Figures
Abstract
Fear of crime has been rising persistently in Chile, even in periods where actual crime rates have decreased, making the perception of crime an important policy issue. This paper presents the results of the impact evaluation of a pilot public policy designed to reduce fear of crime around a shopping centre in Santiago, Chile. The pilot policy consisted of installing a team including police officers and local government officials that handed out information leaflets and talked to passers-by about crime prevention. Pre-intervention and post-intervention surveys were conducted in the shopping centre where the programme was implemented and in a control shopping centre nearby to identify the causal effects of the policy using a difference-in-differences empirical strategy. The results indicate that the programme was effective in reducing fear of crime around the shopping centre, especially at night among its workers, and that it reduced actual crime. However, a deeper analysis suggests that the programme might have actually increased fear of crime among the individuals who directly interacted with the programme. The reduction in crime might have indirectly resulted in an overall reduction in fear among workers, who are likely to be up to date on criminal occurrences in the area, explaining how an increase in fear in those directly contacted is consistent with an overall reduction in fear across workers.
Citation: Benavente JM, Goya D (2023) The fear-increasing and fear-decreasing effects of a pilot policy to reduce fear of crime. PLoS ONE 18(3): e0282461. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0282461
Editor: Sergio A. Useche, University of Valencia: Universitat de Valencia, SPAIN
Received: April 20, 2021; Accepted: February 16, 2023; Published: March 6, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Benavente, Goya. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.
Funding: This paper is based on the results of the impact evaluation of a pilot public policy for the reduction of insecurity implemented by the Chilean Laboratorio de Gobierno and financed by the Inter-American Development Bank (IDB). The information and opinions presented here are entirely those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the IDB, its Board of Executive Directors, or the countries they represent. All data analyses and conclusions, and the manuscript, are the independent work of the authors, and there was no institutional review of the manuscript before submission. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Both being a victim and the fear of being a victim of crime are issues that affect the wellbeing of millions of individuals across the globe [1–3]. In Chile, as in most Latin American countries, the perception of crime is consistently among the top issues of concern among the population according to most opinion polls [4, 5].
As discussed in a review by Hale [6], some of the social costs associated with fear of crime are its psychological effects, changes in individual behaviours, fractures in the sense of community, and increased punitiveness, and fear of crime even risks undermining the legitimacy of the criminal justice system.
There is a wealth of literature on the determinants of fear of crime. There is strong evidence of the roles played by gender and socio-economic factors as determinants of fear. For most variables, however, including age and even previous victimisation, the results do not show a clear effect on fear (see the reviews by Hale [6] and Collins [7] and evidence of possible differences in developing countries in Alda et al. [8]). In public spaces, in particular, there is evidence of greater fear of crime in commercial areas [9], at bus stops [10], and in places in which it would be difficult to escape from potential crime [11].
One theme that is relevant for the interpretation of our findings is the relationship between fear of crime and vicarious victimisation. As argued by [12, 13], previous literature supports the idea that the fear of crime is associated with knowledge about the victimisation suffered by other individuals. Some examples include [14, 15]. There is evidence that the perception of neighbourhood crime trends [16] and spatial proximity to crime [17] affect fear of crime. It is important to understand flows of information about crime, as evidence of a positive relationship between social cohesion and fear of crime is interpreted as being a result of a greater diffusion of information about crimes [18, 19]. This possibility is relevant for the interpretation of our results.
There has been increasing interest in understanding fear of crime in developing countries. See, for instance, [4, 8, 20–22]. In Latin America in particular, there has been work on the determinants of fear of crime [23] and the perceived risk of crime [24] in Peru; the determinants of fear of crime in Brazil and Argentina [25]; the relationship between fear of crime and the media in Paraguay [26]; the effect of fear of crime on trust in the policy and the judiciary in several countries, including Mexico, Argentina and Brazil [27]; and the relationship between fear of crime and social cohesion in Brazil [18, 28]. Another area that has been studied in the region is the relationship between fear of crime and punitive attitudes [29–31].
A topic that has received particular attention in South America is the relationship between fear of crime and public transport, including how it affects behaviour [32–35]. Soto et al. [10] find a higher fear of crime at bus stops, which are public spaces, like the shopping centres where the policy analysed in this paper was implemented.
