Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Lasting lockdown love? Problem behaviour and pandemic and non-pandemic related risk factors influencing the owner-dog relationship in a UK cohort of dogs reaching early adulthood

  • Bree L. Merritt,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Science and Services, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts, United Kingdom

  • Dan G. O’Neill,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Pathobiology and Population Sciences, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts, United Kingdom

  • Claire L. Brand,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Science and Services, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts, United Kingdom

  • Zoe Belshaw,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation EviVet Evidence-based Veterinary Consultancy, Nottingham, United Kingdom

  • Fiona C. Dale,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Science and Services, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts, United Kingdom

  • Camilla L. Pegram,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Pathobiology and Population Sciences, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts, United Kingdom

  • Rowena M. A. Packer

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    rpacker@rvc.ac.uk

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Science and Services, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts, United Kingdom

Abstract

The owner-dog relationship is a critical component of sustained dog ownership. Increased understanding of risk factors for weak owner-dog relationships can identify owner-dog dyads at higher risk of poor welfare outcomes, including dog relinquishment and euthanasia. The internationally documented boom in puppy acquisition during the COVID-19 pandemic led to welfare concerns for this cohort of dogs, including impulsive purchasing of puppies to unsuitable homes, increased supply of puppies from poor-welfare sources, and deficits in early-life experiences for puppies. Combined, these changes were feared to lead to problem behaviours, weak owner-dog relationships and increased future relinquishment in this uniquely vulnerable generation. The Pandemic Puppies project longitudinally studied dogs bought as puppies aged < 16-weeks old during the 2020 phase of the COVID-19 pandemic by collecting owner-completed data during puppyhood and as adults. This study aimed to investigate factors associated with the owner-dog relationship in early-adulthood via a cross-sectional analysis of a subset of Pandemic Puppies data (n = 794). When dogs were 21-months old owners completed the Monash Dog-Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS), from which the Perceived Emotional Closeness (Closeness) and Perceived Costs subscales were established as reliable in this sample and were used as outcome variables in multivariable analyses to explore risk factors such as health, behaviour, and acquisition-related variables. Problem behaviours, including those related to lack of control, fear, separation, and aggression were the predominant risk factors associated with increased Perceived Costs score. The presence of most problem behaviours was not associated with reduced Closeness, suggesting a potential source of emotional conflict for owners. However, owners of dogs showing aggressive behaviours had lower Closeness scores. Puppy acquisitions explicitly motivated by the pandemic were associated with increased Perceived Costs. Support interventions targeted at owners of dogs with problem behaviours are of high importance if positive owner-dog relationships are to be maintained.

Introduction

The emotional relationship between an owner and their dog is widely recognised as a critical element of dog ownership with the potential to influence welfare on both the dog and human sides of the relationship, positively and negatively. A person’s welfare is their health and happiness [1], and similarly animal welfare has been defined as how the animal is feeling [2]. Animal welfare is often interpreted using the Five Domains Model, considering the animal’s mental state regarding their experiences of nutrition, their environment, physical health, and behavioural interactions [3]. A strong owner-dog relationship has been associated with improved physical health both for the dog [4] and owner [5, 6]. However, dog ownership has been associated with poorer owner wellbeing across a range of common circumstances including the negative physical and emotional effects of caregiving for dogs with health or behavioural problems (‘caregiver burden’) [7, 8] or disenfranchised guilt related to work-family conflict [9]. Weak owner-dog relationships are associated with a higher risk of dog relinquishment [10]. Relinquishment can harm welfare when there is reduced capacity to care for relinquished dogs in the new temporary or permanent home. Inability to provide for dogs can lead to the morally challenging decision as to whether euthanasia is chosen, either at the level of the owner who no longer feels able to care for the dog or those providing management of unowned dog populations and local authority or non-governmental sheltering organisations [11]. In addition to the impact on canine welfare, the ethically complex decision to euthanise a dog for the lack of appropriate care options, however humane the physical act, can cause moral injury to all people involved [12]. Euthanasia is also an essential welfare tool, enabling the relief of suffering, and may be withheld to prolong a human’s relationship with their dog. This is especially concerning given the strong owner-dog relationship to dogs with extreme body type [13] that may necessitate euthanasia. Thus, an increased understanding of different elements of the owner-dog relationship, and risk factors for weakening of these relationship elements can help to identify owner-dog dyads at higher risk of poor welfare outcomes.

To date, research has identified a range of dog- and owner-related factors associated with the strength of the owner-dog relationship. Owner demographics previously reported as associated with a stronger owner-dog relationship include being a younger adult; female; unmarried; having no children; living alone and being the primary care provider for the dog [1416], suggesting that time spent together and less division of owner attention is associated with a stronger relationship [16]. Owners who acquire a dog already confident of their abilities as a dog owner, such as prior ownership or professional experience, may subsequently have a stronger relationship with their dog [17].

Dog characteristics associated with a stronger owner-dog relationship include smaller size [18], purebred breed status (compared to crossbreeds) [15], and brachycephalic conformation (with strong owner-dog relationships identified in some extreme brachycephalic breeds: the Pug, French Bulldog, and English Bulldog) [13]. In contrast, weaker owner-dog relationships were observed when the only purpose for the dog was companionship compared to working, sporting or show purposes [19]. The presence of dog behaviours normally considered a problem by the owner has been associated with a weaker owner-dog relationship [18, 20]. Reward-based, rather than aversive, training techniques to address or prevent problem behaviours, can be more effective [21] and can be associated with a stronger owner-dog relationship [22, 23].

In the UK, demand for puppies increased from March 2020 when the government response to the COVID-19 pandemic mandated many people to stay at home during periods of ‘lockdown’ [2426]. Compared to owners of pre-pandemic puppies, owners of these ‘Pandemic Puppies’ purchased during the 2020 phase of the pandemic were reported to be less likely to purchase their first-choice breed of dog, had a shorter interval between deciding to acquire a puppy and bringing them home, and pay more. Pandemic Puppy purchasers were also less likely to follow recommended practice before and during their puppy purchase, with fewer purchasers collecting their puppy in the supplier’s home, or seeing the puppy interacting with the puppy’s mother [27]. They were also more likely to have risked negative long-term effects on behaviour and health by acquiring their puppy from a poor-welfare source, that is, a breeder or third party supplier who may be selling puppies without due provision for their and their parents’ physical, behavioural, and developmental needs and concealing maternal hereditable and infectious ill health [28, 29]. Furthermore, essential early-life puppy experiences such as socialisation with humans from outside of their household were thwarted by lockdown restrictions [30], with high levels of problem behaviours reported in this cohort as they reached young adulthood [31]. As this cohort of Pandemic Puppies have matured, there are concerns that the aforementioned behavioural problems, combined with the increased cost of living [32] and return to working away from home compared to when they were purchased in 2020 [33] may increase the burden of pet care for these owners. This raises concerns about the durability of owners’ relationships with their dogs, and the subsequent impact on dog welfare and relinquishment [34].

With this background, the current study aimed to identify risk factors associated with a weaker owner-dog relationship in UK dogs acquired during 2020, using the well-established Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS) to quantify this outcome [35]. Greater understanding of these risk factors can be applied to develop better processes and tools to support owners experiencing poor relationships with their dog and to advise future puppy buyers on avoiding risk factors that might endanger a successful relationship with their dog.

