Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown affect risk attitudes?—Evidence from rural Thailand

  • Hao Luo ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    hao.luo@uni-goettingen.de

    Affiliation Department of Agricultural Economics and Rural Development, Georg-August-University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany

  • Charlotte Reich,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Agricultural Economics and Rural Development, Georg-August-University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany

  • Oliver Mußhoff

    Roles Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Agricultural Economics and Rural Development, Georg-August-University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany

Abstract

Empirical research provides evidence on changes in individuals’ risk attitudes after experiencing exogenous shocks. The global outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic has had various adverse impacts on economies and households. This study utilizes the COVID-19 pandemic and the accompanying lockdown to explore its impact on risk attitudes in rural Thailand using a difference-in-difference (DiD) approach. Overall, we do not find evidence on considerable changes in the willingness to take risks of rural household members after experiencing a lockdown during the pandemic. However, a significant heterogenous effect is found between individuals working inside and outside the agricultural sector. Individuals working outside the agricultural sector have a statistically significant reduction in their willingness to take risks after experiencing a lockdown. Our study provides additional empirical evidence to understand the impact of shocks on rural households’ risk attitudes. This sheds light on how policy designs can better help mitigate downward economic trends following exogenous shocks.

1. Introduction

Risk attitudes are a key component of individual decision-making and macroeconomic outcomes. A large number of studies show that risk attitudes have remarkable predictive power for individual portfolio selection, insurance demand, agricultural productivity, occupational choices, migration decisions, and addictive behaviors [16]. At the macroeconomic level, risk aversion is strongly correlated with per capita GDP, national income, and income inequality [79]. In the context of a global pandemic, risk attitudes are shown to considerably affect compliance behavior regarding public health and social measures, including individuals’ decisions to vaccinate [1012].

In standard economics, risk preferences are assumed to be stable across individuals and over time [13]. However, a growing body of empirical studies provides evidence on the temporal variability of risk attitudes and shows that exogenous negative shocks such as natural disasters, social conflicts, or economic recessions can cause a statistically significant change in individuals’ risk attitudes [14, 15] and thus influence economic decision-making. The COVID-19 pandemic constitutes one such extreme event, with potentially substantial impacts on risk attitudes. For example, if risk aversion increases after exposure to the pandemic, individuals may be more likely to comply with preventative measures but may also reduce their investments, which can slow economic recovery. Assessing the temporal stability of risk attitudes can shed additional light on post-pandemic recovery plans. Robust recovery strategies are vital as the global outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic has hit many countries hard and has placed additional health and financial burdens on households worldwide.

Thailand was the first country outside of China to confirm COVID-19 cases, with the first case reported on January 13, 2020 [16]. Following a peak of confirmed cases on March 22, 2020 [17], the Thai government announced a national emergency, including a national lockdown for more than three months, from the end of March 2020 to the end of June 2020 [18, 19]. These government policies were strict, as reflected by a Stringency Index value of approximately 75 [20]. This index ranges from 0 to 100, with 100 denoting the strictest government responses. In comparison, the index value was around 80 in China during the same period [20]. While a broad array of public health intervention strategies was successful in mitigating coronavirus transmission in the first wave [21, 22], the strict lockdown policies had adversely impacted the economy and household livelihoods [23, 24], leading to a sharp decline in annual GDP growth from 2.3% in 2019 to -6.1% in 2020 [25]. Especially, lockdown stringency and duration are shown to be associated with mental health issues and psychological disorder [26, 27].

Recent literature provides contradictory results on the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on risk attitudes. While Angrisani et al. [28], Drichoutis and Nayga [29], and Lohmann et al. [30] do not find considerable changes in risk attitudes due to the pandemic; Bu et al. [31], Graeber et al. [32], and Mussio, Sosa Andrés and Kidwai [33] demonstrate that the pandemic has a statistically significant effect on individuals’ risk attitudes. However, the direction in which the pandemic increases or decreases risk aversion is inconclusive.

Most empirical studies on the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on risk attitudes rely on student samples or samples from developed countries. However, little is known about the impact of the pandemic on risk attitudes of the rural poor in developing countries, a group which is particularly vulnerable to exogenous shocks, as they most often operate with scarce resources and limited financial safety nets. Assessing the effect of shocks on risk attitudes of these population is important, as Gloede, Menkhoff, and Waibel [34] show that shocks perpetuate vulnerability to poverty via their effect on risk attitudes. This paper explores the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on risk attitudes of rural households in Thailand, contributing to empirical literature on exogenous shocks and individual risk aversion, particularly within a non-WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) population context. Since the majority of empirical literature provides evidence on changes in risk attitudes in responses to exogenous shocks including the COVID-19 pandemic, we hypothesize that individuals would change their risk aversion after experiencing the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown. Especially, given the heterogeneity in risk attitudes, also in the context of global pandemic [35], we expect a heterogeneous pattern of changes in risk attitudes. Individuals who are more affected by lockdown policies may be more likely to change their willingness to take risks. Utilizing a large-scale data set from longitudinal household surveys allows for the comparison of risk-taking attitudes before and after the COVID-19 lockdown. In addition, geographical variations in the lockdown duration at the village level enables us to attribute changes in risk attitudes to different lockdown lengths.

The remainder of this paper is organized as follows: Section 2 provides background to the existing literature body on risk preferences and shocks. The working data is described in section 3, while section 4 presents our empirical strategy of which the results are shown and discussed in section 5. The paper closes in section 6 with concluding remarks.

2. Literature review

2.1 The assumption of stable preferences and empirical risk-elicitation measures

The conceptual framework of Stigler and Becker [13] in favor of the stability of preferences over time and across individuals has shaped economics for decades. In their seminal paper, Stigler and Becker [13] argue against incorporating shifts in preferences, proposing that preferences should be considered in different state spaces. As long as choices are state-dependent and state spaces are exogenous to households, any differences in consumption over time and across households can be completely explained by changes in state variables such as prices or incomes, without the need to introduce the unstable preference assumption. Dasgupta et al. [36] empirically test the assertion of Stigler and Becker [13] regarding the stability of preferences under the state-dependent framework and show that while risk attitudes are temporally stable, statistically significant heterogeneity exists among individuals. This echoes empirical findings by Dohmen et al. [37], l’Haridon and Vieider [38], and Vieider, Chmura, and Martinsson [8] demonstrating heterogeneity in risk attitudes across individuals. Besides that, the temporal stability of risk attitudes also lacks empirical robustness. Chuang and Schechter [14] review the results of several empirical papers, concluding that risk attitudes are moderately stable over time, with statistically significant correlation coefficients ranging from 0.13 to 0.55 in most studies. However, the low correlations cannot be entirely attributed to measurement errors and may call into question the empirical validity of the stable risk preference assumption [15]. This prompts the consideration that the degree of preference stability is ultimately an empirical matter [15, 39].

In empirical studies, risk attitudes are typically elicited through incentivized experimental measures or non-incentivized survey questions. A good elicitation method should satisfy both internal validity, implying that different measurements provide a coherent description of the same individual’s risk attitudes, and behavioral validity, meaning that measured risk attitudes can predict risk-taking behavior in the real world [15]. Experiments, due to their controlled design and incentives, are considered the gold standard for assessing risk attitudes [40]. Examples include tasks like the lottery-choice tasks designed by Holt and Laury [41], and Gneezy and Potters [42]; the financial investment task designed by Eckel and Grossman [43]; or the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) [44]. By assigning different probabilities to each outcome, i.e. higher expected payoffs correspond to more risks, experimental measures can precisely quantify risks under consideration and empirically test theoretical framework [15]. Yet, conducting incentivized experiments can be expensive and challenging with large and diverse samples, especially in developing or low-education areas [37]. Another strand of methodology is qualitative measures, often used in nationally representative surveys. This approach consists of straightforward questions that, for example, ask participants to assess their willingness to take risks on Likert scales, either generally or in specific domains like driving, health, or finances [37], thus placing less cognitive burden on participants. Dohmen et al. [37] demonstrate that while asking about the willingness to take risks in a specific domain provides a more robust measure for explaining risky behavior in that domain, general self-assessment is the best all-around predictor of different types of risky behavior. While this elicitation cannot reflect the preference parameter in the Arrow–Pratt measure [45, 46] and usually lacks direct monetary incentives, its test-retest stability as well as behavioral validity in predicting actual choices in incentivized experiments and real-life risky behaviors is well-documented across various contexts and countries [4749]. Moreover, compared with experimental measures, the self-assessment measure has demonstrated greater stability over extended time periods [14] and exhibits better predictive power for real-world outcomes [37].

