Figures
Abstract
Automatic facial coding (AFC) is a promising new research tool to efficiently analyze emotional facial expressions. AFC is based on machine learning procedures to infer emotion categorization from facial movements (i.e., Action Units). State-of-the-art AFC accurately classifies intense and prototypical facial expressions, whereas it is less accurate for non-prototypical and less intense facial expressions. A potential reason might be that AFC is typically trained with standardized and prototypical facial expression inventories. Because AFC would be useful to analyze less prototypical research material as well, we set out to determine the role of prototypicality in the training material. We trained established machine learning algorithms either with standardized expressions from widely used research inventories or with unstandardized emotional facial expressions obtained in a typical laboratory setting and tested them on identical or cross-over material. All machine learning models’ accuracies were comparable when trained and tested with held-out dataset from the same dataset (acc. = [83.4% to 92.5%]). Strikingly, we found a substantial drop in accuracies for models trained with the highly prototypical standardized dataset when tested in the unstandardized dataset (acc. = [52.8%; 69.8%]). However, when they were trained with unstandardized expressions and tested with standardized datasets, accuracies held up (acc. = [82.7%; 92.5%]). These findings demonstrate a strong impact of the training material’s prototypicality on AFC’s ability to classify emotional faces. Because AFC would be useful for analyzing emotional facial expressions in research or even naturalistic scenarios, future developments should include more naturalistic facial expressions for training. This approach will improve the generalizability of AFC to encode more naturalistic facial expressions and increase robustness for future applications of this promising technology.
Citation: Büdenbender B, Höfling TTA, Gerdes ABM, Alpers GW (2023) Training machine learning algorithms for automatic facial coding: The role of emotional facial expressions’ prototypicality. PLoS ONE 18(2): e0281309. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0281309
Editor: Muhammad Fazal Ijaz, Sejong University, REPUBLIC OF KOREA
Received: August 17, 2022; Accepted: January 20, 2023; Published: February 10, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Büdenbender et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: Data for the study are available at https://madata.bib.uni-mannheim.de/id/eprint/327 [1]. 1. Höfling TTA, Alpers GW, Büdenbender B, Gerdes ABM, Föhl U. What’s in a face: Automatic facial coding of basic emotional expressions of untrained participants compared to trained actors [Data file]. University of Mannheim; 2020. doi:10.7801/327 The R code files are available from the data repository of the University of Mannheim (https://madata.bib.uni-mannheim.de/408/); The repository additionally also has a doi: 10.7801/408.
Funding: The publication of this article was funded by the University of Mannheim.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Automatic Facial Coding (AFC) is now ubiquitous and has made great strides in emotion recognition of facial expressions. Such technology is promising because facial expressions are highly relevant for social interactions and carry information about the emotional state of an individual [1, 2]. Thus, assessing and quantifying emotional facial expressions is a primary objective in emotion research [3]. AFC follows the logic of the best established observational system, the Facial Action Coding System (FACS; [4]), which requires manual assignment of facial movements (i.e., Action Unit; AU) by human expert observers. AFC is now promising to accelerate the process of coding facial activities, especially for large samples or large amounts of data making the method more feasible for naturalistic assessments in and outside the laboratory. Because it is mainly unclear how classifiers are trained in AFC systems which are typically proprietary, we investigate the impact of training materials’ prototypicality and the consequent influence on classification performance.
AFC of standardized facial expressions
AFC software, as of today, typically entails two steps of analyzing faces [2, 5]: In the first step, physical properties of the pictures (here, AU features) are extracted, and in the second step, this information is integrated in terms of emotion categories. Regarding the first step, there is evidence of a high agreement between AFC and experienced human FACS coders [6–8] comparable to the inter-rater reliabilities between human FACS coders [9]. In the second step of AFC, developers face the challenge of deciding which and how many emotion categories should be predicted. Also, they need to choose the specific machine learning algorithm for the AFC software (for overviews, see [10, 11]). The so-called basic emotions (i.e., joy, sadness, anger, disgust, fear, and surprise) are the most extensively researched facial expression categories (e.g., [12]). They typically serve as predefined emotion categories for AFC’s supervised learning. Another challenge for new AFC tools is the fact that machine learning algorithms require a large amount of stimulus material (i.e., emotional faces) for their training, which is critical for the stability and generalizability of such systems. To access such datasets, AFC developers typically rely on standardized inventories of highly prototypical emotional facial expressions.
There is abundant evidence that AFC performs impressively in categorizing pictures from standardized inventories, typically comprising intense and prototypical emotional facial expressions (see Table 1). AFC classifies facial expressions of basic emotions with a high accuracy in standardized research inventories, which is a robust pattern for static picture inventories and dynamic video inventories [13–18]. However, decoding the facial expressions of skilled actors exaggerating certain emotions might not always correspond to actual emotional facial reactions that occur spontaneously. Hence, the exclusive use of such material for the development and validation of machine learning procedures can be deceiving in terms of the upper limit performance, in particular for these six basic emotions. Thus, it is essential to validate the sensitivity and specificity of AFC approaches for less intense and less prototypical facial expressions.