Methods

Data collection

Survey data were collected via two surveys as part of the Pandemic Puppies project [27, 30]. The project recruited dog owners who were at least 18 years old, resident in the UK, and had purchased and brought home a puppy of any breed or crossbreed under 16-weeks of age during 23 March 2020–31 December 2020 (‘Pandemic Puppies’), and a comparator population acquired during the same date-period in 2019 (pre-‘Pandemic Puppies’). The first survey, hosted via SurveyMonkey, was open between 10 November and 31 December 2020 and collected data on owner and dog demographics, pre-purchase and purchase motivations and practices, and early-life management and experiences of puppies. A longitudinal study aimed at respondents in the aforementioned Pandemic Puppy cohort (2020 purchases) was launched in 2022, following these puppies as they aged, with data collection timepoints in young adulthood at 21-, 24-, 27- and 36-months of age. These surveys collected data on current dog management, health, behaviour, and the owner-dog relationship. This current study explores data reported from the first survey in 2020 and the 21-month timepoint that was collected during 2022. The outcome of interest for this study, the owner-dog relationship, was measured via the MDORS [35], as described below. The survey was hosted using a version of the Vanderbilt University’s Research Electronic Data Capture platform (REDCap) provided by the Royal Veterinary College, a part of the REDCap Consortium [36, 37].

Ethical approval for the Pandemic Puppies study was granted by the Social Science Research Ethical Review Board at the Royal Veterinary College (SR2020-0259), with written informed consent supplied by all respondents. The full 2020 questionnaire has been published previously [27, 30]. Questions relevant to the current study from the 21-month survey can be found in S1 Appendix.

Survey content and data coding

Owner-dog relationship.

MDORS is a psychometric scale for the measurement of the owner-dog relationship consisting of 28 items grouped into three subscales: Dog Owner Interaction (hereafter referred to as Interaction), Perceived Emotional Closeness (hereafter referred to as Closeness), and Perceived Costs. As described previously [35], respondents were offered fixed-choice response options for each MDORS item which were allocated a value from 1 to 5 where 1 represented strongly disagree or least frequent and 5 represented strongly agree or most frequent. Item values were summed to produce a score for each subscale. Possible scores for Interaction subscale ranged from 9 to 45, where higher scores represented more shared activities between the owner and their dog. Possible scores for Closeness subscale ranged from 10 to 50, where higher scores represented an owner feeling closer to their dog. Possible scores for Perceived Costs subscale ranged from 9 to 45, where higher scores represented an owner feeling more burdened by dog ownership. Where any of the individual 28 MDORS questions were not answered, all of that respondent’s data were excluded from analysis in the current study.

Risk factor variables.

Risk factors for a weaker owner-dog relationship collected from the surveys and evaluated during risk-factor modelling are listed in Table 1. Risk factors included were:

  1. those considered risk factors for owner-dog relationship based on existing literature or plausible association;
  2. risk factors associated with being a ‘Pandemic Puppy’ (purchase-related and early-life factors found to be significantly different in prevalence in the 2020 Pandemic Puppies cohort compared to pre-‘Pandemic Puppies’) [27, 30];
  3. UK legal requirements and proposed best practices and for puppy acquisition; and
  4. risk-factors plausibly influenced by economic and public health events at the time of the surveys [25, 32, 33].
thumbnail
Table 1. Potential risk-factors for weaker owner-dog relationship.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.t001

Identified: Risk factors identified in existing literature

Law: required by law in the United Kingdom

Pandemic Puppy: Risk factor significantly different in prevalence in the 2020 Pandemic Puppies cohort compared to pre-‘Pandemic Puppies’ cohort [27, 30]

COVID-19: Risk factor plausibly influenced by economic and public health events at the time of the surveys

Pre-purchase and purchase motivations and practices

Respondents were asked in the 2020 survey about their motivations, and pre-purchase and purchase behaviours for when they bought their dog. The main reasons (more than one permitted) for dog purchase were recorded with a yes or no response to each of several motivations for purchase. Purchase motivation options indicating companionship for self, children, or other adults were used to generate the dummy variable for when the dog was acquired for companionship of others in the household, but not the respondent. Respondents were asked if their household carried out research before they purchased their puppy, with the response options: yes, no, or ‘no but I am already an experienced dog owner’.

Dog demographics

Owner reported data on their dog’s breed were used to generate derived variables on a range of demographic factors. Applying prior work on cleaning and mapping breed terms from VetCompass [44], breed terms were linked to derived variables for: 1) The Kennel Club (UK) breed groups; 2) typical adult bodyweight (kg); 3) identification as either Pug, French Bulldog, or English Bulldog due to prior interest based upon the work of Packer et al [13]; and 4) breed designation (purebred, crossbred, or designer crossbred—the latter defined as purpose-bred crosses between defined purebred progenitor breeds [45]).

Dog management, training, and health

Respondents reported in the 2020 survey whether they were the primary carer for their dog or if care was shared within or outside the household. In the 21-month survey, respondents were asked if there had been a change in who was involved in their dog’s care, including dog walkers or doggy day-care, and whether the owners left their dogs for more than four hours was reported in both surveys. In the 21-month survey, respondents indicated their current work location (in relation to home); whether any other adult in the household works away from home; whether they took their dog to their place of work; whether COVID-19 affected the household finances; and whether the respondent anticipated that changes in household circumstances would make it easier or harder to own a dog.

Attendance was reported for puppy classes at less than 16-weeks-old, and adult training classes to 21-months-old (in person, online, or no). Respondents reported training methods or aids that they used on their dog up to 21-months of age. Based on the principles of operant conditioning, each method or aid was classified was as reward-based (positive reinforcement, negative punishment) or aversive (negative reinforcement, positive punishment). Respondents were allocated a training style based on the classification of methods or aids they used as reward-only, aversive-only, rewards with one aversive method, or rewards with more than one aversive method [31].

In the 21-month survey, respondents who were registered at a veterinary practice, and had been seen by a veterinary professional since the 2020 survey, were asked if their dog had any ongoing health problems that required ongoing veterinary care, excluding routine preventative care. Respondents were asked if their dog had been neutered in 2020 and 2022, with their most recent neuter status used in this analysis.

Dog behaviour

Problem behaviours considered as risk factors were owner-reported problem behaviour, separation related behaviour, and abnormal repetitive behaviour. Questions regarding owner-reported problem behaviours and separation-related behaviours (SRBs) used in surveys of this Pandemic Puppy cohort have been previously summarised and their risk factors analysed [31]. These variables were further analysed in the current study as risk factors for MDORS subscale scores. Briefly, when dogs were 21-months old, respondents selected behaviours from a fixed-choice list that they considered problematic in their dog, with a free-text option if their specific problem behaviour was not listed. These owner-reported problem behaviours were categorised by likely emotional-contextual origin as: attention seeking, aggression, fear and/or avoidance, reaction to familiar people, reaction to other dogs, and problems related to owner control [31]. The number of specific problem behaviours within each emotional-contextual origin category was reported for each dog.

Respondents also selected behaviours displayed by their dog from a list of nine SRBs together with which of two context options the potential SRB was displayed in: while the dog was with people who were at home relaxing; or while the dog was left at home alone. SRB cases were defined as dogs with at least one reported SRB displayed when home alone [31]. Non-SRB cases were defined as dogs that displayed either none of the nine potential SRBs, or only displayed potential SRBs while the dog was with people who were relaxing within the home.