2.2 Adverse shocks and risk attitudes

Empirical research on the temporal stability of risk attitudes is growing rapidly. A large body of empirical literature suggests that covariate shocks such as natural disasters, conflicts, or economic recessions may considerably alter risk aversion [50, 51]. For an overview of empirical studies on the effects of exogenous shocks on risk attitudes see Chuang and Schechter [14], Liebenehm, Schumacher, and Strobl [52], and Gassmann et al. [53].

The research on natural disasters shows conflicting results regarding the direction of their effects. Some studies, including Cameron and Shah [39], Cassar, Healy, and von Kessler [54], Hoang and Le [55], and Liebenehm, Schumacher, and Strobl [52], find that individuals exhibit higher risk aversion after exposure to natural catastrophes. Conversely, other research documents a decline in risk aversion in response to such rare events [56, 57]. The decrease in risk aversion appears to be persistent even five years after events like the Great East Japan Earthquake [58]. However, Eckel, El-Gamal, and Wilson [50] show that evacuees of Hurricane Katrina display higher risk tolerance shortly after the disaster but become moderately more risk-averse one year later. In addition, further studies distinguish risk attitudes in loss and gain domains, revealing less risk-seeking behavior in the loss domain, but less risk aversion or no statistically significant effects in the gain domain [59, 60]. Similarly, conflicts, wars, or nuclear catastrophe may decrease risk aversion [51] or increase risk aversion [6164], even for those who have not been directly exposed to the traumatic events [65]. Another strand of literature exploring the effects of the macroeconomic environment on risk attitudes provides a relatively consistent conclusion that macroeconomic crises increase individuals’ risk aversion [6670] and that such effects are usually long-lasting [71].

The variation of measurement approaches might be responsible for inconclusive results on whether shocks increase or decrease individuals’ risk aversion. A substantial literature body illustrates that attitudes towards risk change when different elicitation methods are employed, in part because participants do not consistently adhere to the same decision strategy across methods [72]. The comparability of findings across empirical studies may only hold true when the same measurement techniques are consistently applied within those studies. Apart from that, empirical studies often face challenges in separating risk attitudes (the curvature of the utility function [73]) from risk perceptions (subjective judgments or rankings of the riskiness of risky options [74]) [7577]. This could complicate the process of determining changes in risk aversion that can be attributed to shifts in attitudes, perceptions, or a combination of both [52]. This potential bias might be accentuated when utilizing cross-sectional data, given that probability distributions are not uniformly maintained for all respondents. Empirical studies using panel data argue that when respondents are presented with the same explicit stakes and probabilities both before and after the shocks, the observed changes in choices can be attributed to risk attitudes, factoring out the confounder of risk perceptions [58, 67]. Nevertheless, Just and Just [75] point out that the empirical difficulties of disentangling preferences from probability perceptions remains incompletely resolved through experimental methods. Enhanced empirical capability in separately identifying risk attitudes and perceptions could potentially provide more profound insights into the underlying causes behind the conflicting empirical results. In addition, Imas [78] proposes that inconsistent conclusions can be reconciled in distinguishing between realized losses, involving the transfer of money or other forms of value, and paper losses that remain unrealized.

One well-documented channel through which adverse shocks may affect individuals’ risk attitudes is emotional responses, such as fear, anger, or sadness [7982]. Loewenstein et al. [83] propose a risk-as-feelings hypothesis and point out that individuals react to risky situations more emotionally than cognitively and that emotional states often drive behavior. Eckel, El-Gamal, and Wilson [50] empirically test this emotion-based framework and show that emotional variables have strong explanatory power for changes in risk attitudes after experiencing Hurricane Katrina. Similarly, Hanaoka, Shigeoka, and Watanabe [58] find statistically significant correlations between the self-assessment of symptoms of depression, stress, and sleep problems and variations in risk-taking behavior after exposure to the Great East Japan Earthquake. The fear-based explanation is also supported by Callen et al. [61] and Guiso, Sapienza, and Zingales [67] in the context of social conflicts and financial crisis, respectively.

2.3 The COVID-19 pandemic and risk attitudes

The outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic constitutes a natural experiment for researchers to further investigate the stability of risk attitudes. The pandemic can be considered a natural disaster because of the health risks involved as well as a macroeconomic crisis, since the lockdown policy has severely hampered economic growth [32]. Therefore, empirical studies often use geographical variations in virus prevalence or lockdown stringency as proxies for exposure to the pandemic. The available evidence on the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on risk aversion is inconclusive, with most research finding statistically significant changes in risk attitudes after exposure to the pandemic. S1 Table in the supporting information provides an overview of empirical studies on the impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on risk attitudes.

Similar to empirical findings regarding the impact of other adverse shocks on risk attitudes, it proves challenging to generalize patterns concerning the extent and direction in which the COVID-19 pandemic affects individual willingness to take risks. In the study by Zhang and Palma [84], four elicitation methods are employed, revealing differential results when comparing incentivized behavioral measures and self-assessed measures of risk attitudes in response to the pandemic. Similarly, Adema et al. [85] show that individual risk aversion decreases when employing an incentivized lottery choice, while it increases when utilizing a self-assessed measure in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. These findings may suggest that the contradictory results may stem from employing different measures to elicit risk attitudes. However, inconsistencies persist even when comparing studies only employing incentivized gamble choice tasks: Some demonstrate the increased risk aversion after exposure to the global pandemic [33, 86], others reveal the decreased risk aversion [53, 85, 87], and the remaining do not discern considerable changes in the risk attitudes [2830]. Non-incentivized survey questions, i.e., self-reported measures, yield relatively consistent results, with the majority illustrating a decrease in the willingness to take risks [31, 32, 85, 86, 88, 89]. Overall, the results remain inconsistent even when comparing studies using similar elicitation techniques. This may suggest that the noise in risk-elicitation measures, coupled with the presence of diverse contextual factors, contributes to the challenge of obtaining a conclusive understanding of the impact of the global pandemic on individual risk attitudes.

Given the difficulties of data collection during the global pandemic, many empirical studies rely on student samples [29, 30, 53, 87, 90] or online experiment tools, such as Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) [86, 88, 91]. Nonetheless, a few studies use nationally representative household survey data to investigate the stability of risk attitudes under the COVID-19 pandemic. Ikeda, Yamamura, and Tsutsui [92] distinguish between gain and loss domains and show that Japanese households become less sensitive to additional losses, thus increasing the degree of risk tolerance in the loss domain due to the pandemic. In opposition to this, studies using German household surveys offer evidence suggesting a notable increase in risk aversion as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic [32, 89]. To the best of our knowledge, the study by Castillo and Hernandez [93] is the only one to explore the relationship between the COVID-19 pandemic and risk attitudes using a dataset from rural areas in a developing country. The authors find a significant increase in the willingness to take risks among agricultural households in Guatemala when compared to the pre-pandemic period. While their study considers commercial smallholder households, our study broadens perspectives by focusing on the rural poor in general, thus providing further insights into the understanding of risk attitudes under the COVID-19 pandemic.

Our study is similar to that of Gassmann et al. [53], who analyze the impact of the pandemic lockdown on changes in risk attitudes using a sample of students in France. By using incentivized questions in the Multiple Price List format to elicit risk attitudes during and just after the lockdown, the authors find that risk aversion decreases during the lockdown but returns to the initial levels four months after the lockdown, demonstrating the importance of timely and precise political interventions to foster a robust economic recovery. Our study employs a similar measurement of the COVID-19 pandemic, namely the lockdown duration, to analyze its impact on individuals’ risk attitudes.