AFC of unstandardized facial expressions
Regarding AFC performance in less standardized pictures, we recently demonstrated substantial differences in facial movement (AU activity) between highly standardized and prototypical facial expressions compared to unstandardized and less prototypical emotional faces [19, 21]. Consequently, AFC accuracy rates substantially decrease evaluated in less prototypical stimuli (see Table 1) when untrained participants are instructed to mimic or pose facial expressions, particularly for emotion categories like sadness or fear [15, 20, 22]. Consistent with these results, we found that AFC parameters of standardized inventories and unstandardized facial expressions from untrained participants in a typical laboratory setting substantially differ in the relative intensity of AU activity, the resulting AU profiles, and overall classification accuracies. Furthermore, the classification performance of AFC decreases if spontaneous facial responses toward emotional stimuli like scenes or faces are investigated [23, 24]. Hence, the validity of AFC to detect emotional facial expressions is further decreased compared to prototypical facial expressions from standardized inventories.
One potential mechanism that causes this gap in AFC performance between prototypical and non-prototypical facial expressions may be the common usage of prototypical emotional facial expressions for the training of new AFC systems. In the past, AFC systems heavily relied on highly standardized static and dynamic facial expression inventories to both train and test their developed machine learning models [25–37]. Hence, the decrease in the accuracy of AFC to detect less intense and non-prototypical emotional facial expressions in more naturalistic research settings may indicate an overfit of trained AFC machine learning models to detect prototypical facial expressions.
Aims and overview
We set out to examine how the choice of specific material for training affects the accuracy of AFC machine learning algorithms to classify prototypical and non-prototypical emotional facial expressions. To this end, we used two datasets with pictures of emotional facial expressions (basic emotions: joy, sadness, anger, disgust, fear, and surprise) and extracted their AU intensity. One dataset comprises prototypical, standardized, and well-established research inventories (standardized dataset). The other comprises untrained study participants from a typical laboratory setting (unstandardized dataset), who were instructed to display emotional facial expressions. The aim was to investigate the influence of the prototypicality of training material on machine learning classification performance. To this end, we trained three machine learning algorithms based on the same set of AU parameters separately for both datasets (standardized vs. unstandardized) and tested their classification performance on both types of facial expressions. We expected a more accurate classification for the machine learning models trained and tested with matching datasets. Furthermore, we expect that the models which were trained with the standardized dataset would classify facial expressions from the unstandardized dataset less accurately. This study contributes to a better understanding of the inconsistent accuracies of AFC (see Table 1). Furthermore, it provides implications for the development and training procedure to approximate future AFC algorithms’ robustness and ecological validity.
Materials and methods
Datasets and facial expression analysis
The standardized dataset includes expressions from 69 women selected from widely used and standardized picture inventories (Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces, Warsaw Set of Emotional Facial Expression Pictures, Radboud Faces Database, [38–40]). The unstandardized dataset consists of expressions from 69 untrained female students who participated in an experiment in our laboratory (for details on the experimental procedure, see [21]). Participants were instructed to display emotional facial expressions cued by pictures of emotional facial expressions presented on a screen. Each dataset comprised pictures of 69 individuals who depicted posed facial expressions for the basic emotion categories joy, anger, surprise, sadness, disgust, fear, and neutral [41, 42].
Both datasets were processed with FaceReader (FR, Version 7.1, Noldus Information Technology) [43]. For each stimulus, the intensities of twenty Action Units were extracted (AU; AU01—Inner Brow Raiser, AU02—Outer Brow Raiser, AU04—Brow Lowerer, AU05—Upper Lid Raiser, AU06—Cheek Raiser, AU07—Lid Tightener, AU09—Nose Wrinkler, AU10—Upper Lid Raiser, AU15—Lip Corner Depressor, AU17—Chin Raiser, AU18—Lip Tightener, AU24—Lip Pressor, AU25—Lips Part, AU26—Jaw Drop, AU27—Mouth Stretch, AU43—Eyes Closed).
The FR classifies AU intensities with the following algorithmic pipeline [44, 45]: The face is located with a cascade classifier algorithm [46]. Textures of the face are normalized, and an active appearance model synthesizes a digital face model with over 500 location points [47]. Finally, compressed distance information is transmitted to an artificial neural network (ANN; [48]) that classifies the intensities of twenty AUs.
Two happy and one neutral picture from two actors in the standardized dataset were excluded because the FR did not reach convergence in the model fit, resulting in N = 480 observations. None were excluded from the unstandardized dataset (N = 483 observations).
The resulting AU activity scores for each emotional facial expression are predictors of our machine learning procedure, with the respective intended emotion as the prediction criterion. Both datasets are publicly available and completely anonymized and can be obtained from the https://madata.bib.uni-mannheim.de/327/ [49]. All lab participants contained in the unstandardized dataset provided written informed consent, and the experiment was approved by the University Mannheim Research Ethics Committee (EK Mannheim 09-3/2018) [21].