In addition, respondents reported if any of five abnormal repetitive behaviours (ARBs) were displayed by their dog: circling, shadow-chasing, fly-snapping, oral behaviours (e.g., prolonged self-directed licking or licking/sucking of other objects), or pica (see S1 Appendix for complete questionnaire wording). The number of individual ARBs displayed per dog was summarised as a predictor variable.

Data coding

All risk factors were treated as categorical variables. For questions that offered both fixed-choice and free-text responses, data from free-text responses that matched existing fixed-choice responses were manually backcoded, as described previously [27]. If no response was provided for a risk-factor question, “No answer” was included as a category for that factor. Where appropriate, categories containing fewer than ten observations were combined with adjacent categories if both categories were ordinal numerical data; or if both categories indicated uncertainty. Where merging was inappropriate, categories with few observations were retained rather than discarded to avoid reduction of sample size.

Questions that offered fixed-choice responses describing both actions (e.g., whether the owner took their dog to work) and motivations (e.g., why they did not take their dog to work) were collapsed to actions only. Information from a prior question asking whether people could not remember the price paid for their puppy, or would prefer not to say, were added as categories to the price variable.

Analysis

All three MDORS subscale score distributions were not normally distributed on inspection of histograms and subsequently reported as median, interquartile range and range. A Cronbach’s alpha statistic was calculated for each MDORS subscale to determine reliability (internal consistency) in this cohort. A minimum Cronbach’s alpha of 0.7 for each subscale was considered acceptable reliability [35, 46] to proceed to risk factor analyses.

Sample size estimation using OpenEpi [47] indicated that scores from at least n = 148 respondents were required to detect a MDORS subscale score difference of 3.00 between two groups (standard deviation 6.00) where the size of one group was three times that of the other.

Multivariable modelling

Risk factor analysis used separate backwards stepwise multivariable linear mixed modelling for each of the three MDORS subscale outcome variables (Interaction, Closeness and Perceived Costs) provided these showed acceptable reliability from the Cronbach’s alpha testing. Respondent age and gender were retained in the final models as a priori confounders, and UK region and dog birth-month were included as random effects to account for different external events affecting dog ownership in the latter half of 2020. Risk factors with liberal univariable association (p < 0.2) with the separate MDORS subscale outcomes using an F-test were carried forward to the maximum multivariable models. The distribution of standard error of MDORS scores was assumed to be normal, due to central limit theorem [48].

Pearson’s coefficient of > 0.7 and variance inflation factor > 10 were used to identify possible collinearity between risk factors. Where collinearity was identified, the variable considered the most useful explanatory variable was chosen for inclusion in the modelling [46]. At each step during model building, the risk factor with the highest partial F-test p-value of at least 0.05 was eliminated [46]. Model comparisons were conducted with Akaike Information Criterion [49, 50]. If elimination of a risk factor resulted in greater than 25% change to the coefficients of at least one remaining variable, then the risk factor was retained in the model [19]. To show the standardised mean difference of an effect, Cohens ds effect size for the sample was calculated for categories included in the model with a z-test p-value < 0.05 [51]. Microsoft Excel was used for initial data cleaning and Stata 18SE was used for statistical analysis and charts.

Results

Study sample

A total of n = 1007 respondents responded to both the original (puppyhood) and first follow up survey (21-months) of the Pandemic Puppies project. Of these, n = 985 (97.82%) respondents still owned their dog, n = 13 dogs (1.29%) had been rehomed or sold, and n = 9 (0.89%) dogs had died or been euthanised. Of respondents who still owned their dog, n = 978 (99.28%) elected to proceed with the full 21-month survey. Complete responses for all 28 Monash Dog-Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS) items were provided by n = 794 (81.19%) respondents. Incomplete MDORS responses by n = 184 (18.81%) consisted of n = 129 (13.19%) respondents who answered no items, and n = 87 (8.90%) respondents who omitted at least one item. Respondents with incomplete MDORS responses were excluded from analysis (see Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Sample size and response attrition for investigation of risk-factors for weaker owner-dog relationship measured by the Monash Dog-Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS) as reported by owners of Pandemic Puppies aged 21-months, bought as puppies under 16-weeks of age in the UK between 1 July to 31 December 2020.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.g001

Descriptive statistics

Demographics.

In the subset of 794 respondents with complete MDORS responses the majority (n = 719, 90.55%) of respondents identified as female and all age range categories and UK regions were represented. Most dogs were categorised as either purebred (n = 551, 69.40%) or designer crossbred (n = 211, 26.57%), with a minority being crossbreeds (n = 32, 4.03%). Very few dogs were one of three brachycephalic breeds previously associated with a high MDORS scores: Pug, French Bulldog or English Bulldog (total n = 13, 1.64%).

Lifestyle and behaviour.

Nearly half of respondents (n = 359, 45.21%) did not take their 21-month-old dog with them to work outside the home, and a further 43.83% (n = 348) of owners did not leave the home to work. Over half of owners used a mixed training style which included two or more aversive methods (n = 470, 59.19%). Most respondents reported at least one problem behaviour related to control (n = 649, 81.74). A full list of risk factors, the proportion of responses in each category, and univariable linear regression results for each separate outcome, is in S2 Appendix.

MDORS scores.

Median MDORS scores were towards the higher end of the range for Interaction and Closeness subscales, and the lower end of the Perceived Costs subscale (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary Monash Dog-Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS) scores reported by owners of Pandemic Puppies aged 21-months, bought as puppies under 16-weeks of age in the UK between July to December 2020 (n = 794).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.t002

Frequencies of responses to each MDORS item and histograms of subscale scores are in S3 Appendix.

Cronbach’s alpha exceeded 0.7 for the Closeness and Perceived Costs subscales, indicating high internal reliability of these scores. The Interaction subscale did not meet the threshold for reliability in this analysis (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.523) and was thus not analysed further in this study.

Multivariable model building

Four of the 49 risk factors explored did not have a liberal univariable association (p < 0.2) with either of the Closeness and Perceived Costs subscale outcomes and were therefore not considered in multivariable modelling: attendance at training classes (puppy and adult dog); owner leaving their puppy alone for more than four hours; or microchip having been provided by the breeder. The remaining variables met the liberal association criterion with either or both Closeness and Perceived Costs scores. All behaviour variables had a highly statistically significant univariable association (p < 0.003) with the Perceived Costs subscale.

Pearson pairwise correlations coefficients were < |0.5| in both models, and Variance Inflation Factors (VIF) were < 10 for Closeness. In the case of Perceived Costs, VIF were > 10 for The Kennel Club (UK) breed group and breed type designation, therefore breed group was excluded from the modelling. Final linear mixed models for both Closeness and Perceived Costs showed normality of residuals and homoscedasticity by inspection of plots (see S4 Appendix).

Adjusted R-squared for a fixed effects model of Perceived Costs scores was 0.21. Random effects for UK Region and dog birth-month improved the fit of the Costs model (likelihood ratio test p = 0.040) compared to a fixed-effects multivariable linear regression model.

Adjusted R-squared for a fixed effects Closeness model was 0.19. The random effects model was reported, however no improvement was detected from adding UK Region and dog birth-month as random effects to the Closeness model (likelihood ratio test p = 0.996).