3. Data and descriptive statistics

The secondary data used in this study originates from the research project “Poverty dynamics and sustainable development: A long-term panel project in Thailand and Vietnam, 2015–2024”, financed by the German Research Foundation (DFG). Data can be accessed at https://www.tvsep.de/en/tvsep-data-access.

The survey initially aims to cover a sample of 2,200 rural households in 220 villages of three provinces in northeastern Thailand, namely Buriram, Ubon Ratchathani, and Nakhon Phanom, where agricultural production dominates and per capita income is close to the poverty line [94]. The sample selection follows a three-stage stratified cluster sampling strategy, with provinces as strata and sub-districts as the primary sampling units. In the three sampled provinces, 110 sub-districts were randomly chosen with the proportional allocation of population density. At the next stages, two villages were selected from each sampled sub-district using the systematic random sampling design, and then 10 households were randomly selected from each village. Therefore, this sample is representative of the rural population in northeastern Thailand.

This study uses the data from the eighth wave household survey conducted between June and August 2019, and the COVID-19 special household as well as village head surveys conducted between November and December 2020. The 2019 household survey covers 2,199 households and includes detailed demographic and socioeconomic characteristics of each household and its members, such as age, gender, education level, household size, and risk attitudes. The COVID-19 special household survey covers 2,141 panel households, providing basic household demographic characteristics and complete information about the effects of the COVID-19 crisis on household financial, consumption, occupational, and health situations. In addition, 220 village heads were interviewed in 2020, allowing for specific information about the lockdown duration, implemented measures, e.g., curfews, school closures, or non-gatherings, and the impacts of the COVID-19 crisis at the village level. The COVID-19 special surveys were carried out about five months after the first national lockdown, which lasted from the end of March to June, minimizing recall bias among respondents. The data was anonymized to protect the privacy and confidentiality of individuals, and as such, no ethics approval was required. In the study, we have addressed any ethical considerations related to the use of the data and have taken all necessary steps to ensure that the data was used in a responsible and ethical manner. Informed consent was obtained from participants at the time the data was originally collected.

In total, 1,503 individuals were interviewed both in 2019 and in 2020. Of these individuals, 9 respondents reported a different gender between two surveys, and 27 respondents had a negative or greater than a 3-year age difference between the 2020 and 2019 surveys, which is implausible; these 36 respondents are therefore excluded. Moreover, 51 respondents aged 80 years or older in 2020 are also eliminated to ensure the soundness of the data collected [34]. The final data set for the following analysis is a balanced panel of 1,416 identical respondents from 220 villages. Data processing and analyses were conducted using Stata 15.

Table 1 summarizes the individual characteristics and the COVID-19 pandemic situation of the final sample. On average, respondents were 58 years old, and the majority were females (72.95%) and married (75.07%) in 2019. The average number of years of education was less than 6 years, below the national average of around 9 schooling years in 2019 [95]. The majority of respondents (65.32% in 2019 and 66.17% in 2020) were active in the agricultural sector. Agriculture is often characterized by risk and uncertainty; consequently, 62.57% reported little or great fluctuations in their household income in 2019. In terms of the household structure, on average, each household had about five family members, with a relatively low household dependency ratio of 26.42% in 2019 and 27.38% in 2020. Regarding health status, the majority of respondents (95.34%) felt healthy or able to manage their health problems prior to the pandemic. However, nutritional status remained an issue: Almost half of the respondents (43.64%) were under- or overweight.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary statistics of individual characteristics and the COVID-19 pandemic situation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292873.t001

The key variables in the analysis are the willingness to take risks as the dependent variable and the lockdown duration as the main explanatory variable. In both waves of each survey, respondents’ willingness to take risks is measured through the survey item, “Are you generally a person who is fully prepared to take risks or do you try to avoid taking risks?”. Respondents were asked to rate themselves on an ordinal scale from 0 (unwilling to take risks) to 10 (fully prepared to take risks). The lower the score an individual gives to this question, the more risk-averse he or she is. However, due to the type of question asked and the qualitative nature of the scale, a score of 5 (middle category) does not represent risk neutrality [34]. In the household survey 2019, risk attitudes are additionally elicited through a hypothetical investment question asking respondents how much of the 100,000 THB they have just won from a lottery would have been invested in a business that has the equal chance of either doubling or halving the invested amount. A Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.257 with a p-value of 0.000 indicates a highly statistically significant correlation between the self-assessment measure and the hypothetical investment question. Furthermore, using the same data source from different survey years, Hardeweg, Menkhoff, and Waibel [96] validate the simple survey-based risk measure with the Holt and Laury [41] task. This further increases our confidence in the use of the general risk question as a measure of risk attitude and risky behavior among the sampled individuals.

Fig 1 illustrates the distribution of the 1,416 respondents’ general willingness to take risks, measured on an 11-point scale. In 2019, prior to the COVID-19 crisis, the average willingness to take risks was 5.466, slightly higher than the middle category of 5. The spike in the histogram is at its middle, with more than a quarter of the respondents rating their willingness to take risks at 5. Another notable feature is the high response rate to the two extreme categories, with around 12% rating themselves at the score of 0 and more than 15% rating themselves at 10. These features are in line with results of other studies using the same panel data source but different years in rural Thailand [34, 96]. Compared to 2019, the responses of 2020 are particularly concentrated on the right side of the figure, suggesting that individuals became more risk-seeking from 2019 to 2020. In 2020, approximately 90% of respondents assessed their willingness to take risks at 5 or above, with an average willingness to take risks of 7.013, much higher than in 2019. This difference in risk attitudes between the two survey years is statistically significant, with a z-value of -13.134 and a p-value of 0.000 based on a two-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum (Mann-Whitney) test (see also Table 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Histogram of responses to the survey question about general willingness to take risks.

Source: Household survey 2019 and COVID-19 special household survey 2020, own calculations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292873.g001

In an attempt to restrict the possible spread of coronavirus transmission, the Thai government announced an emergency decree and a broad array of public health and social interventions in mid-March of 2020 [20, 21]. As a quick response, all village heads immediately arranged village committee meetings and/or informed their villagers through loudspeakers. Of the 220 villages, 19 villages declared a lockdown. On average, the lockdown lasted approximately 87 days. The shortest lockdown was 41 days, from the end of April to the end of May 2020, and the longest lockdown duration was 193 days, from the end of March to the end of September 2020. During the lockdown, a curfew was in place in 16 villages, drinking parties were forbidden in 17 villages, 12 villages closed schools, and residents of 5 villages were restricted from visiting temples. Compliance was very high, with only 4 out of 1,416 respondents not complying with the COVID-19 regulations during the crisis. As a result, these policies were successful in mitigating the spread of COVID-19 during the first wave of infections in these regions. In fact, only 6.21% of all respondents analyzed in this study showed some symptoms while none tested positive for COVID-19. Nonetheless, around 56% of respondents reported a negative or very negative impact of the COVID-19 crisis on their household’s financial situation. Furthermore, mental health, as indicated by how well an individual can handle (the COVID-19 related) stress and how easily he or she gets nervous, showed a statistically significant change between two survey years. After exposure to the first national lockdown, respondents became less able to address stressful situations and they became nervous more easily, suggesting a negative impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on individual mental health. These results are in line with the findings of Sapbamrer et al. [97] who note a decline in farmers’ mental health following the lockdown in Thailand. Similarly, Muro, Feliu-Soler, and Castellà [27] report an adverse impact of the lockdown duration on women’s wellbeing in Spain, underscoring the universal negative impact of lockdown on individuals’ mental health.

4. Empirical strategy

This study aims to explore the impacts of the COVID-19 lockdown on risk attitudes by using survey data before and after the first national lockdown in Thailand. The empirical strategy relies on variations in the lockdown duration at the village level while controlling for the unobserved individual time-invariant characteristics using the individual fixed-effects model. The impact of the lockdown is estimated by comparing individuals living in villages without a declared lockdown (control group) and in villages with a declared lockdown (treatment group), assuming that individuals would have the same level of risk aversion in the absence of the COVID-19 lockdown. Moreover, taking advantage of the panel structure of the data, the impacts can be further isolated by comparing risk attitudes of the same individuals before and after the COVID-19 crisis.