Selected algorithms and hyperparameters
We investigated the effect of the prototypicality (manifested as differences in the AU activity [21]) of the standardized and the unstandardized dataset on the accuracy of the emotion classification with the following three machine learning algorithms: decision tree [50], random forest [51] and multi-layer perceptron [52]. We optimized hyperparameters with a resampling method (10-fold, grouped cross-validation, see “Machine learning procedure”), maximizing kappa.
The following hyperparameters were optimized: complexity (cp) for the decision trees, the number of trees (ntree) and variables considered at each decision node (mtry) in the random forest, and the number of neurons in the hidden-layer (size) for the multi-layer perceptron. Hyperparameter tuning and the training of the models were conducted with the caret R-package [53, 54]. All further hyperparameters of the algorithms were set to their default values [54]. A complete list of the R-packages used in the analyses is provided in the S1 Table.
Decision trees.
We employed the rpart R-package [50] to train the decision tree. A decision tree is built by identifying the variable that bests splits the data into two groups (i.e., minimized impurity). This procedure is applied recursively to all generated subgroups and continued until the subgroup reaches a minimum size or no further improvement is possible. Such so-called greedy approaches will likely result in complex trees that will not generalize well on new data. To prevent overfitting, the resulting tree is thus, pruned by penalizing the number of terminal nodes. We tuned the complexity (cp) hyperparameter, which determines the minimum improvement necessary in a node split during the pruning process.
Random forest.
For the random forest (RF), we used the randomForest R-package [51]. The RF is a bagging (i.e., bootstrapping and aggregating) ensemble learning method in which multiple independent unpruned decision trees are trained from bootstrapped samples, and their results are aggregated, in case of a classification problem, with a majority vote [55, 56]. Further, to build rather uncorrelated trees, only a random subset of all predictors in the dataset is considered at each decision node in the process of creating the trees. This procedure is supposed to make the RF robust against overfitting [55]. We optimized two hyperparameters: the number of variables considered for each decision node (mtry) and the number of trees in the forest (ntree). In order to perform an extensive grid search for both hyperparameters, we used a slight modification of the RF implementation in caret [51] to simultaneously optimize both hyperparameters (mtry and ntree).
Multilayer perceptron.
We tested different numbers of hidden layers with no improvement in the model’s performance. Therefore, in the hyperparameter tuning phase, we only optimized the number of neurons in a single hidden layer (size) in order not to increase the risk of overfitting. The caret R-package [54] uses the RSNNS R-package [52] to train the multi-layer perceptron.
Machine learning procedure and performance evaluation
The machine learning pipeline is depicted in Fig 1 and was identical for both datasets (standardized dataset and the unstandardized dataset). We first extracted the activity of 20 AUs (see section Datasets) with Noldus FaceReader [43]. During the subsequent preprocessing step, two Action Units with zero variance (AU18 and AU27) were excluded. Both datasets were randomly split into a training (70%) and a test set (30%). As the data is inherently dependent (seven different emotional facial expressions per individual), the random split considered the unique identifier of the individual to prevent biased performance estimates introduced by data leakage (i.e., emotional facial expressions from a person being allocated to the train as well as the test dataset).
Note. Action Unit (AU) activity in both datasets was extracted with Noldus FaceReader (FR, Version 7.1). The training process for both datasets was identical: preprocessing, split into 70% training dataset and 30% test dataset, hyperparameter optimization with grouped 10-fold cross-validation in the training dataset. Models trained on each dataset were evaluated twice: once with the 30% hold-out test dataset from the corresponding training dataset and once with 100% of the respective other dataset. The picture for the standardized dataset is model AF33HAS from the KDEF database [38]. The exemplary picture for the unstandardized dataset symbolizes but is not taken from one of our anonymous participants; the model provided written informed consent.
During the training phase, the hyperparameters of the algorithms were optimized (maximizing kappa, κ) within the 70% training split with a grouped ten-fold cross-validation. Transformation of the data, i.e., scaling and centering, was managed inside the train function of the caret R-package [54] and independently applied to all cross-validation splits to avoid data leakage [57]. Grouping for the cross-validation was again based on the unique identifier of the individual. We decided to assess performance in an independent test set to prevent inflated performance metrics [57] due to optimization errors in cross-validations [58]. Consequently, the final models were trained with the optimal hyperparameters as previously determined by the cross-validation in the 70% training set.