Since police forces have realised that the usual techniques might not be sufficient to reduce fear of crime [36], community-oriented policing and procedures based on broken windows theory have been the main strategies used to overcome the problem [37]. The former focuses on establishing relationships with the community, while the latter focuses on targeting minor offenses to avoid escalation to more serious forms of crime. Weisburd et al. [38] concluded that broken windows policing does not reduce fear of crime. Evidence for community policing is more encouraging [39, 40] but far from conclusive [41]. The evidence for the effect of police presence on fear is mixed, with some studies indicating that it can reduce fear [39, 42], others indicating that it can be interpreted as being related to higher crime and may thus increase fear [43–45], and some find mixed results [46].
In an effort to try new approaches to deal with the problem of fear of crime, the Chilean Government Lab conducted a pilot intervention in a shopping centre in Santiago, Chile. Developed in a participatory process with the different stakeholders, the intervention consisted of a stand where police officers and local government officials would distribute informative brochures and talk to passers-by about crime prevention.
This paper describes that pilot intervention and presents an impact evaluation of its effects. By doing so, it contributes to knowledge about fear of crime in a commercial context, since most research has been conducted in residential settings, even though it is known that fear of crime can have a negative impact on businesses [47, 48]. The mixed findings provide useful insights for policymakers, but lines of inquiry for further research are suggested to reaffirm some of the conclusions.
Intervention and data
Description of the pilot policy
The Chilean Laboratorio de Gobierno (Government Lab) was in charge of implementing a pilot policy to reduce fear of crime in the comuna (smallest administrative unit) of Lo Barnechea, in Santiago, Chile. The first stage of the design of the intervention included local government officials, police officers, and members of the community, and the main conclusion was to focus on the provision of information about crime prevention and hard data on crime in the area. The local government decided to focus the intervention on a shopping centre because according to their knowledge of the area, there were high levels of fear of crime in the surrounding areas, especially among the workers of the shopping centre (most of whome were, according to the local government, residents of the comuna).
The intervention consisted of installing a stand where police officers and officials from the local government would talk to passers-by about crime prevention, and distribute brochures with facts about crime in the area and tips on crime prevention. The programme records indicate that the team was present 33 days out of the 95-day period between the first and last interventions (between September and December 2018), for an average of approximately 1.2 hours each time (for a total of 40 hours of intervention). They interacted with approximately 70 individuals per hour on average (for a total of approximately 2,800 total individuals reached directly). There was always at least one person from the local government present (sometimes two), and of the 33 instances of the intervention, there were one or two police officers present during 23 of them.
The rest of the paper describes the data and methodology used to evaluate the impact of the intervention and presents descriptive statistics and results of the evaluation.
Data
The collection of data to evaluate the intervention was a separate process, independent from the intervention itself. This increases the quality of the intervention and the data gathered, as each team was specialised in its job, and eliminates the conflicts of interest that exists if the data are gathered by the same individuals who are conducting the intervention.
As difference-in-differences was deemed the best method for the evaluation (details in the next section), data before and after the intervention was needed for the shopping centre where the intervention was conducted and for a comparable control zone. Another shopping centre within the same comuna, located at a walking distance of seven hundred metres, was used as the control. A pre-intervention survey and a post-intervention survey were conducted in each shopping centre, to passers-by in the same areas where the intervention was conducted. Each round consisted of 200 surveys in each shopping centre, for a total of 800 observations. All the survey data used were completely anonymous.
There is no information on the population of workers and visitors in the shopping centre that would allow us to evaluate how representative the sample is of the population. Nevertheless, any self-selection biases in the respondents should on average be the same across the four surveys (treatment/control and before/after), meaning that the analyses have internal validity. The respondents in the before and after samples were not the same individuals.
The survey was based on the ENUSC (Encuesta Nacional Urbana de Seguridad Ciudadana), a large-scale national level survey on crime and related topics that has been conducted yearly since 2005. The survey was simplified relative to the ENUSC, with an emphasis on fear of crime questions but also including the key variables that have been identified by the literature as determinants of fear of crime, to use them as controls. Given the particular interest in fear of crime among workers, the survey included a question about the reason for being at the shopping centre. The post-intervention survey included a question that directly asked whether the individual had interacted with the programme. This question was at the end of the survey to avoid influencing the answers to the other questions.
We do not dig into the debate over how to measure fear of crime [6, 7, 49], given that we decided to measure it in the same way currently used in surveys to measure crime in Chile. The questions used are listed in Table 1. The variables are defined such that a higher value is associated with feeling safer.