Formally, the following regression equation is estimated: (1) where αik represents an individual fixed-effects model netting out the influence of time-invariant characteristics. Yikt is the survey-based measure of risk attitudes for individual i in village k at time t, and Durationkt is the lockdown duration in days in village k and takes the value of zero before the lockdown. To facilitate the interpretation of outcomes, both variables are normalized to have a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one, as performed by Castillo and Hernandez [93], and Andersen et al. [98]. Eikt denotes individual emotions, i.e. stress and nervousness. The effect of year is also captured, with τt, representing the general time trend common to all individuals. εikt represents the error term and is clustered at the village level to account for potential intra-group correlations. The coefficient β represents the causal effect of the COVID-19 lockdown on individuals’ risk attitudes, depending on the length of the lockdown.

A potential confounding variable in this study is selective migration. For example, after the first lockdown, some households may have moved to villages where the lockdown was shorter or where lockdown measures were not strictly or completely enforced. However, at the time of the pandemic, mobility was limited due to the declared travel restrictions. Moreover, the mobility rate is usually low in traditional agrarian societies. In this sample, only one household changed residence from one village to another between the two surveys. Thus, migration bias is not a concern in this study.

Taking advantage of the panel structure of the data, i.e. repeated observations of the same individual, the effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on risk attitudes can be isolated by treating the individual fixed effect, αik, as a parameter to be estimated [99]. With two periods, Eq (1) is algebraically equivalent to the differencing, formally: (2) where the Δ prefix indicates the change in variables after and before the COVID-19 lockdown. The differencing removes the unobserved individual effects. For notational convenience, we denote ΔDurationkt as Durationk, because Durationkt takes the value of zero for all observations before the COVID-19 lockdown. This specification is difference-in-difference (DiD), with Durationk being a continuous variable (Durationk takes the value of zero if the lockdown in village k was not declared).

The key underlying assumption of the DiD approach is that, in the absence of the treatment, the outcome of treatment and control groups would follow the same trend over time [100]. Although this common-trends assumption cannot be tested directly, comparing the evolution of the average level of risk attitudes between villages with and without a declared lockdown provides some supportive evidence. We use the fifth to eighth wave surveys, carried out in 2013, 2016, 2017, and 2019 respectively, as well as the COVID-19 special survey to perform the pre-trend analysis. Fig 2 plots the average willingness to take risks in villages with and without a declared lockdown. Generally, the willingness to take risks shows a similar pattern over time between villages that declared a lockdown and those that did not, with individuals in villages that declared a lockdown being, on average, slightly more risk-averse. Based on Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, the differences in the point estimates between the two groups are not statistically significant before the COVID-19 crisis, while in 2020 the difference is statistically significant at the 5% level. In all, there are no systematic differences in the average risk attitudes concerning the duration of the lockdown, supporting the common trends assumption. In addition, the validity of this assumption is further tested by conducting a placebo test in Section 5.3.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Average willingness to take risks over time for villages with and without a declared lockdown.

Source: Household surveys in 2013, 2016, 2017, 2019, and COVID-19 special household survey in 2020, own calculations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292873.g002

5. Results and discussions

5.1 Main results

Table 2 summarizes the estimates using Eq (2). All specifications include individual fixed effects, netting out the influence of unobserved time-invariant variables. Note that a negative coefficient indicates that risk aversion increases with the duration of the lockdown.

thumbnail
Table 2. The impacts of the COVID-19 lockdown on general willingness to take risks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292873.t002

Column (1) displays results for a specification including only individual fixed effects. The willingness to take risks is positively correlated with the lockdown duration and this is statistically significant at the 10% level.

Column (2) presents results, controlling for both individual and year fixed effects. In comparison to the estimate in Column (1), the effect becomes statistically insignificant and changes from positive to negative. The 2020-year fixed effect, which captures general fluctuations in the macroeconomic environment, is large enough to compensate for the effects of the lockdown duration estimated in the model including only the individual fixed effect (Column (1)). The coefficient on the year dummy is positive and highly statistically significant, indicating that individuals are substantially more willing to take risks compared to 2019. Similar large year effects are also found by Graeber et al. [32] using the same self-assessment measure among the German population. They argue that this is unsurprising, given that the COVID-19 pandemic is first and foremost an aggregate shock affecting all individuals.

Since the literature suggests that emotional responses are a potential mechanism through which exogenous shocks affect risk attitudes [82], two individual emotional variables, i.e., handling stress and feeling nervous, are added as controls in Column (3). Both variables are statistically significant in influencing changes in risk attitudes. Individuals who can handle stress well are also more likely to exhibit risk-loving behavior. This is in line with the psychology literature showing that stress can have a statistically significant positive impact on risk aversion [101, 102]. Similar results are demonstrated by Tsutsui and Tsutsui-Kimura [103] in which stress that arose from the COVID-19 pandemic is an influencing factor for changes in risk attitudes. Regarding nervousness, individuals who get nervous easily are more likely to reduce their level of risk aversion. This observation is substantiated by a lab-in-the-field experiment carried out among micro-entrepreneurs in Vietnam. The study reveals that financial worries increase self-reported levels of feeling nervous, leading to reduction in individuals’ levels of risk aversion [80].

After adding all controls (see Column (3)), the coefficient on lockdown duration is slightly smaller than zero and statistically insignificant, indicating that there are no considerable changes in the willingness to take risks in response to the lockdown duration. Employing the same self-assessment measures, studies conducted in developed countries have found only modest effect sizes. Graeber et al. [32] find that a one standard deviation increase in the state-level exposure to COVID-19 decreases the willingness to take risks by about 0.057 of a standard deviation in Germany. Also in Germany, Frondel, Osberghaus, and Sommer [89] show that a one standard deviation increase in the self-assessed financial income losses due to the pandemic is associated with a 0.043 standard deviation decrease in the willingness to take risks, and the corresponding impact size is 0.121 standard deviations when considering severe rather than any financial losses. Our results align with a study conducted among agricultural smallholders in rural areas of Guatemala that employed the same risk-elicitation measure [93]. In general, they observe a significant increased willingness to take risks compared to the pre-pandemic period. However, direct exposure to the virus or experiencing community-level confinement attenuate the effects, resulting in increased risk aversion.

5.2 Effect heterogeneity

Numerous empirical studies demonstrate the substantial heterogeneity in the response of the willingness to take risks to the COVID-19 pandemic [32, 85]. To account for possible effects of heterogeneity, Eq (2) was rerun including all controls and an additional term that interacts the standardized lockdown duration with a binary variable indicating subgroups under consideration. The following analysis follows the work of Graeber et al. [32] and focuses on pre-existing demographics, socioeconomic and health characteristics that, as demonstrated in the literature, are related to differences in risk attitudes among individuals. The results are presented in Table 3.

thumbnail
Table 3. Heterogenous effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on general willingness to take risks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292873.t003

Individuals of different genders, ages, marital and family status often differ in their changes in risk attitudes following negative shocks. For example, Hanaoka, Shigeoka, and Watanabe [58] find that men who experienced the 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake become more risk tolerant, while the opposite pattern is observed for women. Graeber et al. [32] show that men and married individuals increase their willingness to take risks after exposure to the COVID-19 pandemic in a statistically significantly greater degree compared to female and unmarried individuals. Columns (1) to (4) in Table 3 display the corresponding estimates. None of the coefficients on the interaction term is statistically significant, indicating that individuals with these different demographic characteristics do not exhibit differential responses in terms of risk attitudes in our sample. Notably, the marginal change in risk attitudes in response to the lockdown among women and men displays nearly equivalent magnitudes. This result may initially appear unexpected, given the extensive body of empirical research indicating gender risk differences [104, 105] as well as gender differentials in times of the COVID-19 pandemic [106]. While the use of different risk-elicitation methods may influence the manifestation of gender differences in risk taking [107, 108], substantial gender differentials in risk attitudes exist in the pandemic context when employing the self-reported measure [32]. However, a cross-country analysis reports evidence associating the strong gender differentiation in preferences with advanced levels of economic development and gender equality [109]. The study also shows that Thailand exhibits relatively minor gender differences in preferences. Considering the proximity of the surveyed individuals’ income to the poverty line, the absence of gender differences in the willingness to take risks might not be as surprising as it appears at first glance.

