during adolescence and emerging adulthood, which are critical periods for the development of eating disorders [80].
Using network analysis, this study has identified important features of interoceptive sensibility, such as diminished body listening, trust, and emotional regulation, that are central to body dissatisfaction. Future body image interventions could benefit from focusing on enhancing individuals’ ability to attune to and trust their bodily sensations, alongside improving emotional regulation. Such approaches may lead to more effective outcomes in promoting health and well-being among women with high body dissatisfaction.
Supporting information
S1 Fig. Edge weight accuracy plot for high and low body dissatisfaction group.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323524.s001
(DOCX)
S1 Table. Demographic information for all participants (N = 1372) in the study.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323524.s002
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Spearman-product correlation matrix for raw dataset.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323524.s003
(DOCX)
S3 Table. Spearman-product correlation matrix on age regressed dataset. To control for the effect of age, each variable was regressed by age and the obtained standardized residuals were correlated.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323524.s004
(DOCX)
References
- 1. Stice E, Shaw HE. Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research findings. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(5):985–93. pmid:12445588
- 2. Wade TD, Zhu G, Martin NG. Undue influence of weight and shape: is it distinct from body dissatisfaction and concern about weight and shape?. Psychol Med. 2011;41(4):819–28. pmid:20507670
- 3. Rodin J, Silberstein L, Striegel-Moore R. Women and weight: a normative discontent. Nebr Symp Motiv. 1984;32:267–307. pmid:6398857
- 4. Fiske L, Fallon EA, Blissmer B, Redding CA. Prevalence of body dissatisfaction among United States adults: review and recommendations for future research. Eat Behav. 2014;15(3):357–65. pmid:25064281
- 5. M. Bucchianeri M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Body dissatisfaction: an overlooked public health concern. Journal of Public Mental Health. 2014;13(2):64–9.
- 6. Rodgers RF, Laveway K, Campos P, de Carvalho PHB. Body image as a global mental health concern. Glob Ment Health (Camb). 2023;10:e9. pmid:36861019
- 7. Yetsenga R, Banerjee R, Streatfeild J, McGregor K, Austin SB, Lim BWX, et al. The economic and social costs of body dissatisfaction and appearance-based discrimination in the United States. Eat Disord. 2024;32(6):572–602. pmid:38520696
- 8. McKinley NM, Hyde JS. The Objectified Body Consciousness Scale. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1996;20(2):181–215.
- 9. Fredrickson BL, Roberts T-A. Objectification Theory: Toward Understanding Women’s Lived Experiences and Mental Health Risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1997;21(2):173–206.
- 10. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS, et al. Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2018;3(6):501–13.
- 11. Saunders JF, Nutter S, Waugh R, Hayden KA. Testing body-related components of objectification theory: A meta-analysis of the relations between body shame, self-objectification, and body dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2024;50:101738. pmid:38850716
- 12. Quigley KS, Kanoski S, Grill WM, Barrett LF, Tsakiris M. Functions of Interoception: From Energy Regulation to Experience of the Self. Trends Neurosci. 2021;44(1):29–38. pmid:33378654
- 13. Murphy J, Viding E, Bird G. Does atypical interoception following physical change contribute to sex differences in mental illness?. Psychol Rev. 2019;126(5):787–9. pmid:31380676
- 14. Borlimi R, Buattini M, Riboli G, Nese M, Brighetti G, Giunti D, et al. Menstrual cycle symptomatology during the COVID-19 pandemic: The role of interoceptive sensibility and psychological health. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol. 2023;14:100182. pmid:36911251
- 15. Munns LB, Preston C. The role of bodily experiences during pregnancy on mother and infant outcomes. J Neuropsychol. 2025;19 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):131–51. pmid:38764291
- 16. Daubenmier JJ. The Relationship of Yoga, Body Awareness, and Body Responsiveness to Self-Objectification and Disordered Eating. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2005;29(2):207–19.
- 17. Mercurio AE, Landry LJ. Self-objectification and Well-being: The Impact of Self-objectification on Women’s Overall Sense of Self-worth and Life Satisfaction. Sex Roles. 2008;58(7–8):458–66.