Final multivariable model for Perceived Costs

Problem behaviours related to lack of control, fear, aggression, and separation represented many of the risk factors significantly associated with increased Perceived Costs score, after adjusting for UK Region and the dog birth-month, when more than one different problem behaviour was reported within each motivation-context of control, fear, and aggression (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Mixed multivariable linear regression model for risk factors influencing Perceived Costs to owners of 21-month-old Pandemic Puppies bought as puppies under 16-weeks of age in the UK between July to December 2020, as measured by the Perceived Costs subscale of the Monash Dog-Owner Relationship Scale (n = 794).

Blue/up arrows indicate risk factors that increase Perceived Costs reflecting a weaker owner-dog relationship, orange/down arrows display decreased Perceived Costs reflecting a stronger owner-dog relationship. Arrow width proportional to effect size. Regression coefficient (95% confidence interval) represented to the right of the arrow for each risk factor. Risk factors with an effect size with a confidence interval that includes 0 are not included. Adjusted for training method and anticipated changes to household circumstances. Random effects: UK Region and dog birth month.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.g002

Two risk factors related to motivation for purchase were associated with increased Perceived Costs scores: dogs acquired as a companion for others in the household rather than the respondent; and the COVID-19 pandemic explicitly influencing the puppy purchase (Fig 2).

Factors associated with reduced Perceived Costs score related to the provision of care by owners for their dog, and their prior experience before acquiring this dog (Fig 2). Respondents who stated they were experienced dog owners as the reason for not doing research prior to purchasing their puppy had a lower Perceived Costs score compared to those who reported that they did prior research before purchasing their puppy (Fig 2). Perceived Costs scores were lower among respondents who reported the primary caregiver for their dog had changed since the first survey compared to those reporting no change, and for respondents who took their dog with them to work (if they worked outside the home), compared to those that did not leave the home to work (Fig 2).

Training methods and anticipated changes to household circumstances in the next three months were retained in the model with a significant contribution based on the partial F-test, but no categories showed a significant effect size.

Largest effect sizes (those categories with the highest standardised magnitude of change to the outcome, Perceived Costs, compared to their base category) were observed as an increase in Perceived Costs when aggressive behaviours were reported in 3 or 4 different contexts, such as towards their owner, strangers, other dogs (Cohens ds +0.95, 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.40–1.51); and a decrease in Perceived Costs when the respondent took their dog to work outside of the home at least half of the time, compared to not leaving the home for work (Cohens ds -0.57, 95% CI: -1.00 to -0.14) (Fig 2).

Final multivariable model for Perceived Emotional Closeness

Risk factors associated with changes to Closeness scores were largely related to dog and owner demographics, dog purchase, and management of the dog, after adjusting for UK Region and dog birth-month. Aggression was the only statistically significant behavioural risk factor in the Closeness model (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Mixed multivariable linear regression model for risk factors influencing Perceived Emotional Closeness between owners of 21-month-old Pandemic Puppies bought as puppies under 16-weeks of age in the UK between July to December 2020, as measured by the Perceived Emotional Closeness subscale of the Monash Dog-Owner Relationship Scale (n = 794).

Orange/up arrows indicate risk factors that increase Closeness reflecting a stronger owner-dog relationship, blue/down arrows display decreased Closeness reflecting a weaker owner-dog relationship. Arrow width proportional to effect size. Regression coefficient (95% confidence interval) represented to the right of the arrow for each risk factor. Risk factors with an effect size with a confidence interval that includes 0 are not included. Random effects: UK Region and dog birth month.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.g003

Many demographic risk factors were associated with Closeness scores. Closeness scores were lower for men compared to women; for respondents aged 35–44 or 55–64 years old compared to those aged 25–34 years old; for dogs which weighed 10–30 kg compared to those 0–10 kg; for respondents with children in the home compared to none; and respondents working a mixture of at home and outside the home compared to working solely from home (Fig 3). Living alone compared to living with another adult was associated with higher Closeness scores, as was another other adult working outside the home compared to working at home, and change (for better or worse) in household finances compared to those prior to the COVID-19 pandemic (Fig 3).

Closeness scores were higher when the dog had been acquired as a companion for the respondent; acquired for a specific working role; acquired within six months of deciding to own a dog compared to taking longer to decide; and if the respondent had previously owned a dog as an adult (Fig 3). Closeness scores were lower when primary care for the dog was shared with another individual living separately from the respondent’s household, and higher if respondents never took their dog to work with them outside the home compared to not leaving the home for work (Fig 3). Using only one aversive-based method compared to two or more aversive-based training methods was associated with increased Closeness scores (Fig 3).

Risk factors associated with lower Closeness scores with notable effect sizes were the primary caring being shared with another individual living separately from the respondents household compared to the respondent being the primary caregiver for the dog (Cohens ds -0.77, 95% CI:-1.28—-0.25) and if 3 or 4 different types of aggressive behaviour were reported (Cohens ds -0.77, 95%CI -1.32 –-0.22) (Fig 3). Duration of acquisition decisions was also significantly associated with Closeness scores, with more rapid decisions associated with higher scores; taking less than a week between the decision to acquire a puppy and bringing them home was associated with a higher Closeness score with a large effect compared to those who took six months or longer (Cohens ds +0.62, 95% CI 0.10–1.14) (Fig 3). Although smaller effect sizes, the same direction of relationship was seen between those who took between 1 week– 1 month (Cohens ds +0.48, 95% CI 0.20–0.77) or 1–6 months (Cohens ds +0.16, 95% CI 0.01–0.30) between decision to acquire to bringing their puppy home, compared to taking six months or longer.

Discussion

Despite earlier fears of high levels of relinquishment for puppies acquired during the early part of the pandemic [24, 34], few dogs in this study had been relinquished by 21-months of age. However, measurement of the owner-dog relationship in the current study has still identified risk factors, both pandemic and non-pandemic related, that can negatively impact owner’s emotional relationship with their dog and increase the burdens of dog ownership. If not addressed, these factors could impair the welfare of dogs, reduce the wellbeing of owners, and lead to relinquishment and euthanasia of dogs in the future.

This current study highlights the association of problem canine behaviours with increased burdens of dog ownership; reveals complexity between owner’s relationships with their dogs and work commitments; and confirms that demographic risk factors represented many of the factors influencing the Closeness score—including owner age [18, 19, 38], gender [18] and owners having no children in the house [15, 16, 19].

Dog behaviour and Perceived Costs

Owner-reported problem behaviours were identified as the primary drivers of increased Perceived Costs of dog ownership, a negative element of the owner-dog relationship reflecting the ‘burden’ of caregiving. Problem behaviour has previously been reported as a risk factor for a reduced owner-dog relationship [18, 43], relinquishment [52] and euthanasia [53]. As aforementioned, although in the current study only 22 owners who responded at 21-months had either relinquished their dog or had their dog euthanised, four of the 13 relinquishments and three of the nine euthanasias were due to behavioural issues [31]. Future behavioural relinquishments and euthanasia may occur in pandemic puppies if problem behaviour is exacerbated or cannot be resolved, given that behaviour problems are most commonly presented in dogs aged 2.5–3.7 years of age [54], and there is a proportionally high risk of euthanasia for undesirable behaviour at this age [53]. Aggression towards people and other animals have been identified as the main drivers of behavioural euthanasia in previous international studies [5557], and thus particular focus on this population of dogs and their owners is needed by researchers, and also from organisations offering practical support. Longitudinal study of this population may provide insight as to whether resolution of problem behaviour can decrease the Perceived Costs of dog ownership, and thus reduce relinquishment and euthanasia on behavioural grounds, or whether persistent or worsening behavioural problems are associated with these negative outcomes.