Similarly, less economically deprived individuals are shown to be more sensitive to the pandemic and to change their risk attitudes more strongly [32]. To account for the effect of heterogeneity in terms of socioeconomic backgrounds, Columns (5) to (7) include individual educational achievement, agricultural occupation, and income fluctuation, respectively. There is a statistically significant difference in the effect of the lockdown duration on risk aversion between individuals in and outside the agricultural sector. On average, as the lockdown duration increases by one standard deviation, the willingness to take risks decreases by 0.105 of a standard deviation for individuals working outside the agricultural sector. Individuals in the non-agricultural sector are more sensitive to the lockdown duration and become statistically significantly more risk-averse, while the lockdown has no statistically significant impact on the willingness to take risks of individuals in the agricultural sector. The result is plausible given that adverse impacts of the lockdown measures, such as social distancing or closure of the workplace, are more pronounced in individuals in the non-agricultural sector [110]. In addition, agriculture is often characterized by risk and uncertainty, especially in rural areas of developing countries. Compared to non-agricultural workers, individuals working in the agricultural sector are already exposed to multiple risks and may have learnt to adopt and navigate unpredictable and hazardous circumstances. As a result, they could be less sensitive to pandemic in terms of changing attitudes towards risk. Other socioeconomic characteristics do not account for a statistically significant heterogeneity of the main result.

Medical literature demonstrates that fatal COVID-19 outcomes are often observed in certain subgroups with pre-existing health problems [111]. Therefore, individuals with certain pre-determined health conditions may exhibit differential responses in terms of risk attitudes, i.e., the high-risk groups of COVID-19 may become more risk averse than the rest of the population. Column (8) and (9) display results considering individuals of different health statuses. After experiencing the lockdown, sick individuals exhibit a much stronger increase in risk aversion than healthy individuals or individuals who can manage their health problems. However, this difference is not statistically significant. In addition, the adjustment in risk attitudes of under- or overweight respondents is not considerably different from others.

5.3 Robustness checks

In this section, we perform a series of robustness analyses. Thus far, we have shown that although there is no statistically significant change in risk attitudes among the full sample, the risk aversion of respondents outside the agricultural sector shows a statistically significant increase after experiencing the first lockdown in rural Thailand. Therefore, the following robustness checks focus on individuals outside the agricultural sector, of which results are presented in Table 4. Column (1) repeats the results of Column (6) in Table 3, taking into account the effect heterogeneity between individuals inside and outside the agricultural sector.

First, the estimated causal inference of the lockdown on risk attitudes relies on the DiD approach. Fig 2 shows that the average willingness to take risks in villages with and without a declared lockdown exhibits approximately parallel, period-specific changes before the pandemic, providing graphical evidence for the credibility of the common trend assumption. To further assess the internal validity of this assumption, we perform a placebo regression based on data from two periods before the lockdown, namely the seventh household survey wave in 2017 and the eighth wave in 2019. If the common trend assumption indeed holds, one would expect that the statistically significant impacts of the lockdown on risk attitudes would not occur in earlier periods. Column (2) in Table 4 presents the results. The coefficient on the lockdown duration is statistically insignificant and has a different sign compared to the baseline results in column (1), which validates the DiD method.

Second, we allow for a nonlinearity in the main independent variable by introducing a squared term of the standardized lockdown duration in the regression equation. Results in Column (2) show that individuals outside the agricultural sector have a strong statistically significant increase in their risk aversion at the beginning of the lockdown, but after a certain length of the lockdown, they become risk tolerant.

Another concern is that results are sensitive to outliers. To address this issue, we follow the M-estimation for regression proposed by Huber [112], which gives less weight to residuals that are more likely to be outliers. The results using M-estimator (Column (3)) remain mostly unchanged both quantitatively and in terms of their statistical significance, suggesting that the main result is not driven by outliers.

Next, the results may be influenced by the way in which risk attitudes are measured. The dependent variable, the willingness to take risks, is an ordinally scaled variable that allows for monotonic increasing transformations. When the empirical analysis relies on models appropriate for quantitative variables, such as the fixed-effects model, such a transformation can lead to contradictory results [113]. To account for this concern, we follow the idea of Graeber et al. [32] and transform the dependent variable into a dummy that is insensitive to monotonic transformations. The dummy variable takes the value of one if a respondent rates his or her willingness to take risks above 5, and zero otherwise. The results are presented in Column (5) and are robust to this alternative construction of the dependent variable.

As a final robustness check, we assess the results by measuring lockdown severity in different terms. In the main analysis, the lockdown duration measured in days is considered a proxy for the lockdown intensity. However, not only the length of the lockdown, but also how many different measures were implemented, such as curfew, school closures, or forbidding gatherings, contribute to the severity of the lockdown. Thus, we use of the number of measures implemented in each village as an alternative indicator for the lockdown stringency. The estimates are displayed in Column (6), where patterns similar to the baseline results can be observed.

6. Concluding remarks

A better understanding of the temporal stability of risk attitudes is of great importance in economics given their role in individual decision-making behavior and macroeconomic outcomes. While classical economics assumes risk preferences to be consistent over time and across individuals [13], new empirical literature suggests that negative shocks such as natural disasters, conflicts, wars, or economic crises can considerably alter individuals’ risk aversion [14, 15]. The onset of the COVID-19 pandemic has posed threats to many aspects of individuals’ lives, therefore giving rise to the question whether individuals’ risk attitudes also change considerably as a result of exposure to the pandemic.

This study contributes to empirical literature on the temporal stability of risk attitudes through investigating whether and to what extent experiencing the first lockdown affects individuals’ risk attitudes in rural Thailand. Risk attitudes are elicited by a simple survey item whose internal and behavioral validity is well-documented [37, 47]. A high-quality data set was obtained from longitudinal household surveys, allowing for a clear comparison of specific individuals’ willingness to take risks before and after the lockdown. Together with village-level variations in the lockdown duration, causal inference can be established by using a DiD approach.

This study shows that, on average, rural individuals statistically significantly increase their willingness to take risks in 2020 compared to the pre-pandemic period of 2019. However, this increased risk tolerance is not mainly the result of the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown. Instead, the lockdown policies do not considerably change the level of risk attitudes in our sample. As expected, a statistically significant effect heterogeneity is found between individuals inside and outside the agricultural sector. Individuals working outside the agricultural sector experienced a statistically significant reduction in their willingness to take risks after experiencing the lockdown, while individuals working within the agricultural sector do not substantially change their risk aversion. For individuals outside the agricultural sector, the willingness to take risks decreases by approximately 0.105 of a standard deviation with one standard deviation increase in the lockdown duration at the village level. Consistent with other literature, emotional reactions, i.e., stress and nervousness, are found to influence the adjustment in risk attitudes in a statistically significant way. The result of changes in risk attitudes of non-agricultural sector workers is robust to a placebo test, accounting for the nonlinearity in the effects of lockdown, replacing outliers, and alternative constructions of both dependent and explanatory variables.

The findings of this study elicit important implications for both individual decision-making and broader economic outcomes. As the COVID-19 pandemic influenced lives globally, our research sheds light on the dynamic nature of risk attitudes, showing that exposure to the pandemic, such as lockdown, can influence individuals’ willingness to take risks. Particularly noteworthy is the variation in this effect among different sectors of the population. The observed increase in risk aversion of individuals working in the non-agricultural sector after exposure to the first national lockdown may have multifaceted consequences. On the one hand, it could lead to a greater willingness to comply with public health and social measures, but on the other hand, it could reduce their likelihood of investing and starting new businesses. Rural households are generally regarded as risk-averse, and this has the potential to lead to suboptimal decision-making and consequently undermine the overall welfare of impoverished rural populations. Increased risk aversion of non-agricultural workers could become an additional barrier to their escape from poverty and thus stagnate long-term growth. Our findings thus underscore the necessity for nuanced policy responses. Policymakers should recognize that pandemic-induced alterations in risk attitudes may exert enduring impacts, potentially influencing compliance behavior and the trajectory of economic recovery.