In the evaluation phase, we first determined a baseline performance in previously unseen data from the same dataset that was used to train the models, i.e., with the hold-out 30% test set. Models trained with the standardized dataset were evaluated in the hold-out 30% test set of this dataset and vice versa; the models trained with the unstandardized dataset predicted the unseen hold-out 30% test set of the unstandardized dataset. Performance metrics obtained in the hold-out test sets from the same dataset provide an indicator of the model’s ability to predict facial expressions of similar prototypicality. Finally, to determine the influence of the specific training material on the performance, we used the models trained in one dataset to predict the respective other datasets. The models based on the standardized dataset were tested with the unstandardized dataset and vice versa. We evaluate the classification performance for all models in terms of overall accuracy, kappa (κ), and logLoss, as well as additional binary classification metrics separately for each emotion category: sensitivity (Sn), specificity (Sp), and the F1-score (F1). These binary classification metrics are calculated with a one-versus-all approach [54].
Results
Hyperparameter tuning and cross-validation
Optimal hyperparameters for each algorithm were determined with grouped 10-fold cross-validations (maximizing kappa). We tuned the complexity parameter (cp) for the decision tree and tested 100 values between 0 and 0.1614 with an interval step of 0.00163. The number of trees (ntree) and the variables considered at each split (mtry) were optimized for the random forest. We spun a grid with the following values: mtry = [1; 18] with steps of 1 and ntree = [100; 1000] with intervals of 100. We further optimized the numbers of neurons (size) in the single hidden layer of the multi-layer perceptron. In the tuning process, we considered values for size between 1 and 18, with intervals of 1. Additional hyperparameters of the algorithms (e.g., minbucket the minimum number of observations in a terminal node for the decision tree) are set to their default values; for more details, see rpart, randomForest, RSNNS documentation [50–52].
The optimal hyperparameters for models based on the standardized dataset were: cp = 0.1207546 (decision tree), mtry = 3 and ntree = 700 (random forest) and size = 11 (multi-layer perceptron). The optimal hyperparameters for models based on the unstandardized dataset were: cp = 0.006593715 (Decision Tree), mtry = 1 and ntree = 800 (random forest) and size = 17 (multi-layer perceptron). Table 2 presents the performance metrics obtained during the hyperparameter tuning for both datasets.
Classification performance of standardized dataset models
Train standardized–test standardized.
All models based on the standardized dataset predicted the hold-out data from the standardized dataset with high accuracy and few misclassifications (Fig 2, Panel A left; decision tree: Acc = 83.4%, κ = 0.81, logLoss = 2.71; random forest: Acc = 86.9%, κ = 0.85, logLoss = 0.39; multi-layer perceptron: Acc = 84.1%, κ = 0.81, logLoss = 0.84). With respect to the binary classification metrics, all emotions were classified with high F1-scores (decision tree: F1 = [76.6%; 97.7%]; random forest: F1 = [78.9%; 100%]; multi-layer perceptron F1 = [78.9%; 97.7%]).
Note. Overall classification accuracy for the models trained with the standardized dataset (Panel A) and the models trained with the unstandardized dataset (Panel B); for different test sets on the x-axis. Models were evaluated with either a hold-out test set from the same material or with the respective other dataset. Ntest = 147 in both test sets, Nstandardized = 480 in the standardized dataset, and Nunstandardized = 483 in the unstandardized dataset. Error bars represent the 95% confidence interval [59]. The no-information rate is depicted as a dotted line (14.3%). All accuracies were significantly higher than the no information rate (exact binomial test, all ps < .0001, 1-sided).
Train standardized–test unstandardized.
The same models (i.e., trained with the standardized dataset) classified the unstandardized dataset with substantially impaired accuracy (Fig 2, Panel A, right; decision tree: Acc = 69.8%, κ = 0.65, logLoss = 6.71; random forest: Acc = 65%, κ = 0.59, logLoss = 1.09; multi-layer perceptron: Acc = 52.8%, κ = 0.45, logLoss = 2.71). Substantially impaired classification performance for the models trained with the standardized dataset and evaluated in the unstandardized dataset is also evident in the low sensitivities for several emotion categories; see the diagonals in the confusion matrices (Fig 3, Panels D-F). All three models trained with the standardized dataset show overall reduced accuracies when tested with the less prototypical unstandardized dataset (decision tree: -13.6%, random forest: -21.9%, multi-layer perceptron: -31.3%). However, there are some specific differences in the classification performance of the models. For example, the random forest and the multi-layer perceptron models fail to correctly classify surprise in the unstandardized dataset (random forest: Sn = 14.5%, Sp = 100%, F1 = 25.3%; multi-layer perceptron: Sn = 17.4%, Sp = 99.5%, F1 = 28.9%). In contrast, the more basic decision tree classified surprise well (Sn = 85.5%, Sp = 84.1%, F1 = 60.8%), but it was less sensitive for joy (Sn = 34.8%). The multi-layer perceptron also classified joy with a reduced sensitivity (Sn = 52.2%). Common to all three models which were trained with standardized data is an increased misclassification of facial expression as neutral. Also, their sensitivities are reduced for fear (all Sn ≤ 49.3%) and disgust (all Sn ≤ 60.9%).
Note. Confusion matrices of the models trained with the standardized dataset. Left panels A–C performance in the hold-out data from the standardized dataset. Right panels D–F performance in the unstandardized dataset. The diagonal displays the sensitivity for each basic emotion. Darker color indicates higher frequencies for the specific predicted emotion.