The current study demonstrates the differential effects of problem behaviour on different facets of the owner-dog relationship: problem behaviour is shown as a major contributor to the Perceived Cost of pet ownership but has a lesser impact on the owner’s feelings of emotional closeness towards that dog, with the notable exception of aggressive behaviours in multiple contexts. Aggression has the potential to reduce many of the social activities that people find pleasurable with their dog, both inside the home and in the community, thus reducing the opportunity for shared time and development of the emotional relationship [16]. It has been suggested that lower attachment from their owner may lead to increased dog aggression [58], or alternatively the fear of harm to self, other people, and other dogs may have made owners feel insecure in their relationship with their dog and therefore reduced the closeness of the owner to their aggressive dog. A Danish study measuring MDORS [19], also found owner perceived problems with fear were associated with increased Perceived Costs but not Closeness. Aggression or being home alone were not shown to be associated with either outcome, perhaps reflecting the different cultural expectations and management of dogs.

Other studies of showing the relationship between problem behaviour and the owner-dog relationship have examined the interaction between the owner and the dog [18], or attachment [10, 43], and thus have not been able to identify the contrasting effect of behaviour on different aspects of the relationship. The low frequency of relinquishment in this study could be further explained by problem behaviour not being associated with decreased Closeness (and thus the close emotional relationship with the dogs buffers the ‘burden’ of owning them resulting in less relinquishment [10]). However, it has been shown that relinquishing owners do not necessarily have lower emotional attachment to their dogs [39] so more information on the changes to the owner-dog relationship over time in relation to risk factors and relinquishment is warranted.

That owners are emotionally close to their dogs despite problem behaviour does not mean the burden of problem behaviour is not of wider concern. Some behaviours that were reported in the current study, such as vocalisation or toileting indoors, can indicate a poor emotional state in the dog displaying them [59] rather than merely normal canine behaviour that is unwanted by the owner. Problem behaviour in dogs can negatively affect owners’ mental health [7], potentially through the mechanism of disenfranchised guilt [9], and thus even though owners may still enjoy a close relationship with their dog, it may have an impact on the owners’ mental wellbeing. Given that problem behaviours were found to be widespread in this cohort (96.7%; reported in a sister paper exploring behaviour in the Pandemic Puppy cohort at 21-months [31]), evidence-based interventions aimed at reducing problem behaviour are likely to be vital in improving and protecting dog welfare as well as reducing associated burdens of dog ownership, and thereby restoring the owner-dog relationship.

Training

Only the use of mainly reward-based training (with one aversive) compared to those who used multiple aversive training methods was associated with a stronger relationship (higher Closeness, lower Perceived Costs), and, of these, only the association with Closeness had a 95% confidence of a positive effect size. No association was detected for purely reward-based training, and no respondents used only aversive training methods. The most common aversive training method used by respondents was physically moving their dog (e.g., to move hindquarters to encourage sit, or move off furniture) [31], but additional data on the degree of force and emotional reaction of the dog was not collected. Given that reward-based training has previously been shown to be more effective [60], and associated with less problem behaviour in this sample [31], the inclusion of problem behaviour in the model has reduced statistically the association between relationship and training methods. The observation of a weaker owner-dog relationship with increasing use of aversive techniques in this current study supports current evidence promoting reward-based training [21, 22, 60]. This is an important result given the high prevalence of the use of aversive dog training techniques among owners in this [31] and other study populations [21, 60, 61].

The COVID-19 pandemic and the owner-dog relationship

Of those risk factors tested that were related to changes in dog acquisitions during the pandemic [27, 30], several were found to be associated with MDORS outcomes. COVID-19 explicitly influencing the owners’ decision to acquire a dog was associated with increased Costs: this may reflect these decisions being based on the specific lifestyle of owners during this atypical time, but that once changed, led to a higher burden of ownership. Given that approximately 40% of owners felt that the COVID-19 pandemic influenced their decision to acquire a puppy in 2020 [27], a large number of dog-owners nationally may be affected by this additional burden. It follows that public messaging and behavioural interventions, e.g., from animal charities, should focus on reaching future owners, considering the potentially 11-year average lifespan [62] of UK dogs, and to reflect on potential lifestyle changes in this 11-year period that could impair their ability to care for their dog in the medium-long term. However, efforts to encourage ‘responsible’ acquisition, where prospective owners are aware of the responsibilities that come with owning a dog and consider their ability to care for a dog for the whole of the dog’s lifespan, appear to have limited effectiveness in the UK. For example, the charity Dogs Trust’s famous slogan, ‘A Dog is for Life, not just for Christmas’ was repurposed for the pandemic as ‘A Dog is for Life, not just for Lockdown’, and yet the Pandemic Puppy phenomenon was widespread [63]. Studies report that owners’ awareness of the legal responsibilities that come with dog ownership are poor, including those related to safeguarding health and welfare. For example, Irish dog owners were no more knowledgeable than non-dog owners regarding the responsibilities of dog owners prescribed by law in Ireland [64]. Consequently, greater understanding is needed of why owners continue to impulsively acquire dogs in the face of such widespread messaging, when the owners’ future circumstances are liable to lead to challenges providing care; and ultimately even relinquishment. Restrictions to current acquisition practices (e.g., compulsory courses that new owners must attend, as already implemented in countries such as Spain since 2023 [65] and in the Canton of Zurich since 2022 [66]) could be an effective deterrent for impulsive acquisition. However, the efficacy of these rules and whether there would be political will and resources to implement them in the UK is currently unknown.

COVID-19 affecting household finances, for better or worse, was associated with increased Closeness. Both directions of change may have led to the respondent and their dog spending more time together strengthening the relationship [67], either through more time spent at home (rather than paid work) or ability to participate in shared paid-for activities.

A shorter period between deciding to acquire a puppy and bringing them home was significantly associated with increased Closeness. This could be a reflection of owners acting to prevent the pandemic thwarting a long-held desire to get a puppy, rather than impulsivity, although the pandemic influencing the decision to acquire a dog was a separate risk factor. It is possible the strong relationship between Closeness score and a short interval between decision and acquisition of a puppy may be because both are a measure of the owner’s expression of emotion, rather than rapid acquisition genuinely leading to forming a stronger relationship. However, Closeness score has been previously associated with the human personality trait “conscientiousness” [41], which is not consistent with this hypothesis.

Owner working and dog-care arrangements

Risk factors related to management of the dog (work location, taking dog to work, change in care arrangements) showed varied and superficially contradictory associations with either outcome of Closeness or Perceived Costs. For example, compared to working at home, taking the dog to work outside the home was associated with reduced Perceived Costs (stronger relationship; perhaps due to the increased efficiency of working while caring for their dog at no extra financial cost, e.g., daycare). However, working outside the home address without the dog was associated with increased Closeness (also stronger relationship; perhaps due to the relative novelty of interactions between dog and owner compared to those together all day, or reduced conflict between the demands of paid work and caring for their dog). Also, not sharing primary care was associated with increased Closeness (stronger relationship) and changing who was involved in the care of the dog since 2020 was associated with reduced Perceived Costs (also stronger relationship). The apparent contradiction could be explained by interaction between variables, whereby subpopulations of owners within this sample may have had different motivations for working location and dog management which could not be identified with this sample size. The complex relationship between owners’ feelings for their dogs, burdens of pet ownership, and commitment to work warrants further investigation, particularly as work-family conflict has been identified as a stressor in dog owners [9].