Possible future research is necessary to explore whether the effects of the pandemic on risk attitudes are persistent or transitory, since the data used in this study was collected only five months after the first national lockdown in Thailand. Furthermore, due to the lack of specific data, it remains unexamined whether changes in risk attitudes will lead to changes in real-life risk-taking behavior, such as gambling, smoking, or virus prevention. This could offer additional valuable insights into understanding individuals’ risk attitudes and related behavior and remains a valuable avenue for future studies.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Overview of studies investigating the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on risk attitudes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292873.s001

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the TVSEP (Thailand Vietnam Socio Economic Panel) research project for providing data. We would like to express our gratitude to two anonymous reviewers and the editors of PLOS ONE for their insightful comments.

References

  1. 1. Frijns B, Koellen E, Lehnert T. On the determinants of portfolio choice. J Econ Behav Organ. 2008 May 1;66(2):373–86.
  2. 2. Guiso L, Paiella M. THE ROLE OF RISK AVERSION IN PREDICTING INDIVIDUAL BEHAVIOR [Internet]. Bank of Italy, Economic Research and International Relations Area; 2005 Feb [cited 2023 Feb 7]. Report No.: 546. https://econpapers.repec.org/paper/bdiwptemi/td_5f546_5f05.htm
  3. 3. Herrero J, Urueña A, Torres A, Hidalgo A. Smartphone addiction: Psychosocial correlates, risky attitudes, and smartphone harm. J Risk Res. 2019 Jan 2;22(1):81–92.
  4. 4. Hsieh C, Parker SC, van Praag CM. Risk, balanced skills and entrepreneurship. Small Bus Econ. 2017 Feb 1;48(2):287–302.
  5. 5. Jaeger DA, Dohmen T, Falk A, Huffman D, Sunde U, Bonin H. Direct evidence on risk attitudes and migration. Rev Econ Stat. 2010;92(3):684–9.
  6. 6. Outreville JF. Risk aversion, risk behavior, and demand for insurance: A survey. J Insur Issues. 2014;37(2):158–86.
  7. 7. Kanbur SM. Of risk taking and the personal distribution of income. J Polit Econ. 1979;87(4):769–97.
  8. 8. Vieider FM, Chmura T, Martinsson P. Risk attitudes, development, and growth: Macroeconomic evidence from experiments in 30 countries [Internet]. WZB Discussion Paper; 2012 [cited 2023 Feb 7]. Report No.: SP II 2012–401. https://www.econstor.eu/handle/10419/66905
  9. 9. Vieider FM, Lefebvre M, Bouchouicha R, Chmura T, Hakimov R, Krawczyk M, et al. Common components of risk and uncertainty attitudes across contexts and domains: Evidence from 30 countries. J Eur Econ Assoc. 2015;13(3):421–52.
  10. 10. Müller S, Rau HA. Economic preferences and compliance in the social stress test of the COVID-19 crisis. J Public Econ. 2021 Feb 1;194:104322. pmid:35702336
  11. 11. Nikolov P, Pape A, Tonguc O, Williams C. Predictors of Social Distancing and Mask-Wearing Behavior: Panel Survey in Seven U.S. States. 2020.
  12. 12. Trueblood JS, Sussman AB, O’Leary D. The role of risk preferences in responses to messaging about COVID-19 vaccine take-up. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2022 Jan 1;13(1):311–9.
  13. 13. Stigler GJ, Becker GS. De Gustibus Non Est Disputandum. Am Econ Rev. 1977;67(2):76–90.
  14. 14. Chuang Y, Schechter L. Stability of experimental and survey measures of risk, time, and social preferences: A review and some new results. J Dev Econ. 2015 Nov 1;117:151–70. pmid:30078930
  15. 15. Schildberg-Hörisch H. Are risk preferences stable? J Econ Perspect. 2018 May;32(2):135–54. pmid:30203933
  16. 16. World Health Organization. Thailand: How a strong health system fights a pandemic [Internet]. 2020 Sep [cited 2022 Feb 13]. https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/thailand-how-a-strong-health-system-fights-a-pandemic
  17. 17. Mathieu E, Ritchie H, Rodés-Guirao L, Appel C, Giattino C, Hasell J, et al. Our World in Data. 2020 [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Thailand: Coronavirus Pandemic Country Profile. https://ourworldindata.org/coronavirus/country/thailand
  18. 18. Mathieu E, Ritchie H, Rodés-Guirao L, Appel C, Giattino C, Hasell J, et al. COVID-19: Stringency Index [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2023 Aug 25]. https://ourworldindata.org/covid-stringency-index
  19. 19. Dechsupa S, Assawakosri S, Phakham S, Honsawek S. Positive impact of lockdown on COVID-19 outbreak in Thailand. Travel Med Infect Dis. 2020 Jul 1;36:101802. pmid:32569811
  20. 20. Wongtanasarasin W, Srisawang T, Yothiya W, Phinyo P. Impact of national lockdown towards emergency department visits and admission rates during the COVID-19 pandemic in Thailand: A hospital-based study. Emerg Med Australas. 2021;33(2):316–23. pmid:33070468
  21. 21. World Health Organization Thailand. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) WHO Thailand Situation Report—18 March 2020—Thailand [Internet]. 2020 Mar [cited 2023 Feb 13]. https://reliefweb.int/report/thailand/coronavirus-disease-2019-covid-19-who-thailand-situation-report-18-march-2020
  22. 22. World Health Organization Thailand. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) WHO Thailand Situation Report—3 April 2020—Thailand [Internet]. 2020 Apr [cited 2023 Feb 13]. https://reliefweb.int/report/thailand/coronavirus-disease-2019-covid-19-who-thailand-situation-report-3-april-2020-enth
  23. 23. Baker C, Phongpaichit P. Thailand and the COVID-19 Pandemic in 2020. Southeast Asian Aff. 2021;356–78.
  24. 24. Hirunyatrakul P. The costs of lockdown: Assessing the employment and livelihood impacts of lockdown in Thailand during the Covid-19 pandemic. TDRI Q Rev. 2020 Mar;35(1):17–32.
  25. 25. The World Bank. GDP growth (annual %)—Thailand [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2023 Feb 10]. https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/NY.GDP.MKTP.KD.ZG?locations=TH
  26. 26. de la Rosa PA, Cowden RG, de Filippis R, Jerotic S, Nahidi M, Ori D, et al. Associations of lockdown stringency and duration with Google searches for mental health terms during the COVID-19 pandemic: A nine-country study. J Psychiatr Res. 2022 Jun 1;150:237–45. pmid:35398667
  27. 27. Muro A, Feliu-Soler A, Castellà J. Psychological impact of COVID-19 lockdowns among adult women: the predictive role of individual differences and lockdown duration. Women Health. 2021 Aug 9;61(7):668–79. pmid:34284689
  28. 28. Angrisani M, Cipriani M, Guarino A, Kendall R, Ortiz de Zarate J. Risk preferences at the time of COVID-19: An experiment with professional traders and students [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2020 May [cited 2023 Feb 11]. Report No.: 3609586. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3609586
  29. 29. Drichoutis AC, Nayga RM. On the stability of risk and time preferences amid the COVID-19 pandemic. Exp Econ. 2022 Jun 1;25(3):759–94. pmid:34404975
  30. 30. Lohmann PM, Gsottbauer E, You J, Kontoleon A. Anti-social behaviour and economic decision-making: Panel experimental evidence in the wake of COVID-19. J Econ Behav Organ. 2023 Feb 1;206:136–71. pmid:36531911
  31. 31. Bu D, Hanspal T, Liao Y, Liu Y. Risk taking, preferences, and beliefs: Evidence from Wuhan [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2020 Dec [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 3559870. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3559870