Classification performance of unstandardized dataset models
Train unstandardized–test unstandardized.
All models based on the unstandardized dataset predicted the hold-out data from the unstandardized dataset with high accuracy and few misclassifications (see Fig 2, Panel B left; decision tree: Acc = 86.4%, κ = 0.84, logLoss = 2.84; random forest: Acc = 92.5%, κ = 0.91, logLoss = 0.43; multi-layer perceptron: Acc = 89.1%, κ = 0.87, logLoss = 0.54). Correspondingly, the F1-Scores for all emotion categories were high (Fig 4, Panel A-C; decision tree: F1 = [76.6%; 97.7%]; random forest: F1 = [78.9%; 100%]; multi-layer perceptron: F1 = [78.9%; 97.7%]).
Note. Confusion matrices of the models trained with the unstandardized dataset. Left panels A–C: performance in the hold-out data from the unstandardized dataset. Right panels D–F: performance in the standardized dataset. The diagonal displays the sensitivity for each basic emotion. Darker color indicates higher frequencies for the specific predicted emotion.
Train unstandardized–test standardized.
The same models classified the standardized data with only a slight decrease in the overall classification performance (Fig 2, Panel B right; decision tree: Acc = 82.7%, κ = 0.8, logLoss = 1.94; random forest: Acc = 86.9%, κ = 0.85, logLoss = 0.5; multi-layer perceptron: Acc = 84.6%, κ = 0.82, logLoss = 1.24). The F1-Scores for all three models trained with the unstandardized dataset remained high when evaluated with the standardized dataset (Fig 4, Panel D-F; decision tree: F1 = [71.6%; 95.7%]; random forest: F1 = [73.5%; 98.5%]; multi-layer perceptron: F1 = [73.5%; 98.5%]). For these models, anger is classified by all unstandardized models with slightly reduced sensitivities (all Sn ≥ 69.6%). Furthermore, the random forest model performs weakest in the classification of fear (Sn = 62.3%, Sp = 98.8%, F1 = 73.5%).
Discussion
Automatic facial coding is a promising new tool to efficiently analyze facial expressions in many research settings and prospectively also in naturalistic settings. However, recent AFC and computer vision developments have heavily relied on highly standardized emotion inventories to train and test the underlying machine learning models. Such prototypical facial expressions are referred to as the gold standard when new software is evaluated [60]. This is problematic because this may not say much about the performance on less intense and less prototypical facial expressions of emotions as they occur in typical laboratory settings or even in the natural environment [19–21]. Accordingly, the present study aimed to investigate the influence of the prototypicality of the picture material (standardized vs. unstandardized facial expressions) used to train machine learning algorithms on the classification of standardized and non-standardized emotional expressions. This is highly relevant for emotion researchers, who are primarily interested in the valid measurement of naturalistic facial expressions that are less intense and less prototypical [21, 24].
In the present study, machine learning models trained with expressions from either standardized inventories or unstandardized laboratory participants had excellent accuracies when tested on the same source of facial expressions. The recognition rates of the models are comparable to those of human raters and comparable to those reported for models evaluated in standardized research inventories (see Table 1) [60–62]. Accordingly, the models trained with standardized facial expressions predicted expressions from the same dataset with excellent performance metrics. However, these models predicted unstandardized facial expressions of untrained lab-study participants with substantially impaired classification performances, which is well in line with the performance reported by others (accuracy = [.21; .61], M = .48, [15, 19, 20]). Hence, models trained with standardized dataset did not generalize well to the less prototypical and less intense facial expressions in the unstandardized dataset. In contrast, models trained with the unstandardized dataset classified the prototypical expressions in the standardized dataset again with impressive performance metrics.
This implies that machine learning models benefit from training on less prototypical facial expressions, leading to higher accuracy and increasing the generalizability of the underlying model to more naturalistic facial expressions. Accordingly, such a training approach could improve the ecological validity of AFC systems because typical research participants are untrained and display less intense and less prototypical facial expressions.
While all three models trained with the standardized dataset and evaluated in the unstandardized dataset show reduced overall accuracy and difficulty detecting the emotions fear and disgust, there are also algorithm-specific deficits for some emotion categories. Compared to the more basic decision tree, the random forest and the multi-layer perceptron, which are considered more sophisticated algorithms, have a pronounced deficit in the classification of surprise. Even though the models trained with unstandardized facial expressions generalized well to the standardized dataset, we found lowered sensitivities, e.g., predicting fearful facial expressions. However, this is in line with previous research showing reduced sensitivities for fear not only for AFC systems [7, 20] but also for human emotion recognition [63–65].