Descriptive statistics
Table 2 presents the means for the main variables for three groups: the first two are workers and non-workers in the intervention area, using our survey data (combining the before and after intervention data). As a reference, we present a third column where we use data from the ENUSC 2018 survey to estimate the means for the same variables for the Santiago region. There is significant heterogeneity across comunas in Santiago; however, unfortunately, the publicly available ENUSC data identifies the region but not the comuna of respondents, so this is the most precise reference we can obtain.
Relative to the means in the region, shopping centre customers (non-workers) are younger, and the main income providers in their households are more educated and more likely to be employed. This is consistent with the sociodemographic profile of residents of Lo Barnechea. The workers are younger than both other groups and have a slightly higher education level than the average for Santiago, but still much lower than that of non-workers. There is a higher proportion of women among workers.
The most striking differences are in victimisation: the likelihood of robbery of non-workers in the area is slightly higher than the region’s average, but almost three times as higher for workers in the area than is the average for the region’s population. The differences are starker for theft: non-workers in the area are twice as likely to experience theft than the average resident of the region, and workers in the area are more than three times as likely to experience theft than is the region’s average resident (20% and 6%, respectively). Almost a third of all workers (31%) have suffered from at least one of these forms of victimisation. The fact that the population of the intervention area is not representative of the whole region is not a problem for the causal interpretation of the estimates we present, but it warns against generalising results to other groups.
The perception from the local government that workers of this shopping centre have high levels of fear of crime is consistent with these data on victimisation, assuming a positive relationship between victimisation and fear of crime (although evidence on this is mixed, see [6, 50]).
Evaluation methods
Difference-in-differences
The evaluation was designed before the implementation of the programme. A randomised experiment was not feasible. The characteristics of the intervention meant that it was not possible to randomly compel some individuals to interact with it and others to avoid it. Difference-in-differences was chosen as the preferred method to estimate the causal effect of the intervention. This method consists of comparing the change (before and after) in measures of fear of crime in the intervention area, with the change in the same variables in a control area over the same period. The assumption required for this estimator to have a causal interpretation is that the change in the outcome variables in the absence of the programme would have been the same in both areas (this is the ‘parallel trends’ assumption). The credibility of this assumption depends on the quality of the control area, which in this case is highly reliable due to its geographical proximity and socioeconomic similarity. More formally, we can assume that in the absence of the ‘treatment’ (the pilot intervention), the measure of fear of crime for individual i in shopping centre s at time t, yi,s,t is determined by the following additive structure:
where λs and τt are place- and time-specific factors, xi,s,t is a vector of individual-level characteristics, and εi,s,t contains the other unobserved, individual-specific determinants of the measure of fear of crime. If the treatment status treatedi (binary variable identifying observations from the shopping centre with the intervention) is unrelated to the change in outcomes, i.e., E(yi,s,1 − yi,s,0|treatedi, xi,s,t) = E(yi,s,1 − yi,s,0|xi,s,t), then the difference-in-differences estimate of the impact of the programme for individuals in the shopping centre where the programme was implemented (the ‘average treatment effect on the treated’) corresponds to the δ coefficient in the following regression equation:
(1)
where treated is a binary variable that equals one for observations from the treated shopping centre, and post_treatment is a binary variable equal to one for the post-intervention surveys. The time and shopping centre indices (t and s respectively) were omitted for simplicity. The individual-level variables in x are unlikely to cause bias if omitted, but it is useful to include them to increase efficiency. For more details on the methodology, see [51, 52]. The shopping centre which we are using as a control is located nine minutes’ walking distance in the same comuna and serves a similar segment of the population.
The variables that are used as controls, which are motivated by the previous literature on fear of crime, are age, gender, having been a victim of theft, having been a victim of robbery, a measure of the opinion of the performance of the police, and a measure of how frequently the individual goes to that shopping centre. In addition, there are three variables related to the household’s main income provider, namely, level of education, a dummy indicating whether he or she is currently employed, and a measure of his or her type of job, included as a fixed-effect given its categorical nature. A measure of household income is not included because the question went unanswered for an important share of respondents. Details on the measurement of all these variables are available in S1 Table.