  32. 32. Graeber D, Schmidt U, Schroeder C, Seebauer J. The effect of a major pandemic on risk preferences—Evidence from exposure to COVID-19 [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2020 Nov [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 3724461. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3724461
  33. 33. Mussio I, Sosa Andrés M, Kidwai AH. Higher Order Risk Attitudes in the Time of COVID-19: An Experimental Study [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2021 Jun [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 3870398. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3870398
  34. 34. Gloede O, Menkhoff L, Waibel H. Shocks, Individual Risk Attitude, and Vulnerability to Poverty among Rural Households in Thailand and Vietnam. World Dev. 2015 Jul 1;71:54–78.
  35. 35. Guenther B, Galizzi MM, Sanders JG. Heterogeneity in Risk-Taking During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Evidence From the UK Lockdown. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2023 Aug 13];12. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.643653 pmid:33868115
  36. 36. Dasgupta U, Gangadharan L, Maitra P, Mani S. Searching for preference stability in a state dependent world. J Econ Psychol. 2017 Oct 1;62:17–32.
  37. 37. Dohmen T, Huffman D, Schupp J, Falk A, Sunde U, Wagner GG. Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. J Eur Econ Assoc. 2011;9(3):522–50.
  38. 38. l’Haridon O, Vieider FM. All over the map: A worldwide comparison of risk preferences. Quant Econ. 2019;10(1):185–215.
  39. 39. Cameron L, Shah M. Risk-taking behavior in the wake of natural disasters. J Hum Resour. 2015;50(2):484–515.
  40. 40. Charness G, Gneezy U, Imas A. Experimental methods: Eliciting risk preferences. J Econ Behav Organ. 2013 Mar 1;87:43–51.
  41. 41. Holt CA, Laury SK. Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects. Am Econ Rev. 2002 Dec;92(5):1644–55.
  42. 42. Gneezy U, Potters J. An experiment on risk taking and evaluation periods. Q J Econ. 1997 May 1;112(2):631–45.
  43. 43. Eckel CC, Grossman PJ. Sex differences and statistical stereotyping in attitudes toward financial risk. Evol Hum Behav. 2002 Jul 1;23(4):281–95.
  44. 44. Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol Appl. 2002;8:75–84. pmid:12075692
  45. 45. Arrow KJ. Essays in the theory of risk-bearing. Chicago: Markham Pub. Co; 1971. 278 p. (Markham economics series).
  46. 46. Pratt JW. Risk Aversion in the Small and in the Large. Econometrica. 1964;32(1/2):122–36.
  47. 47. Ding X, Hartog J, Sun Y. Can we measure individual risk attitudes in a survey? [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2010 Mar [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 1570425. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=1570425
  48. 48. Falk A, Becker A, Dohmen T, Huffman D, Sunde U. The Preference Survey Module: A Validated Instrument for Measuring Risk, Time, and Social Preferences. Manag Sci. 2023 Apr;69(4):1935–50.
  49. 49. Lönnqvist JE, Verkasalo M, Walkowitz G, Wichardt PC. Measuring individual risk attitudes in the lab: Task or ask? An empirical comparison. J Econ Behav Organ. 2015 Nov 1;119:254–66.
  50. 50. Eckel CC, El-Gamal MA, Wilson RK. Risk loving after the storm: A Bayesian-Network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. J Econ Behav Organ. 2009 Feb 1;69(2):110–24.
  51. 51. Voors MJ, Nillesen EEM, Verwimp P, Bulte EH, Lensink R, Van Soest DP. Violent conflict and behavior: A field experiment in Burundi. Am Econ Rev. 2012 Apr;102(2):941–64.
  52. 52. Liebenehm S, Schumacher I, Strobl E. Rainfall shocks and risk aversion: Evidence from Southeast Asia. Am J Agric Econ. 2023;n/a(n/a):1–32.
  53. 53. Gassmann X, Malézieux A, Spiegelman E, Tisserand JC. Preferences after pan(dem)ics: Time and risk in the shadow of COVID-19. Judgm Decis Mak. 2022;17(4):23.
  54. 54. Cassar A, Healy A, von Kessler C. Trust, Risk, and Time Preferences After a Natural Disaster: Experimental Evidence from Thailand. World Dev. 2017 Jun 1;94:90–105.
  55. 55. Hoang TX, Le NVT. Natural disasters and risk aversion: Evidence from Vietnam. Nat Resour Forum. 2021;45(3):211–29.
  56. 56. Abatayo AL, Lynham J. Risk preferences after a typhoon: An artefactual field experiment with fishers in the Philippines. J Econ Psychol. 2020 Aug 1;79:102195.
  57. 57. Bchir MA, Willinger M. Does the exposure to natural hazards affect risk and time preferences? Some insights from a field experiment in Perú [Internet]. LAMETA, Universtiy of Montpellier; 2013 Mar [cited 2023 Feb 9]. https://econpapers.repec.org/paper/lamwpaper/13-04.htm
  58. 58. Hanaoka C, Shigeoka H, Watanabe Y. Do Risk Preferences Change? Evidence from the Great East Japan Earthquake. Am Econ J Appl Econ. 2018 Apr;10(2):298–330.
  59. 59. Li JZ, Li S, Wang WZ, Rao LL, Liu H. Are people always more risk averse after disasters? Surveys after a heavy snow-hit and a major earthquake in China in 2008. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2011;25(1):104–11.
  60. 60. Reynaud A, Aubert C. Does flood experience modify risk preferences? Evidence from an artefactual field experiment in Vietnam. Geneva Risk Insur Rev. 2020 Mar 1;45(1):36–74.
  61. 61. Callen M, Isaqzadeh M, Long JD, Sprenger C. Violence and risk preference: Experimental evidence from Afghanistan. Am Econ Rev. 2014 Jan;104(1):123–48.
  62. 62. Danzer N, Danzer AM, Fehr E. The behavioral and psychological consequences of a nuclear catastrophe: The case of Chernobyl. In Augsburg, Germany: Kiel und Hamburg: ZBW—Deutsche Zentralbibliothek für Wirtschaftswissenschaften, Leibniz-Informationszentrum Wirtschaft; 2016 [cited 2023 Feb 13]. https://www.econstor.eu/handle/10419/145944
  63. 63. Kim YI, Lee J. The long-run impact of a traumatic experience on risk aversion. J Econ Behav Organ. 2014 Dec 1;108:174–86.
  64. 64. Moya A. Violence, psychological trauma, and risk attitudes: Evidence from victims of violence in Colombia. J Dev Econ. 2018 Mar 1;131:15–27.
  65. 65. Sacco K, Galletto V, Blanzieri E. How has the 9/11 terrorist attack influenced decision making? Appl Cogn Psychol. 2003;17(9):1113–27.
  66. 66. Dohmen T, Lehmann H, Pignatti N. Time-varying individual risk attitudes over the Great Recession: A comparison of Germany and Ukraine. J Comp Econ. 2016 Feb 1;44(1):182–200.
  67. 67. Guiso L, Sapienza P, Zingales L. Time varying risk aversion. J Financ Econ. 2018 Jun 1;128(3):403–21.
  68. 68. Levin R, Vidart D. Risk-taking adaptation to macroeconomic experiences: theory and evidence from developing countries [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2020 Oct [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 3748186. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3748186
  69. 69. Necker S, Ziegelmeyer M. Household risk taking after the financial crisis. Q Rev Econ Finance. 2016 Feb 1;59:141–60.
  70. 70. West T, Worthington AC. Macroeconomic conditions and Australian financial risk attitudes, 2001–2010. J Fam Econ Issues. 2014 Jun 1;35(2):263–77.
  71. 71. Shigeoka H. Long-term consequences of growing up in a recession on risk preferences [Internet]. National Bureau of Economic Research (NBER); 2019 Oct [cited 2023 Mar 20]. Report No.: 26352. https://www.nber.org/papers/w26352
  72. 72. Pedroni A, Frey R, Bruhin A, Dutilh G, Hertwig R, Rieskamp J. The risk elicitation puzzle. Nat Hum Behav. 2017 Nov;1(11):803–9. pmid:31024120
  73. 73. Weber EU. Risk attitude and preference. WIREs Cogn Sci. 2010;1(1):79–88. pmid:26272841