- 18. Felig RN, Jordan JA, Shepard SL, Courtney EP, Goldenberg JL, Roberts T-A. When looking “hot” means not feeling cold: Evidence that self-objectification inhibits feelings of being cold. Br J Soc Psychol. 2022;61(2):455–70. pmid:34346518
- 19. Stevenson RJ, Hill BJ, Hughes A, Wright M, Bartlett J, Saluja S, et al. Interoceptive hunger, eating attitudes and beliefs. Front Psychol. 2023;14:1148413. pmid:37151322
- 20. Pennebaker JW, Lightner JM. Competition of internal and external information in an exercise setting. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(1):165–74. pmid:7411392
- 21. Ainley V, Tsakiris M. Body conscious? Interoceptive awareness, measured by heartbeat perception, is negatively correlated with self-objectification. PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e55568. pmid:23405173
- 22.
Roberts T-A, Calogero RM, Gervais SJ. Objectification theory: Continuing contributions to feminist psychology. APA handbook of the psychology of women: History, theory, and battlegrounds. (Vol. 1). American Psychological Association. 2018;249–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000059-013
- 23. Noll SM, Fredrickson BL. A Mediational Model Linking Self-Objectification, Body Shame, and Disordered Eating. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1998;22(4):623–36.
- 24. McKinley NM. The developmental and cultural contexts of objectified body consciousness: a longitudinal analysis of two cohorts of women. Dev Psychol. 2006;42(4):679–87. pmid:16802900
- 25. Slater A, Tiggemann M. Media Exposure, Extracurricular Activities, and Appearance-Related Comments as Predictors of Female Adolescents’ Self-Objectification. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2014;39(3):375–89.
- 26. Fiissel DL, Lafreniere KD. Weight Control Motives for Cigarette Smoking: Further Consequences of the Sexual Objectification of Women?. Feminism & Psychology. 2006;16(3):327–44.
- 27. Choukas-Bradley S, Nesi J, Widman L, Noar SM. Examining the Roles of Self-Objectification and Appearance Expectations in Young Women’s Indoor Tanning Behavior. Sex Roles. 2018;80(1–2):52–62.
- 28. Vaughan‐Turnbull C, Lewis V. Body Image, Objectification, and Attitudes Toward Cosmetic Surgery. J Appl Biobehavioral Res. 2015;20(4):179–96.
- 29. Knauss C, Paxton SJ, Alsaker FD. Relationships amongst body dissatisfaction, internalisation of the media body ideal and perceived pressure from media in adolescent girls and boys. Body Image. 2007;4(4):353–60. pmid:18089281
- 30. Emanuelsen L, Drew R, Köteles F. Interoceptive sensitivity, body image dissatisfaction, and body awareness in healthy individuals. Scand J Psychol. 2015;56(2):167–74. pmid:25444023
- 31. Pollatos O, Georgiou E. Normal interoceptive accuracy in women with bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2016;240:328–32. pmid:27138826
- 32. van de Veer E, van Herpen E, van Trijp HCM. How do I look? Focusing attention on the outside body reduces responsiveness to internal signals in food intake. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2015;56:207–13.
- 33. Mehling WE, Acree M, Stewart A, Silas J, Jones A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS One. 2018;13(12):e0208034. pmid:30513087
- 34. Naraindas AM, Cooney SM. Body image disturbance, interoceptive sensibility and the body schema across female adulthood: a pre-registered study. Front Psychol. 2023;14:1285216. pmid:38098520
- 35. Naraindas AM, Moreno M, Cooney SM. Beyond Gender: Interoceptive Sensibility as a Key Predictor of Body Image Disturbances. Behav Sci (Basel). 2023;14(1):25. pmid:38247677
- 36. Todd J, Aspell JE, Barron D, Swami V. Multiple dimensions of interoceptive awareness are associated with facets of body image in British adults. Body Image. 2019;29:6–16. pmid:30771695
- 37. Zhang N, Yang H-X. A network analysis of body image concern, interoceptive sensibility, self-consciousness, and self-objectification. J Clin Psychol. 2024;80(11):2247–67. pmid:39082124
- 38. Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Fried EI. Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behav Res Methods. 2018;50(1):195–212. pmid:28342071
- 39. Hevey D. Network analysis: a brief overview and tutorial. Health Psychol Behav Med. 2018;6(1):301–28. pmid:34040834
- 40.
Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Sociocultural theory: The media and society. Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. American Psychological Association. p. 85–124. https://doi.org/10.1037/10312-003
- 41.
Qualtrics. Provo, Utah, USA; 2018. Available: https://www.qualtrics.com
- 42. Cooper PJ, Taylor MJ, Cooper Z, Fairbum CG. The development and validation of the body shape questionnaire. Int J Eat Disord. 1987;6(4):485–94.
- 43. Yang H-X, Hu H-X, Zhang Y-J, Wang Y, Lui SSY, Chan RCK. A network analysis of interoception, self-awareness, empathy, alexithymia, and autistic traits. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2022;272(2):199–209. pmid:33987711
- 44. Gaggero G, Dellantonio S, Pastore L, Sng KHL, Esposito G. Shared and unique interoceptive deficits in high alexithymia and neuroticism. PLoS One. 2022;17(8):e0273922. pmid:36044535
- 45.
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. Available: https://www.R-project.org//
- 46. Epskamp S, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D. qgraph: Network Visualizations of Relationships in Psychometric Data. J Stat Soft. 2012;48(4).
- 47. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics. 2008;9(3):432–41. pmid:18079126
- 48. van Borkulo C, Boschloo L, Borsboom D, Penninx BWJH, Waldorp LJ, Schoevers RA. Association of Symptom Network Structure With the Course of [corrected] Depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(12):1219–26. pmid:26561400
- 49. Peat CM, Muehlenkamp JJ. Self-Objectification, Disordered Eating, and Depression. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2011;35(3):441–50.
- 50. Tiggemann M, Williams E. The Role of Self-Objectification in Disordered Eating, Depressed Mood, and Sexual Functioning Among Women. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2011;36(1):66–75.
- 51. Borsboom D. A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry. 2017;16(1):5–13. pmid:28127906
- 52. Robinaugh DJ, Millner AJ, McNally RJ. Identifying highly influential nodes in the complicated grief network. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125(6):747–57. pmid:27505622
- 53. Kinkel-Ram SS, Kunstman J, Hunger JM, Smith A. Examining the relation between discrimination and suicide among Black Americans: The role of social pain minimization and decreased bodily trust. Stigma and Health. 2023;8(4):428–36.
- 54. Hielscher E, Zopf R. Interoceptive Abnormalities and Suicidality: A Systematic Review. Behav Ther. 2021;52(5):1035–54. pmid:34452660
- 55. Gioia AN, Forrest LN, Smith AR. Diminished body trust uniquely predicts suicidal ideation and nonsuicidal self-injury among people with recent self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2022;52(6):1205–16. pmid:36029117
- 56. Duffy ME, Rogers ML, Joiner TE. Body trust as a moderator of the association between exercise dependence and suicidality. Compr Psychiatry. 2018;85:30–5. pmid:29957375
- 57. Eck KM, Quick V, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Body Dissatisfaction, Eating Styles, Weight-Related Behaviors, and Health among Young Women in the United States. Nutrients. 2022;14(18):3876. pmid:36145252
- 58. Brown TA, Vanzhula IA, Reilly EE, Levinson CA, Berner LA, Krueger A, et al. Body mistrust bridges interoceptive awareness and eating disorder symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. 2020;129(5):445–56. pmid:32202809
- 59. Bijsterbosch JM, Hasenack B, van Rooijen B, Sternheim LC, Boelen PA, Dijkerman HC, et al. Intolerable feelings of uncertainty within the body: Associations between interoceptive awareness, intolerance of uncertainty, and body dissatisfaction. J Adolesc. 2023;95(8):1678–88. pmid:37655512
- 60. Holmes SC, Johnson DM. Applying Objectification Theory to the Relationship Between Sexual Victimization and Disordered Eating. The Counseling Psychologist. 2017;45(8):1091–114.