Simple differences
The inclusion of a question to identify the individuals who directly interacted with the programme allows for exploring whether the policy had a direct effect on them. However, an assumption much stronger than the ‘parallel trends’ difference-in-differences assumption would be needed to provide these estimates with a causal interpretation. For individuals in the intervention centre in the post-intervention period, if the variable interactedi identifies the individuals who had a direct relationship with the programme, we can assume that fear of crime is determined as follows:
(2)
If E(ui|interactedi, xi) = E(ui|xi), then δ′ would estimate the causal effect of the policy on the individuals who interacted with it. However, this assumption is strong, it implies that once the observable characteristics xi are controlled for, there are no unobservable differences between the individuals who did and did not interact with the programme. Since interaction with the programme required the individual to approach the programme team, there could be self-selection issues. The exogeneity assumption breaks down, for example, if the individuals who are more fearful of crime are more likely to approach a police officer.
Simple difference estimates are unlikely to be unbiased estimates of causal effects, but we provide evidence that individuals who did and did not interact with the programme are on average almost indistinguishable across a broad set of observable characteristics, suggesting that they are also similar across unobservables. That fact, together with a relatively good set of controls, including measures of victimisation and the individual’s evaluation of the police, suggest that the simple difference estimates still provide useful information.
The methods explained above will be used to explore a possible effect of the programme not only on fear of crime but also on other relevant variables, such as the perception of police effectiveness and actual crime, to inform our interpretation of the results.
All of the calculations and estimations of regression models were conducted using Stata/MP version 15.1.
Results
Main result: Causal effect on fear of crime
Table 3 presents the results for the main evaluation of the programme using difference-in-differences. The dependent variables are those listed in Table 1. Only the coefficients for the impact of the policy (δ in Eq 1) are reported, for three different samples (the full regression output for these and all of the other regressions reported are available in S1 Appendix).
The first row includes the whole sample, the second row includes only non-workers at the shopping centres, and the third row includes only workers at the shopping centres. Examining these groups separately is justified not only because of the policy interest in workers. As argued by Dammert and Salazar Tobar [5], the feeling of insecurity in Latin American countries can also be related to deep social changes that have resulted in increased socioeconomic inequalities. Workers and customers at this shopping centre may face different socioeconomic conditions, and as a result, their relationships to crime, the police, and fear of crime might differ.
Examining the full sample first, only the coefficients for ‘walking around this shopping centre’ are significant and positive, indicating that the policy had the expected fear-reduction effect. However, this effect is spatially limited to the shopping centre where the programme was implemented. When splitting the sample into workers and non-workers, an interesting result appears: fear-reduction effect at night is driven exclusively by the workers at the shopping centre. The coefficient of 0.5 indicates that the programme, on average, reduced fear by half a point of a four-point scale, or in other words, half of the workers at the shopping centre had a one-point reduction in their level of fear using the four-point scale, which is an important effect.
Additional results
Interacting with the programme.
Since the respondents of the post-intervention survey were asked specifically if they had interacted with the programme, we can estimate the relationship between directly interacting with the programme and fear of crime. First, as explained in the methodology section, it is necessary to check whether the individuals who interacted with the programme are similar at least in observable characteristics to those who did not. Tables 4 and 5 show the average values for our control variables for individuals who did and did not interact with the programme, together with the p-values for a two-tailed t-test for differences in means. Table 4 shows results for the whole sample, and Table 5 shows the results for non-workers and workers separately.
For the whole sample, the only significant difference is in age (those who interacted with the programme were on average approximately seven years older). For non-workers, there is a similar difference in ages and a slight difference in the education of the household’s main income provider. For workers, there are differences only in the frequency of visits (with more frequent visits by the workers who interacted with the programme). Given the qualitative nature of most variables, additional results for the Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon rank-sum test and Pearson’s chi-squared test are reported in S2 Appendix. The only difference from the results from Tables 4 and 5 is some evidence of differences in the reported rates of robbery, which are significantly higher for those who interacted with the programme but only across non-workers. Crime victims (among non-workers) were more likely to interact with the programme than non-crime victims; if this is indicative of unobservables that are also related to fear, estimates should not be interpreted causally for non-workers. All of the other tests indicate that the groups are comparable, suggesting that the estimates might be close to a causal effect, especially for workers.
Table 6 has the same structure as Table 3; however, the coefficients are not the effects estimated through difference-in-differences but the simple difference in fear of crime between those who did and did not interact with the programme (δ′ in Eq 2), controlling for the same factors as before.
The sample sizes are smaller because only observations from the post-intervention survey at the treated shopping centre can be used (and only those without missing information for covariates).