  74. 74. Slovic P. Perception of Risk. Science. 1987 Apr 17;236(4799):280–5. pmid:3563507
  75. 75. Just DR, Just RE. Empirical Identification of Behavioral Choice Models under Risk. Am J Agric Econ. 2016;98(4):1181–94.
  76. 76. Just RE, Just DR. Global identification of risk preferences with revealed preference data. J Econom. 2011 May 1;162(1):6–17.
  77. 77. Just DR, Khantachavana SV, Just RE. Empirical Challenges for Risk Preferences and Production. Annu Rev Resour Econ. 2010;2(1):13–31.
  78. 78. Imas A. The Realization Effect: Risk-Taking after Realized versus Paper Losses. Am Econ Rev. 2016 Aug;106(8):2086–109.
  79. 79. Cohn A, Engelmann J, Fehr E, Maréchal MA. Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: An experiment with financial professionals. Am Econ Rev. 2015 Feb;105(2):860–85.
  80. 80. Dalton PS, Nhung N, Rüschenpöhler J. Worries of the poor: The impact of financial burden on the risk attitudes of micro-entrepreneurs. J Econ Psychol. 2020 Aug 1;79:102198.
  81. 81. Lerner JS, Gonzalez RM, Small DA, Fischhoff B. Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychol Sci. 2003 Mar 1;14(2):144–50. pmid:12661676
  82. 82. Meier AN. Emotions and Risk Attitudes. Am Econ J Appl Econ. 2022 Jul;14(3):527–58.
  83. 83. Loewenstein GF, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001;127:267–86. pmid:11316014
  84. 84. Zhang P, Palma MA. Stability of Risk Preferences During COVID-19: Evidence From Four Measurements. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2022 [cited 2023 Aug 14];12. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.702028 pmid:35222133
  85. 85. Adema J, Nikolka T, Poutvaara P, Sunde U. On the stability of risk preferences: Measurement matters. Econ Lett. 2022 Jan 1;210:110172. pmid:36540652
  86. 86. Mussio I, Sosa Andrés M, Kidwai AH. Higher order risk attitudes in the time of COVID-19: an experimental study. Oxf Econ Pap. 2023 Jan 1;75(1):163–82.
  87. 87. Shachat J, Walker MJ, Wei L. How the onset of the Covid-19 pandemic impacted pro-social behaviour and individual preferences: Experimental evidence from China. J Econ Behav Organ. 2021 Oct 1;190:480–94. pmid:34642514
  88. 88. Alsharawy A, Ball S, Smith A, Spoon R. Fear of COVID-19 changes economic preferences: evidence from a repeated cross-sectional MTurk survey. J Econ Sci Assoc. 2021 Dec 1;7(2):103–19.
  89. 89. Frondel M, Osberghaus D, Sommer S. Corona and the stability of personal traits and preferences: Evidence from Germany [Internet]. Mannheim, Germany: Leibniz-Zentrum für Europäische Wirtschaftsforschung (ZEW), Mannheim, Germany; 2021 [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 21–029. https://www.econstor.eu/handle/10419/232943
  90. 90. Li H. Life is either a daring adventure, or it is boring: The impact of COVID-19 on immoral and nonmoral risk taking behaviors. J Behav Decis Mak. 2023;e2319.
  91. 91. Tsai CI, Zeng Y. Risky but alluring: Severe COVID-19 pandemic influence increases risk taking. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2021;27:679–94. pmid:35073131
  92. 92. Ikeda S, Yamamura E, Tsutsui Y. COVID-19 enhanced diminishing sensitivity in prospect-theory risk preferences: A panel analysis [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Rochester, NY; 2020 Oct [cited 2023 Feb 13]. Report No.: 3715236. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3715236
  93. 93. Castillo JG, Hernandez MA. The unintended consequences of confinement: Evidence from the rural area in Guatemala. J Econ Psychol. 2023 Mar 1;95:102587.
  94. 94. Hardeweg B, Klasen S, Waibel H. Establishing a database for vulnerability assessment. In: Vulnerability to poverty: theory, measurement and determinants, with case studies from Thailand and Vietnam. 2013. (Vulnerability to poverty: theory, measurement and determinants, with case studies from Thailand and Vietnam.—Basingstoke, Hampshire [u.a.]: Palgrave Macmillan, ISBN 0-230-24891-8.—2013, p. 50–79).
  95. 95. Statista. Thailand: mean years of schooling 2021 [Internet]. 2022 [cited 2023 Feb 13]. https://www.statista.com/statistics/1258329/thailand-mean-years-of-schooling-people-aged-15-and-older/
  96. 96. Hardeweg B, Menkhoff L, Waibel H. Experimentally Validated Survey Evidence on Individual Risk Attitudes in Rural Thailand. Econ Dev Cult Change. 2013;61(4):859–88.
  97. 97. Sapbamrer R, Chittrakul J, Sirikul W, Kitro A, Chaiut W, Panya P, et al. Impact of COVID-19 Pandemic on Daily Lives, Agricultural Working Lives, and Mental Health of Farmers in Northern Thailand. Sustainability. 2022 Jan;14(3):1189.
  98. 98. Andersen S, Harrison GW, Lau MI, Rutström EE. Lost in state space: Are preferences stable? Int Econ Rev. 2008;49(3):1091–112.
  99. 99. Angrist JD, Pischke JS. Chapter 5. Parallel Worlds: Fixed Effects, Differences-in-Differences, and Panel Data. In: Mostly Harmless Econometrics [Internet]. Princeton University Press; 2008 [cited 2023 Feb 9]. p. 221–48. https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/9781400829828-010/html?lang=en
  100. 100. Wing C, Simon K, Bello-Gomez RA. Designing Difference in Difference Studies: Best Practices for Public Health Policy Research. Annu Rev Public Health. 2018;39(1):453–69. pmid:29328877
  101. 101. Cahlíková J, Cingl L. Risk preferences under acute stress. Exp Econ. 2017 Mar 1;20(1):209–36.
  102. 102. Kandasamy N, Hardy B, Page L, Schaffner M, Graggaber J, Powlson AS, et al. Cortisol shifts financial risk preferences. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2014 Mar 4;111(9):3608–13. pmid:24550472
  103. 103. Tsutsui Y, Tsutsui-Kimura I. How does risk preference change under the stress of COVID-19? Evidence from Japan. J Risk Uncertain. 2022 Apr 1;64(2):191–212. pmid:35342225
  104. 104. Eckel CC, Grossman PJ. Chapter 113 Men, Women and Risk Aversion: Experimental Evidence. In: Plott CR, Smith VL, editors. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results [Internet]. Elsevier; 2008 [cited 2023 Aug 17]. p. 1061–73. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1574072207001138
  105. 105. Croson R, Gneezy U. Gender Differences in Preferences. J Econ Lit. 2009 Jun;47(2):448–74.
  106. 106. Belot M, Müller S, Rau HA, Schwieren C. Editorial: Gender Differentials in Times of COVID-19. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2022 [cited 2023 Aug 17];13. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.901087 pmid:35677123
  107. 107. Charness G, Eckel C, Gneezy U, Kajackaite A. Complexity in risk elicitation may affect the conclusions: A demonstration using gender differences. J Risk Uncertain. 2018 Feb 1;56(1):1–17.
  108. 108. Filippin A, Crosetto P. A Reconsideration of Gender Differences in Risk Attitudes. Manag Sci. 2016 Nov;62(11):3138–60.
  109. 109. Falk A, Hermle J. Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality. Science. 2018 Oct 19;362(6412):eaas9899. pmid:30337384
  110. 110. Gottlieb C, Grobovšek J, Poschke M, Saltiel F. Lockdown accounting. BE J Macroecon. 2022 Jan 1;22(1):197–210.
  111. 111. Treskova-Schwarzbach M, Haas L, Reda S, Pilic A, Borodova A, Karimi K, et al. Pre-existing health conditions and severe COVID-19 outcomes: an umbrella review approach and meta-analysis of global evidence. BMC Med. 2021 Aug 27;19(1):212. pmid:34446016
  112. 112. Huber PJ. Robust estimation of a location parameter. Ann Math Stat. 1964;35(1):73–101.
  113. 113. Schröder C, Yitzhaki S. Revisiting the evidence for cardinal treatment of ordinal variables. Eur Econ Rev. 2017 Feb 1;92:337–58.