Limitations

The respondents of the Pandemic Puppies surveys were highly skewed towards women, as is common in survey research about pets [7, 8, 13, 35]. The dog’s primary caregiver was asked to complete the survey, so this may be less of a limitation and more a reflection of dog caregiving as historically women’s work [68]. Data were owner-reported; while this was essential for data regarding owners’ relationships with their dogs, measures of dog behaviour and owner actions may be less reliable due to the known unreliability of owner interpretation and reporting of dog behaviour [69]. However, this epidemiological approach facilitated collection of a large-scale dataset that practical measures would preclude.

Very few respondents owned Pugs, English Bulldogs or French Bulldogs and thus any effects of this demographic on MDORS could not be effectively explored. Data on education and socio-economic status were not collected and may influence measures of the owner-dog relationship, and thus will be explored in this cohort in future studies.

MDORS scores were not available for respondents who no longer had their dog (and reported this at 21-months), or for those that took part in the original study in puppyhood but chose not to engage in the longitudinal study and were lost to follow up. Social desirability bias may also have influenced whether and how the survey was completed; for example, owners who had rehomed their dog may have chosen not to participate further to avoid reporting their ownership status. As such, the current results may be positively biased and reflect ‘best case scenarios’ for national pandemic puppy ownership when dogs reached young adulthood.

Other factors not measured here may have resulted in higher relinquishment levels among the wider population of pandemic puppies not included in this study. It is possible that in a sample of owners with lower socio-economic status (SES) that these Perceived Costs may have resulted in higher levels of relinquishment, based on findings from previous studies [39], and possible link to less secure housing another major reason for relinquishment [52]. Measures of SES are included in future time points of this longitudinal study, in light of the current cost of living crisis in the UK.

An owner-dog relationship is a reflection of a history of interactions between owner and dog [70], and by using a survey it is only reported through one party, the owner. The Closeness scale does report some behaviour of the dog towards the owner, but Costs reports the owners attitude towards their dog. Thus in the present study attitude is biased towards the owners’ perception of the owner-dog relationship, in common with many other studies [71]. Owner attitude is a useful measure as owners control many factors relating to welfare of their dog, and validation of MDORS in the UK would benefit future analysis of attitudes to UK owners towards their dogs. A strong relationship is not sufficient to ensure good welfare, so MDORS score should not be taken as a surrogate welfare measure for the dog.

In addition, the cross-sectional nature of the current study could not investigate if the increased burden of dog ownership reflected in the Costs scores will result in future relinquishment and behavioural euthanasia; however, future longitudinal analysis of this cohort has the potential to fill this gap. Furthermore, investigation of the relationships between the owner-dog relationship, behaviour, relinquishment and euthanasia in this Pandemic Puppies cohort as they age could inform if MDORS Costs scores are reduced as behaviour improves.

Conclusions

Many of the risk factors found to decrease the owner-dog relationship in the Pandemic Puppies cohort are not unique to the COVID-19 pandemic time-period and its atypical circumstances. Instead, they largely relate to problem behaviours, which although amplified in this cohort, are highly prevalent in the wider dog population. Interventions to support these owners with evidence-based behavioural advice, utilising reward-based training could improve their relationship with their dog, and the quality of life on both sides of the dog-human relationship. Lessons can also be learned regarding the importance of well-considered puppy acquisitions, with owners whose decision to purchase a puppy was motivated specifically by the pandemic being more likely to experience weaker owner-dog relationships. Identifying effective strategies to support owners in contemplating the stability of their circumstances at the time of proposed acquisition, and to reflect on whether they are suited for long-term dog ownership may help prevent ill-considered purchasing that results in weak owner-dog relationships and potential future relinquishment.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. 21-Month survey questions relevant to study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.s001

(PDF)

S2 Appendix. Risk factor category frequency distributions and univariable associations with Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale subscales “Perceived Emotional Closeness” and “Perceived Costs”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.s002

(PDF)

S3 Appendix. Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale item frequency and subscale score distributions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.s003

(PDF)

S4 Appendix. Full mixed linear regression models for Closeness and Costs, with effect sizes and regression diagnostics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0316166.s004

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the following organisations for their support and assistance in disseminating the survey: the British Veterinary Association (BVA), Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA), The People’s Dispensary for Sick Animals (PDSA), The Blue Cross, Battersea Dogs and Cats Home, Agria Pet Insurance, Marc Abraham and the All-Parliamentary Dog Advisory Welfare Group (APDAWG), Pets at Home, Vets4Pets, Viovet, the Animal Behaviour and Training Council (ABTC), the International Partnership for Dogs (IPFD), the Brachycephalic Working Group (BWG), Cariad Hound, Pets4Homes, The Doodle Trust, and many other organisations and individuals. We are also grateful to the The Kennel Club (UK) Breed Health Coordinators and The Kennel Club (UK) Breed Health Coordinators Mentors Group for sharing the survey, and The Kennel Club (UK) Charitable Trust for supporting VetCompass (D.G.O., F.C.D., and C.L.P.).