Limitations and future directions
One limitation of our study may be the number of pictures used our machine learning pipeline, which is at the lower end of what is typically recommended for machine learning. Nevertheless, our machine learning models reached very high accuracies in unseen hold-out data, comparable to those in the field [19]. Furthermore, accuracies in the test sets were approaching the accuracies obtained during cross-validation in the training set; thus, there is no serious overfitting. Moreover, accuracies were very well comparable to a now considerable number of studies that relied on models that were trained on larger datasets by the developers of commercial software packages (see Table 1).
However, future studies should test a higher number of pictures and more datasets with further variation of the level of prototypicality in the expressed emotions to establish the generalizability of this result and build up a stock of available datasets with varying degrees of prototypicality in the facial expressions. Correspondingly, our datasets contained expressions from female, relatively young individuals of primarily European descent. Hence, future studies need to replicate our findings on more diverse samples to establish the generalizability of such algorithms regarding gender, age, and ethnicity [66–68].
Three pictures in the standardized dataset had to be excluded as the FaceReader was not able to localize the face. The FaceReader utilizes the viola-jones algorithm for face detection [46]. However, future developments could benefit from adapting newer methods of face detection, e.g., a recent paper described a promising approach of utilizing lightweight convolutional neural networks with ADAM optimization to locate faces in risk situations [69].
Important for future developments in this field could be a look beyond basic emotion categories. While these categories are thought to cover the majority of qualitatively different emotional experiences, there is evidence that facial expressions are much finer-grained and consist of a wider variety of meaningful facial expression categories (e.g.,[2, 70, 71]). In addition to more emotion categories, future studies should also consider new developments in emotion research and evaluate the classification of dimensional representations of emotions, such as valence (e.g., [72, 73]).
Our results demonstrate the relevance of evaluating machine learning algorithms on more naturalistic facial expressions which are different from standardized research inventories. More datasets with lay participants in a laboratory setting are needed to improve the training of underlying machine learning algorithms and optimize the performance of automatic facial coding. With more naturalistic datasets being available to the research community, the generalizability and accuracy of automatic facial coding approaches are expected to increase. Accordingly, the ability of AFC to detect subtle facial responses might improve and approach the sensitivity of other methods, such as facial EMG [74–76]. Higher generalizability and ecological validity of automatic facial coding software will likely lead to broader dissemination of this technology, which can bear ethical implications (e.g., when participants are unaware of being observed) that need to be addressed.
Conclusions
AFC is an innovative research tool to classify emotional facial expressions effectively. Compared to more traditional manual coding by humans, it is faster and can potentially be implemented in real-time recognition systems (e.g., smartphones [28]). However, AFC is substantially less accurate in unstandardized facial expressions. The present data support the conclusion that this gap in accuracy may be due to the prototypicality of the material used to train algorithms; AFC classifiers, as of today, are typically trained with highly standardized and intense facial expressions. Our results imply that models trained on standardized inventories do not generalize well to unstandardized facial expressions of untrained individuals whom we recruit as laboratory participants. However, models trained with unstandardized expressions performed substantially better when evaluated with frequently used standardized facial expressions from research inventories. Future developments in automatic facial coding will benefit from using less prototypical training materials to increase classification performance in more natural settings. Such a training approach will improve AFC systems’ robustness and ecological validity and contribute to the broader applicability of AFC tools in emotion research.
Acknowledgments
We thank Friedrich-Samuel Taubitz, Katharina Siebenhaar and Timo Kienzler for the valuable feedback on the manuscript.
References
- 1. Scherer KR, Ellgring H. Are facial expressions of emotion produced by categorical affect programs or dynamically driven by appraisal? Emotion. 2007;7: 113–130. pmid:17352568
- 2. Dalvi C, Rathod M, Patil S, Gite S, Kotecha K. A Survey of AI-Based Facial Emotion Recognition: Features, ML & DL Techniques, Age-Wise Datasets and Future Directions. IEEE Access. 2021;9: 165806–165840.
- 3. Plusquellec P, Denault V. The 1000 most cited papers on visible nonverbal behavior: A bibliometric analysis. J Nonverbal Behav. 2018;42: 347–377.
- 4.
Ekman P, Friesen W V., Hager JC. Facial action coding system. Manual and investigator’s guide. Salt Lake City, UT: Research Nexus; 2002.
- 5.
Mortillaro M, Meuleman B, Scherer KR. Automated recognition of emotion appraisals. Handbook of Research on Synthesizing Human Emotion in Intelligent Systems and Robotics. IGI Global; 2015. pp. 338–551.
- 6. Bartlett MS, Hager JC, Ekman P, Sejnowski TJ. Measuring facial expressions by computer image analysis. Psychophysiology. 1999;36: 253–263. pmid:10194972
- 7. Skiendziel T, Rösch AG, Schultheiss OC. Assessing the convergent validity between the automated emotion recognition software Noldus FaceReader 7 and facial action coding system scoring. PLoS One. 2019;14: 1–18. pmid:31622426
- 8.