Some coefficients are significant and negative for the full sample and for non-workers, and all coefficients are significant and negative for workers. The negative coefficients imply that if these estimates have a causal interpretation–which according to Table 5 is more likely for workers–the programme increased fear of crime among those directly treated, which at first sight seems inconsistent with the difference-in-differences estimates. If there are differences in unobservables between the two groups (and thus the estimates are not causal effects), one interpretation could be that workers who were initially more fearful were more likely to approach the intervention. We conduct some additional exercises to help us interpret this result.
Police evaluation.
The literature suggests that there could be a relationship between confidence in the police and fear of crime [7, 27, 53]. Now, we explore whether there is evidence of an effect of the policy on individuals’ evaluations of the police as a possible mechanism through which the programme could be affecting fear of crime. Table 7 presents difference-in-differences estimates and the simple differences estimates for the impact of interacting with the programme for the three samples. For comparison, it presents the results not only for trust in the police but also for trust in the local government (which helped deliver the programme) and the judiciary (for which the programme should have no effect). All of the estimates are insignificant.
Victimisation.
One possible explanation for the seemingly inconsistent results from Tables 3 and 6 is that the direct effect of the programme is an increase in fear of crime but that, at the same time, police presence reduced actual crime. If workers are aware of the crime levels around the shopping centre, a reduction in crime could have resulted indirectly in a reduction in fear of crime among this group.
Table 8 shows in its first column the difference-in-differences estimates for being a victim of any crime and, in the second column, for being a victim of any crime near the shopping centre where the individual was interviewed. The results show a weakly but significant negative effect on crime (reduction in victimisation) but only for crimes occurring near the shopping centre. This result supports the idea that the reason for an overall fear-reduction effect, together with a fear-inducing one among those directly treated, could be that increased police presence reduced crime, which is what translated into overall reduced fear across workers, something we discuss in more detail below.
Discussion
This paper presented the results of the impact evaluation of a pilot programme that aimed to reduce fear of crime around a shopping centre in Santiago, Chile. The policy consisted of setting up a stand where local government officials and members of the police engaged in conversations with passers-by about crime prevention and distributed brochures with crime prevention tips and facts about crime in the area.
The difference-in-differences estimates showed an overall fear-reduction effect across workers, spatially circumscribed around the area close to the shopping centre where the programme was implemented. Using information about who interacted directly with the programme, it is possible to explore whether interacting with the officials had an impact on the individual level. There is a significant relationship between interacting with the programme and higher fear of crime. There are two possible explanations for this outcome: it could reflect that more fearful individuals are more likely to approach the police or that interacting with the police increased their fear. The similarity across individuals who did and did not interact with the programme–especially across workers–even in variables such as police satisfaction, together with previous evidence of a possible increase in fear associated with police presence [43, 44], suggests that the latter explanation is feasible.
How can we reconcile the fear-reduction effect obtained for workers of the ‘treated’ shopping centre with the fear-induction effect observed across those who directly interacted with the programme? The results also show that the policy had the effect of reducing actual crime in the intervened shopping centre. There could be three mechanisms at play. First, the direct effect of the programme (i.e., on those who approached it) was negative: fear increased, a result consistent with [43–46]. [43, 44]. Second, police presence resulted in a reduction in crime rates. This outcome is not surprising considering the evidence of the crime-deterring effect of police presence [54–57]. Third, information about the lower crime rates was transmitted through informal interactions between the workers at the shopping centre, leading to an overall reduction in fear of crime among workers at the intervened shopping centre. This outcome is consistent with the strong evidence of the effect of vicarious victimisation on fear of crime [6, 13]. Specifically, [16] shows evidence that fear of crime can be affected by the perception of neighbourhood crime trends, and [17] shows that spatial proximity to crime has an effect on fear. Moreover, workers as a group probably have a certain degree of social cohesion, which could lead to higher transmission of information about crime and thus a relationship between effective crime and fear (see the discussions in [18, 19]).
The evidence for the second and third mechanisms described above is stronger, based on difference-in-differences causal estimates. The evidence that the first mechanism (the fear-increasing direct effect of interacting) is a causal impact of the intervention is weaker, but even if it is not, that would not affect the rest of the interpretation, i.e., the intervention reduced fear through a reduction in overall crime in the area, but the effect was only on workers, who are up to date on changes in the victimisation levels in the area.