- 61. Alleva JM, Diedrichs PC, Halliwell E, Martijn C, Stuijfzand BG, Treneman-Evans G, et al. A randomised-controlled trial investigating potential underlying mechanisms of a functionality-based approach to improving women’s body image. Body Image. 2018;25:85–96. pmid:29522927
- 62. Swami V, Barron D, Todd J, Horne G, Furnham A. Nature exposure and positive body image: (Re-)examining the mediating roles of connectedness to nature and trait mindfulness. Body Image. 2020;34:201–8. pmid:32604023
- 63. Flasinski T, Dierolf AM, Rost S, Lutz APC, Voderholzer U, Koch S, et al. Altered Interoceptive Awareness in High Habitual Symptom Reporters and Patients With Somatoform Disorders. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1859. pmid:32849092
- 64. Mehling W. Differentiating attention styles and regulatory aspects of self-reported interoceptive sensibility. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1708):20160013. pmid:28080970
- 65. Ventura-Bort C, Wendt J, Weymar M. The Role of Interoceptive Sensibility and Emotional Conceptualization for the Experience of Emotions. Front Psychol. 2021;12:712418. pmid:34867591
- 66. Kilpela LS, Hooper SC, Straud CL, Marshall VB, Verzijl CL, Stewart TM, et al. The Longitudinal Associations of Body Dissatisfaction with Health and Wellness Behaviors in Midlife and Older Women. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2023;20(24):7143. pmid:38131695
- 67. McIntosh RC, Lobo JD, Reed M, Britton JC. Anterior Insula Activation During Cardiac Interoception Relates to Depressive Symptom Severity in HIV-Positive and HIV-Negative Postmenopausal Women. Psychosom Med. 2022;84(8):863–73. pmid:36162077
- 68. Teixeira ALS, Dias MRC, Damasceno VO, Lamounier JA, Gardner RM. Association between different phases of menstrual cycle and body image measures of perceived size, ideal size, and body dissatisfaction. Percept Mot Skills. 2013;117(3):892–902. pmid:24665805
- 69. Crossland A, Kirk E, Preston C. Interoceptive sensibility and body satisfaction in pregnant and non-pregnant women with and without children. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):16138. pmid:36168024
- 70. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol Psychol. 2015;104:65–74. pmid:25451381
- 71. Calì G, Ambrosini E, Picconi L, Mehling WE, Committeri G. Investigating the relationship between interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive awareness, and emotional susceptibility. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1202. pmid:26379571
- 72. Ceunen E, Van Diest I, Vlaeyen JWS. Accuracy and awareness of perception: related, yet distinct (commentary on Herbert et al., 2012). Biol Psychol. 2013;92(2):426–7. pmid:23059638
- 73. Forkmann T, Scherer A, Meessen J, Michal M, Schächinger H, Vögele C, et al. Making sense of what you sense: Disentangling interoceptive awareness, sensibility and accuracy. Int J Psychophysiol. 2016;109:71–80. pmid:27702644
- 74. Chentsova-Dutton YE, Dzokoto V. Listen to your heart: the cultural shaping of interoceptive awareness and accuracy. Emotion. 2014;14(4):666–78. pmid:24749640
- 75. Frederick DA, Kelly MC, Latner JD, Sandhu G, Tsong Y. Body image and face image in Asian American and white women: Examining associations with surveillance, construal of self, perfectionism, and sociocultural pressures. Body Image. 2016;16:113–25. pmid:26808353
- 76. Chan J, Hurst M. South Asian women in the United Kingdom: The role of skin colour dissatisfaction in acculturation experiences and body dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2022;42:413–8. pmid:35930876
- 77. Todd J, Barron D, Aspell JE, Lin Toh EK, Zahari HS, Mohd. Khatib NA, et al. Examining Relationships Between Interoceptive Sensibility and Body Image in a Non-Western Context. International Perspectives in Psychology. 2022;11(1):53–63.
- 78. Runfola CD, Von Holle A, Trace SE, Brownley KA, Hofmeier SM, Gagne DA, et al. Body dissatisfaction in women across the lifespan: results of the UNC-SELF and Gender and Body Image (GABI) studies. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2013;21(1):52–9. pmid:22949165
- 79. Baker L, Gringart E. Body image and self-esteem in older adulthood. Ageing and Society. 2009;29(6):977–95.
- 80. Klump KL. Puberty as a critical risk period for eating disorders: a review of human and animal studies. Horm Behav. 2013;64(2):399–410. pmid:23998681