References

  1. 1. Cambridge Dictionary. Essential British English. 2024 [cited 2024 Sep 24]. Welfare. Available from: https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/essential-british-english/welfare
  2. 2. Cobb ML, Otto CM, Fine AH. The Animal Welfare Science of Working Dogs: Current Perspectives on Recent Advances and Future Directions. Front Vet Sci [Internet]. 2021 Oct 15 [cited 2024 Apr 22];8. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fvets.2021.666898 pmid:34722690
  3. 3. Mellor DJ, Beausoleil NJ, Littlewood KE, McLean AN, McGreevy PD, Jones B, et al. The 2020 Five Domains Model: Including Human–Animal Interactions in Assessments of Animal Welfare. Anim Open Access J MDPI. 2020 Oct 14;10(10):1870. pmid:33066335
  4. 4. Rohlf VI, Bennett PC, Toukhsati S, Coleman G. Beliefs Underlying Dog Owners’ Health Care Behaviors: Results from a Large, Self-Selected, Internet Sample. Anthrozoös. 2012 Jun 1;25(2):171–85.
  5. 5. Curl AL, Bibbo J, Johnson RA. Dog Walking, the Human–Animal Bond and Older Adults’ Physical Health. The Gerontologist. 2017 Oct 1;57(5):930–9. pmid:27002004
  6. 6. Martins CF, Silva L, Soares J, Pinto GS, Abrantes C, Cardoso L, et al. Walk or be walked by the dog? The attachment role. BMC Public Health. 2024 Mar 4;24(1):684. pmid:38438977
  7. 7. Barcelos AM, Kargas N, Assheton P, Maltby J, Hall S, Mills DS. Dog owner mental health is associated with dog behavioural problems, dog care and dog-facilitated social interaction: a prospective cohort study. Sci Rep. 2023 Dec 8;13(1):21734. pmid:38066034
  8. 8. Krouzecky C, Aden J, Hametner K, Klaps A, Kovacovsky Z, Stetina BU. Fantastic Beasts and Why It Is Necessary to Understand Our Relationship—Animal Companionship under Challenging Circumstances Using the Example of Long-Covid. Animals. 2022 Jan;12(15):1892. pmid:35892542
  9. 9. Kogan LR, Bussolari C, Currin-McCulloch J, Packman W, Erdman P. Disenfranchised Guilt—Pet Owners’ Burden. Anim Open Access J MDPI. 2022 Jun 30;12(13):1690. pmid:35804588
  10. 10. Kwan JY, Bain MJ. Owner Attachment and Problem Behaviors Related to Relinquishment and Training Techniques of Dogs. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2013 Apr 1;16(2):168–83. pmid:23544756
  11. 11. Newbury S, Blinn MK, Bushby PA, Cox CB, Dinnage JD, Griffin B, et al. The Association of Shelter Veterinarians • 2010.
  12. 12. Scotney RL, McLaughlin D, Keates HL. A systematic review of the effects of euthanasia and occupational stress in personnel working with animals in animal shelters, veterinary clinics, and biomedical research facilities. 2015 Nov 15 [cited 2024 Sep 24]; Available from: https://avmajournals.avma.org/view/journals/javma/247/10/javma.247.10.1121.xml pmid:26517615
  13. 13. Packer RMA , O’Neill DG, Fletcher F, Farnworth MJ. Great expectations, inconvenient truths, and the paradoxes of the dog-owner relationship for owners of brachycephalic dogs. PLOS ONE. 2019 Jul 19;14(7):e0219918. pmid:31323057
  14. 14. Calvo P, Bowen J, Bulbena A, Tobeña A, Fatjó J. Highly Educated Men Establish Strong Emotional Links with Their Dogs: A Study with Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS) in Committed Spanish Dog Owners. PLOS ONE. 2016 Dec 29;11(12):e0168748. pmid:28033397
  15. 15. Dotson MJ, Hyatt EM. Understanding dog–human companionship. J Bus Res. 2008 May 1;61(5):457–66.
  16. 16. Marinelli L, Adamelli S, Normando S, Bono G. Quality of life of the pet dog: Influence of owner and dog’s characteristics. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007 Dec 10;108(1):143–56.
  17. 17. Bouma EMC, Vink LM, Dijkstra A. Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction of Dog Owners and Canine Behavior Problems. Anthrozoös. 2020 Sep 2;33(5):659–72.
  18. 18. Bennett PC, Rohlf VI. Owner-companion dog interactions: Relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007 Jan 1;102(1):65–84.
  19. 19. Meyer I, Forkman B. Dog and owner characteristics affecting the dog–owner relationship. J Vet Behav. 2014;9(4):143–50.
  20. 20. Herwijnen IR van, van der Borg JAM, Naguib M, Beerda B. Dog ownership satisfaction determinants in the owner-dog relationship and the dog’s behaviour. Olsson IAS, editor. PLOS ONE. 2018 Sep 20;13(9):e0204592. pmid:30235347
  21. 21. Blackwell EJ, Twells C, Seawright A, Casey RA. The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs. J Vet Behav. 2008 Sep 1;3(5):207–17.
  22. 22. Deldalle S, Gaunet F. Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–owner relationship. J Vet Behav. 2014 Mar 1;9(2):58–65.
  23. 23. Vieira de Castro AC, Barrett J, de Sousa L, Olsson IAS. Carrots versus sticks: The relationship between training methods and dog-owner attachment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2019 Oct 1;219:104831.
  24. 24. Battersea. The Impact of COVID-19 on Companion Animal Welfare [Internet]. 2020 Nov [cited 2023 Apr 8]. Available from: https://www.battersea.org.uk/what-we-do/animal-welfare-campaigning/impact-covid-19-companion-animal-welfare
  25. 25. Institute for Government. Timeline of UK government coronavirus lockdowns and restrictions [Internet]. 2022 [cited 2023 Aug 26]. Available from: https://www.instituteforgovernment.org.uk/data-visualisation/timeline-coronavirus-lockdowns
  26. 26. Siettou C. Societal interest in puppies and the Covid-19 pandemic: A google trends analysis. Prev Vet Med. 2021 Nov 1;196:105496. pmid:34555632
  27. 27. Packer RMA, Brand CL, Belshaw Z, Pegram CL, Stevens KB, O’Neill DG. Pandemic Puppies: Characterising Motivations and Behaviours of UK Owners Who Purchased Puppies during the 2020 COVID-19 Pandemic. Animals. 2021 Sep;11(9):2500. pmid:34573466
  28. 28. McMillan FD, Serpell JA, Duffy DL, Masaoud E, Dohoo IR. Differences in behavioral characteristics between dogs obtained as puppies from pet stores and those obtained from noncommercial breeders. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2013 May 15;242(10):1359–63. pmid:23634679
  29. 29. Schumaker BA, Miller MM, Grosdidier P, Cavender JL, Montgomery DL, Cornish TE, et al. Canine distemper outbreak in pet store puppies linked to a high-volume dog breeder. J Vet Diagn Invest. 2012 Nov 1;24(6):1094–8. pmid:23012378
  30. 30. Brand CL, O’Neill DG, Belshaw Z, Pegram CL, Stevens KB, Packer RMA. Pandemic Puppies: Demographic Characteristics, Health and Early Life Experiences of Puppies Acquired during the 2020 Phase of the COVID-19 Pandemic in the UK. Animals. 2022 Jan;12(5):629. pmid:35268198
  31. 31. Brand CL, O’Neill DG, Belshaw Z, Dale FC, Merritt BL, Clover KN, et al. Impacts of Puppy Early Life Experiences, Puppy-Purchasing Practices, and Owner Characteristics on Owner-Reported Problem Behaviours in a UK Pandemic Puppies Cohort at 21 Months of Age. Animals. 2024 Jan;14(2):336. pmid:38275796
  32. 32. ONS. Consumer price inflation, UK—Office for National Statistics [Internet]. 2022 [cited 2023 Apr 18]. Available from: https://www.ons.gov.uk/economy/inflationandpriceindices/bulletins/consumerpriceinflation/august2022
  33. 33. ONS. Characteristics of homeworkers, Great Britain—Office for National Statistics [Internet]. 2023 [cited 2023 Apr 13]. Available from: https://www.ons.gov.uk/employmentandlabourmarket/peopleinwork/employmentandemployeetypes/articles/characteristicsofhomeworkersgreatbritain/september2022tojanuary2023