As argued by Bennett [58] and Abbott et al. [40], police presence or community-oriented practices might not directly reduce fear but might require a simultaneous reduction in crime or perceived disorder to have that effect, which could be what we are observing here. The potential of there being a fear-inducing effect only when there is no reduction in actual crime rates should be considered when designing policies to reduce fear of crime that involve interaction with the police.
The descriptive statistics showed that the population of workers and non-workers of the shopping centre where the intervention was conducted is not comparable to the average resident of the Santiago region. This is not a concern for the causal interpretation of our results (which hinges on the similarity between treatment and control areas), but it suggests caution when generalising the findings to other contexts.
Conclusions and further research
Our results show that attempts to reduce fear of crime based on local goverment officials and police officers talking to individuals about crime prevention can have mixed results. Our results suggest that interacting with police officers could actually increase individuals’ fear of crime. However, we also have strong evidence that the intervention in this study reduced fear of crime across workers of the shopping centre in general and that it reduced actual crime. It is possible that this reduction in effective crime was what ended up reducing fear. However, lingering issues imply that further research is needed.
The reduction occurred only among workers (who were actually the main target group). One possible explanation is that this is a relatively small and stable group through which updated information about crime is easily shared, something that cannot be scaled up to other contexts. Confirming the role of the reduction in crime on the effect on fear, and the mechanisms through which it occurred requires further research.
For two of the estimated effects (the reduction in crime and the reduction in fear of crime across workers), the results are highly reliable, but the estimated fear-increasing effect of directly interacting with the intervention at the individual level relies on stronger assumptions. The impossibility of estimating credible causal effects of the direct interaction with the intervention is a limitation of our work that should be addressed in future research.
A third consideration is that the workers, and especially the non-workers visiting the shopping centre where the intervention was conducted, are not representative of the overall population. While this is not a problem for the internal validity of the estimates, it limits their external validty (i.e., the possibility of generalising the findings to other groups). Similar policy experiments in different socioeconomic contexts would be useful to reaffirm the findings.
Supporting information
S1 Table. List of variables.
List of variables used and how they are measured.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0282461.s002
(PDF)
S1 Appendix. Detailed regression output.
The detailed output of all of the regressions reported in the paper.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0282461.s003
(PDF)
S2 Appendix. Additional balancing tests.
Extensions of Tables 4 and 5 with statistical tests for discrete variables.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0282461.s004
(PDF)
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Roman Yosif and the team at Laboratorio de Gobierno and at the Subsecretariat of Crime Prevention, as well as María José Jarquin, Nathalie Alvarado, Gloriana Sojo, Lucía Dammert, Patricio Domínguez, the Editor Sergio Useche and two anonymous referees for their comments and suggestions. All errors remain our own.
References
- 1. Lorenc T, Clayton S, Neary D, Whitehead M, Petticrew M, Thomson H, et al. Crime, fear of crime, environment, and mental health and wellbeing: mapping review of theories and causal pathways. Health & place. 2012;18(4):757–765. pmid:22542441
- 2. Lorenc T, Petticrew M, Whitehead M, Neary D, Clayton S, Wright K, et al. Fear of crime and the environment: systematic review of UK qualitative evidence. BMC public health. 2013;13(1):1–8. pmid:23705936
- 3. Pearson AL, Breetzke GD. The association between the fear of crime, and mental and physical wellbeing in New Zealand. Social Indicators Research. 2014;119(1):281–294.
- 4. Dammert L, Malone MFT. Fear of Crime or Fear of Life? Public Insecurities in Chile. Bulletin of Latin American Research. 2003;22(1):79–101.
- 5.
Dammert L, Salazar Tobar F. Fear and insecurity in Latin America. In: The Routledge international handbook on fear of crime. Routledge; 2017.
- 6. Hale C. Fear of Crime: A Review of the Literature. International Review of Victimology. 1996;4(2):79–150.
- 7. Collins RE. Addressing the inconsistencies in fear of crime research: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2016;47:21–31.
- 8. Alda E, Bennett RR, Morabito MS. Confidence in the police and the fear of crime in the developing world. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management. 2017;40(2):366–379.
- 9. Hassinger J. Fear of crime in public environments. Journal of Architectural and Planning Research. 1985; p. 289–300.
- 10. Soto J, Orozco-Fontalvo M, Useche SA. Public transportation and fear of crime at BRT Systems: Approaching to the case of Barranquilla (Colombia) through integrated choice and latent variable models. Transportation research part A: policy and practice. 2022;155:142–160.