Tian Ying-li, Kanade T, Cohn JF. Recognizing lower face action units for facial expression analysis. Proceedings Fourth IEEE International Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition. IEEE Comput. Soc; 2001. pp. 484–490. https://doi.org/10.1109/AFGR.2000.840678
- 9. Sayette MA, Cohn JF, Wertz JM, Perrott MA, Parrott DJ. A psychometric evaluation of the facial action coding system for assessing spontaneous expression. J Nonverbal Behav. 2001;25: 167–185.
- 10. Alexandre GR, Soares JM, Thé GAP. Systematic review of 3D facial expression recognition methods. Pattern Recognit. 2020;100: 107108.
- 11. Oh YH, See J, Le Ngo AC, Phan RCW, Baskaran VM. A survey of automatic facial micro-expression analysis: Databases, methods, and challenges. Front Psychol. 2018;9. pmid:30042706
- 12. Feldman Barrett L, Adolphs R, Marsella S, Martinez AM, Pollak SD. Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychol Sci Public Interes. 2019;20: 1–68. pmid:31313636
- 13. Calvo MG, Fernández-Martín A, Recio G, Lundqvist D. Human observers and automated assessment of dynamic emotional facial expressions: KDEF-dyn database validation. Front Psychol. 2018;9: 1–12. pmid:30416473
- 14. Mavadati SM, Mahoor MH, Bartlett K, Trinh P, Cohn JF. DISFA: A spontaneous facial action intensity database. IEEE Trans Affect Comput. 2013;4: 151–160.
- 15. Yitzhak N, Giladi N, Gurevich T, Messinger DS, Prince EB, Martin K, et al. Gently does it: Humans outperform a software classifier in recognizing subtle, nonstereotypical facial expressions. Emotion. 2017;17: 1187–1198. pmid:28406679
- 16. Zhang X, Yin L, Cohn JF, Canavan S, Reale M, Horowitz A, et al. BP4D-Spontaneous: A high-resolution spontaneous 3D dynamic facial expression database. Image Vis Comput. 2014;32: 692–706.
- 17. Lewinski P. Automated facial coding software outperforms people in recognizing neutral faces as neutral from standardized datasets. Front Psychol. 2015;6: 1–6. pmid:26441761
- 18. Lewinski P, den Uyl TM, Butler C. Automated facial coding: Validation of basic emotions and FACS AUs in FaceReader. J Neurosci Psychol Econ. 2014;7: 227.
- 19. Küntzler T, Höfling TTA, Alpers GW. Automatic facial expression recognition in standardized and non-standardized emotional expressions. Front Psychol. 2021;12: 1–13. pmid:34025503
- 20. Stöckli S, Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Borer S, Samson AC. Facial expression analysis with AFFDEX and FACET: A validation study. Behav Res Methods. 2018;50: 1446–1460. pmid:29218587
- 21. Höfling TTA, Alpers GW, Büdenbender B, Föhl U, Gerdes ABM. What’s in a face: Automatic facial coding of untrained study participants compared to standardized inventories. Hadjileontiadis LJ, editor. PLoS One. 2022;17: e0263863. pmid:35239654
- 22. Sato W, Hyniewska S, Minemoto K, Yoshikawa S. Facial expressions of basic emotions in Japanese laypeople. Front Psychol. 2019;10: 259. pmid:30809180
- 23. Höfling TTA, Gerdes ABM, Föhl U, Alpers GW. Read my face: Automatic facial coding versus psychophysiological indicators of emotional valence and arousal. Front Psychol. 2020;11: 1–15. pmid:32636788
- 24. Höfling TTA, Alpers GW, Gerdes ABM, Föhl U. Automatic facial coding versus electromyography of mimicked, passive, and inhibited facial response to emotional faces. Cogn Emot. 2021;35: 874–889. pmid:33761825
- 25. Burkert P, Trier F, Afzal MZ, Dengel A, Liwicki M. DeXpression: Deep convolutional neural network for expression recognition. 2015; 1–8.
- 26.
Fathallah A, Abdi L, Douik A. Facial expression recognition via deep learning. 2017 IEEE/ACS 14th International Conference on Computer Systems and Applications (AICCSA). IEEE; 2017. pp. 745–750. https://doi.org/10.1109/AICCSA.2017.124
- 27. Shih FY, Chuang C-F, Wang PSP. Performance comparisons of facial expression recognition in JAFFE database. Int J Pattern Recognit Artif Intell. 2008;22: 445–459.
- 28. Song I, Kim HJ, Jeon PB. Deep learning for real-time robust facial expression recognition on a smartphone. Dig Tech Pap—IEEE Int Conf Consum Electron. 2014; 564–567.
- 29. Wang Z, Ying Z. Facial expression recognition based on adaptive local binary pattern and sparse representation. CSAE 2012—Proceedings, 2012 IEEE International Conference on Computer Science and Automation Engineering. 2012. pp. 440–444.
- 30. Gargesha M, Kuchi P, Torkkola K. Facial expression recognition using artificial neural networks. Artif Neural Comput Syst. 2002. pp. 1–6.