  34. 34. RSPCA. Meet the unwanted lockdown puppies—RSPCA [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2023 Apr 8]. Available from: http://www.rspca.org.uk/-/blog-meet-the-unwanted-lockdown-puppies
  35. 35. Dwyer F, Bennett PC, Coleman GJ. Development of the Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS). Anthrozoös. 2006 Sep;19(3):243–56.
  36. 36. Harris PA, Taylor R, Minor BL, Elliott V, Fernandez M, O’Neal L, et al. The REDCap consortium: Building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inform. 2019 Jul;95:103208.
  37. 37. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009 Apr 1;42(2):377–81. pmid:18929686
  38. 38. Bouma EMC, Vink LM, Dijkstra A. Expectations Versus Reality: Long-Term Research on the Dog–Owner Relationship. Anim Open Access J MDPI. 2020 Apr 29;10(5):772. pmid:32365588
  39. 39. Dolan ED, Scotto J, Slater M, Weiss E. Risk Factors for Dog Relinquishment to a Los Angeles Municipal Animal Shelter. Animals. 2015 Dec;5(4):1311–28. pmid:26690483
  40. 40. Chira AM, Kirby K, Epperlein T, Bräuer J. Function predicts how people treat their dogs in a global sample | Scientific Reports. Sci Rep. 2023 Mar 27;13(1):4954.
  41. 41. Höglin A, Van Poucke E, Katajamaa R, Jensen P, Theodorsson E, Roth LSV. Long-term stress in dogs is related to the human–dog relationship and personality traits. Sci Rep. 2021 Apr 21;11:8612. pmid:33883667
  42. 42. Norling AY, Keeling L. Owning a Dog and Working: A Telephone Survey of Dog Owners and Employers in Sweden. Anthrozoös. 2010 Jun 1;23(2):157–71.
  43. 43. Serpell JA. Evidence for an association between pet behavior and owner attachment levels. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1996 Apr 1;47(1):49–60.
  44. 44. O’Neill DG, McMillan KM, Church DB, Brodbelt DC. Dog breeds and conformations in the UK in 2019: VetCompass canine demography and some consequent welfare implications. PloS One. 2023;18(7):e0288081. pmid:37494312
  45. 45. Burnett E, Brand CL, O’Neill DG, Pegram CL, Belshaw Z, Stevens KB, et al. How much is that doodle in the window? Exploring motivations and behaviours of UK owners acquiring designer crossbreed dogs (2019–2020). Canine Med Genet. 2022 Dec;9(1):8. pmid:35610665
  46. 46. Dohoo IR, Martin SW, Stryhn H. Veterinary Epidemiologic Research. VER, Incorporated; 2009. 865 p.
  47. 47. Dean AG, Sullivan KM, Soe MM. OpenEpi: Open Source Epidemiologic Statistics for Public Health [Internet]. 2013 [cited 2023 Apr 14]. Available from: https://www.openepi.com/Menu/OE_Menu.htm
  48. 48. Kirkwood BR, Sterne JAC. Essential medical statistics. 2nd ed. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell Science; 2003. (Essentials (Wiley-Blackwell)).
  49. 49. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol. 2009 Mar 1;24(3):127–35. pmid:19185386
  50. 50. Bono R, Alarcón R, Blanca MJ. Report Quality of Generalized Linear Mixed Models in Psychology: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2024 Jan 23];12. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.666182 pmid:33967923
  51. 51. Lakens D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2013 [cited 2023 Aug 24];4. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
  52. 52. Diesel G, Brodbelt D, Pfeiffer DU. Characteristics of Relinquished Dogs and Their Owners at 14 Rehoming Centers in the United Kingdom. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2010 Jan 1;13(1):15–30. pmid:20017043
  53. 53. Boyd C, Jarvis S, McGreevy PD, Heath S, Church DB, Brodbelt DC, et al. Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged under three years attending primary-care veterinary practices in England. Anim Welf. 2018 Aug;27(3):251–62.
  54. 54. Martínez ÁG, Santamarina Pernas G, Diéguez Casalta FcoJ, Suárez Rey ML, De la Cruz Palomino LF. Risk factors associated with behavioral problems in dogs. J Vet Behav. 2011 Jul 1;6(4):225–31.
  55. 55. Hitchcock M, Workman MK, Guthrie AP, Ruple A, Feuerbacher EN. Factors associated with behavioral euthanasia in pet dogs. Front Vet Sci. 2024 Apr 17;11:1387076. pmid:38746931
  56. 56. Yu Y, Wilson B, Masters S, van Rooy D, McGreevy PD. Mortality Resulting from Undesirable Behaviours in Dogs Aged Three Years and under Attending Primary-Care Veterinary Practices in Australia. Anim Open Access J MDPI. 2021 Feb 13;11(2):493. pmid:33668532
  57. 57. Gorodetsky E. Epidemiology of dog and cat euthanasia across Canadian prairie provinces. Can Vet J. 1997 Oct;38(10):649–52. pmid:9332753
  58. 58. Gobbo E, Zupan M. Dogs’ Sociability, Owners’ Neuroticism and Attachment Style to Pets as Predictors of Dog Aggression. Animals. 2020 Feb;10(2):315. pmid:32085391
  59. 59. de Winkel T, van der Steen S, Enders-Slegers MJ, Griffioen R, Haverbeke A, Groenewoud D, et al. Observational behaviors and emotions to assess welfare of dogs: A systematic review. J Vet Behav. 2024 Mar 1;72:1–17.
  60. 60. Hiby EF, Rooney NJ, Bradshaw JWS. Dog training methods: their use, effectiveness and interaction with behaviour and welfare. Anim Welf. 2004 Feb;13(1):63–9.
  61. 61. Woodward JL, Casey RA, Lord MS, Kinsman RH, Da Costa REP, Knowles TG, et al. Factors influencing owner-reported approaches to training dogs enrolled in the Generation Pup longitudinal study. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2021 Sep 1;242:105404.
  62. 62. Teng KT yun , Brodbelt DC, Pegram C, Church DB, O’Neill DG. Life tables of annual life expectancy and mortality for companion dogs in the United Kingdom. Sci Rep. 2022 Apr 28;12:6415. pmid:35484374
  63. 63. Quinn B. Dogs | The Guardian [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2024 Jul 12]. Available from: https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/dogs
  64. 64. Keogh L, Hanlon A, Kelly A, Devitt C, Messam L. Self-reported awareness of the legal status of eight responsibilities of dog owners in Ireland: are dog owners different from non-dog owners? Ir Vet J. 2022 Jan 5;75(1):1. pmid:34986879
  65. 65. Article 30. Ownership of dogs—Animal Welfare Law—Spain 2023 [Internet]. 2023 [cited 2024 Oct 15]. Available from: https://animalwelfarelaw.es/legislation/title-ii-ownership-and-responsible-coexistence-with-animals/chapter-ii-pets/article-30-ownership-of-dogs/
  66. 66. De Boer J. IamExpat. [cited 2024 Oct 15]. New dog law to be enforced in Zurich: What owners need to know. Available from: https://www.iamexpat.ch/lifestyle/lifestyle-news/new-dog-law-be-enforced-zurich-what-owners-need-know
  67. 67. Kurdek LA. Pet dogs as attachment figures for adult owners. J Fam Psychol. 2009;23(4):439–46. pmid:19685978
  68. 68. Quick T. Puppy Love: Domestic Science, “Women’s Work,” and Canine Care. J Br Stud. 2019 Apr;58(2):289–314.
  69. 69. Powell L, Duffy DL, Kruger KA, Watson B, Serpell JA. Relinquishing Owners Underestimate Their Dog’s Behavioral Problems: Deception or Lack of Knowledge? Front Vet Sci [Internet]. 2021 Sep 10 [cited 2024 Oct 2];8. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/veterinary-science/articles/10.3389/fvets.2021.734973/full pmid:34568478
  70. 70. Hosey G, Melfi V. Human-Animal Interactions, Relationships and Bonds: A Review and Analysis of the Literature. Int J Comp Psychol [Internet]. 2014 [cited 2024 Sep 3];27(1). Available from: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6955n8kd
  71. 71. Samet LE, Vaterlaws-Whiteside H, Harvey ND, Upjohn MM, Casey RA. Exploring and Developing the Questions Used to Measure the Human–Dog Bond: New and Existing Themes. Animals. 2022 Jan;12(7):805. pmid:35405795