- 11. Fisher BS, Nasar JL. Fear of crime in relation to three exterior site features: Prospect, refuge, and escape. Environment and Behavior. 1992;24(1):35–65.
- 12. Fox KA, Nobles MR, Piquero AR. Gender, crime victimization and fear of crime. Security Journal. 2009;22:24–39.
- 13. Lee HD, Reyns BW, Kim D, Maher C. Fear of Crime Out West: Determinants of Fear of Property and Violent Crime in Five States. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 2020;64(12):1299–1316. pmid:32111137
- 14. Meško G, Šifrer J, Vošnjak L. Fear of Crime Urban and Rural Environments in Slovenia. Journal of Criminal Justice and Security. 2012;(3):259–276.
- 15. Gomme IM. The Role of Experience in the Production of Fear of Crime: A Test of a Causal Model. Canadian Journal of Criminology. 1988;30:67.
- 16. Kim S, Han S, Maskály J. Factors of fear of crime among Korean citizens: The mediating effect of confidence in the police. International Journal of Law, Crime and Justice. 2021;66:100488.
- 17. Zhao JS, Lawton B, Longmire D. An examination of the micro-level crime–fear of crime link. Crime & Delinquency. 2015;61(1):19–44.
- 18. Villarreal A, Silva BF. Social cohesion, criminal victimization and perceived risk of crime in Brazilian neighborhoods. Social forces. 2006;84(3):1725–1753.
- 19. Drakulich KM. Social capital, information, and perceived safety from crime: The differential effects of reassuring social connections and vicarious victimization. Social Science Quarterly. 2015;96(1):176–190.
- 20.
Dammert L, Reyes GAL. Victimización y temor en Chile: Revisión teórico-empírica en doce comunas del país. Centro de Estudios en Seguridad Ciudadana, Instituto de Asuntos Públicos …; 2002.
- 21.
Dammert L. Fear and crime in Latin America: redefining state-society relations. vol. 3. Routledge; 2012.
- 22. Medina JC, Rodrigues H. The effects of victimization and fear of crime on the legal socialization of young adolescents in São Paulo, Brazil. International journal of offender therapy and comparative criminology. 2019;63(8):1148–1174. pmid:30547705
- 23. Hernández W, Dammert L, Kanashiro L. Fear of crime examined through diversity of crime, social inequalities, and social capital: An empirical evaluation in Peru. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology. 2020;53(4):515–535.
- 24.
Hernández W. Additive and synergistic perceived risk of crime: A multilevel longitudinal study in Peru. In: The Routledge international handbook on fear of crime. Routledge; 2017. p. 368–387.
- 25. Valente R, Vacchiano M. Determinants of the fear of crime in Argentina and Brazil: A cross-country comparison of non-criminal and environmental factors affecting feelings of insecurity. Social Indicators Research. 2021;154:1077–1096.
- 26. Molinas-Delvalle DB. Influencia de la televisión en la percepción de inseguridad y miedo al delito de la población paraguaya. Revista Internacional de Investigación en Ciencias Sociales. 2021;17(1):158–174.
- 27. Singer AJ, Chouhy C, Lehmann PS, Walzak JN, Gertz M, Biglin S. Victimization, Fear of Crime, and Trust in Criminal Justice Institutions: A Cross-National Analysis. Crime & Delinquency. 2019;65(6):822–844.
- 28. Borges D, Cano I. Determinants of fear of crime in Brazil: the effect of social cohesion. Civitas-Revista de Ciências Sociais. 2022;21:455–466.
- 29. Chevigny P. The populism of fear: Politics of crime in the Americas. Punishment & Society. 2003;5(1):77–96.
- 30. Singer AJ, Chouhy C, Lehmann PS, Stevens JN, Gertz M. Economic anxieties, fear of crime, and punitive attitudes in Latin America. Punishment & Society. 2020;22(2):181–206.
- 31.
Gutierrez CDC. Trust in Government versus Fear of Crime as Predictors of Support for Authoritarian Policies in Ecuador: A Cross-Sectional Study. East Tennessee State University; 2014.
- 32. Orozco-Fontalvo M, Soto J, Arévalo A, Oviedo-Trespalacios O. Women’s perceived risk of sexual harassment in a Bus Rapid Transit (BRT) system: The case of Barranquilla, Colombia. Journal of Transport & Health. 2019;14:100598.
- 33. Alonso F, Useche SA, Faus M, Esteban C. Does urban security modulate transportation choices and travel behavior of citizens? A national study in the Dominican Republic. Frontiers in Sustainable Cities. 2020;2:42.
- 34.
Allen H, Cárdenas G, Pereyra LP, Sagaris L, et al. Ella se mueve segura. Un estudio sobre la seguridad personal de las mujeres y el transporte público en tres ciudades de América Latina. 2019.
- 35. Vilalta CJ. Fear of crime in public transport: Research in Mexico City. Crime prevention and community safety. 2011;13:171–186.
- 36.
Moore MH, Trojanowicz RC. Corporate strategies for policing. 6. US Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of Justice; 1989.
- 37.
Cordner G. Reducing fear of crime. Strategies for police; 2010.
- 38. Weisburd D, Hinkle JC, Braga AA, Wooditch A. Understanding the mechanisms underlying broken windows policing: The need for evaluation evidence. Journal of research in crime and delinquency. 2015;52(4):589–608.
- 39. Weisburd D, Eck JE. What can police do to reduce crime, disorder, and fear? The annals of the American academy of political and social science. 2004;593(1):42–65.
- 40. Abbott J, McGrath SA, May DC. The Effects of Police Effort on Victims’ Fear of Crime. American Journal of Criminal Justice. 2020;45(5):880–898.
- 41. Gill C, Weisburd D, Telep CW, Vitter Z, Bennett T. Community-oriented policing to reduce crime, disorder and fear and increase satisfaction and legitimacy among citizens: A systematic review. Journal of experimental criminology. 2014;10(4):399–428.
- 42. Sampson RJ, Cohen J. Deterrent effects of the police on crime: A replication and theoretical extension. Law and Society Review. 1988; p. 163–189.
- 43.
Worrall JL. Does “broken Windows” Law Enforcement Reduce Serious Crime? California Institute for County Government Sacramento, CA; 2002.
- 44. Fernandes A. Red lights and handcuffs: The effect of arrests on the fear of crime. Social Science Quarterly. 2018;99(4):1390–1408.
- 45. Hinkle JC, Weisburd D. The irony of broken windows policing: A micro-place study of the relationship between disorder, focused police crackdowns and fear of crime. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2008;36(6):503–512.
- 46. Scheider MC, Rowell T, Bezdikian V. The impact of citizen perceptions of community policing on fear of crime: Findings from twelve cities. Police quarterly. 2003;6(4):363–386.
- 47. Fisher B. A Neighborhood Business Area is Hurting: Crime, Fear of Crime, and Disorders Take their Toll. Crime & Delinquency. 1991;37(3):363–373.
- 48. Greenbaum RT, Tita GE. The Impact of Violence Surges on Neighbourhood Business Activity. Urban Studies. 2004;41(13):2495–2514.
- 49. Ferraro KF, Grange RL. The Measurement of Fear of Crime. Sociological Inquiry. 1987;57(1):70–97.
- 50.
Hanslmaier M, Kemme S, Baier D. Victimisation, fear of crime and life satisfaction. In: Representative studies on victimisation. Nomos Verlagsgesellschaft mbH & Co. KG; 2016. p. 251–284.
- 51.
Wooldridge JM. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. MIT press; 2010.
- 52.
Angrist JD, Pischke JS. Mostly harmless econometrics: An empiricist’s companion. Princeton university press; 2008.
- 53. Box S, Hale C, Andrews G. Explaining fear of crime. The British Journal of Criminology. 1988;28(3):340–356.
- 54. Sherman LW, Weisburd D. General deterrent effects of police patrol in crime “hot spots”: A randomized, controlled trial. Justice Quarterly. 1995;12(4):625–648.
- 55. Di Tella R, Schargrodsky E. Do Police Reduce Crime? Estimates Using the Allocation of Police Forces After a Terrorist Attack. American Economic Review. 2004;94(1):115–133.
- 56. Lin MJ. More police, less crime: Evidence from US state data. International Review of Law and Economics. 2009;29(2):73–80.
- 57. Weisburd S. Police Presence, Rapid Response Rates, and Crime Prevention. The Review of Economics and Statistics. 2019; p. 1–45.
- 58. Bennett T. The effectiveness of a police-initiated fear-reducing strategy. The British Journal of Criminology. 1991;31(1):1–14.