- 31. Lajevardi SM, Hussain ZM. Feature extraction for facial expression recognition based on hybrid face regions. Adv Electr Comput Eng. 2009;9: 63–67.
- 32. Lee Kuang-Chih, Ho J Kriegman DJ. Acquiring linear subspaces for face recognition under variable lighting. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell. 2005;27: 684–698. pmid:15875791
- 33. Liu P, Han S, Meng Z, Tong Y. Facial expression recognition via a boosted deep belief network. Proc IEEE Comput Soc Conf Comput Vis Pattern Recognit. 2014; 1805–1812.
- 34.
Liu W, Song C, Wang Y. Facial expression recognition based on discriminative dictionary learning. Proceedings—International Conference on Pattern Recognition. IEEE; 2012. pp. 1839–1842.
- 35. Mollahosseini A, Chan D, Mahoor MH. Going deeper in facial expression recognition using deep neural networks. 2016 IEEE Winter Conf Appl Comput Vision, WACV 2016. 2016; 1–10.
- 36. Raj D. A realtime face recognition system using PCA and various distance classifiers. CS676 Comput Vis Image Process. 2011.
- 37. Rejila RC, Monisha M. Automatic facial expression recognition based on the salient facial patches. Int J Sci Technol Eng. 2016;2: 772–780.
- 38. Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A. The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). CD ROM from Dep Clin Neurosci Psychol Sect Karolinska Inst. 1998;91.
- 39. Olszanowski M, Pochwatko G, Kuklinski K, Scibor-Rylski M, Lewinski P, Ohme RK, et al. Warsaw set of emotional facial expression pictures: A validation study of facial display photographs. Front Psychol. 2015;5. pmid:25601846
- 40. Langner O, Dotsch R, Bijlstra G, Wigboldus DHJ, Hawk ST, van Knippenberg A. Presentation and validation of the radboud faces database. Cogn Emot. 2010;24: 1377–1388.
- 41.
Ekman P. Facial expression. In: Dols JMF, Russell JA, editors. The Science of Facial Expression. Cambridge: University Press; 2017. pp. 39–56.
- 42. Ekman P, Levenson RW, Friesen W V. Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science (80-). 1983;221: 1208–1210. pmid:6612338
- 43.
Noldus Information Technology. FaceReader Version 7.1 Reference Manual. Wageningen: Noldus Information Technology; 2017.
- 44. van Kuilenburg H, den Uyl MJ, Israël ML, Ivan P. Advances in face and gesture analysis. Proceedings of 6th International Conference on Methods and Techniques in Behavioral Research. 2008. pp. 371–372.
- 45.
van Kuilenburg H, Wiering M, den Uyl MJ. A model based method for automatic facial expression recognition. Proceedings of the 16th European Conference on Machine Learning. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2005. pp. 194–205.
- 46. Viola P, Jones MJ. Robust Real-Time Face Detection PAUL. Int J Comput Vis. 2004;57: 137–154.
- 47.
Cootes TF, Taylor CJ. Statistical models of appearance for computer vision. Unpubl manuscript, Wolfson Image Anal Unit, Imaging Sci Biomed Eng Univ Manchester, United Kingdom. 2004. Available: http://personalpages.manchester.ac.uk/staff/timothy.f.cootes/Models/app_models.pdf
- 48.
Bishop CM. Neural networks for pattern recognition. Reprinted. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.; 2010.
- 49.
Höfling TTA, Alpers GW, Büdenbender B, Gerdes ABM, Föhl U. What’s in a face: Automatic facial coding of basic emotional expressions of untrained participants compared to trained actors [Data file]. University of Mannheim; 2020. https://doi.org/10.7801/327
- 50. Theranau TM, Atkinson EJ. An introduction to recursive partitioning using the RPART routines. 1997.
- 51. Liaw A, Wiener M. Classification and regression by randomForest. R news. 2002;2: 18–22.
- 52. Bergmeir C, Benítez JM. Neural networks in R using the stuttgart neural networksimulator: RSNNS. J Stat Softw. 2012;46: 1–26.
- 53. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: [Computer software]; 2021. Available: https://cran.r-project.org/
- 54. Kuhn M. Building predictive models in R using the caret package. J Stat Softw. 2008;28: 1–26.
- 55. Breiman L. Random forests. Mach Learn. 2001;45: 5–32.
- 56. Breiman L. Bagging predictors. Mach Learn. 1996;24: 123–140.
- 57. Varma S, Simon R. Bias in error estimation when using cross-validation for model selection. BMC Bioinformatics. 2006;7: 1–8. pmid:16504092
- 58.
Kuhn M, Johnson K. Applied predictive modeling. Vol. 26. New York: Springer; 2013.
- 59. Clopper CJ, Pearson ES. The use of confidence or fiducial limits illustrated in the case of the binomial. Biometrika. 1934;26: 404.
- 60. Dupré D, Krumhuber EG, Küster D, McKeown GJ. A performance comparison of eight commercially available automatic classifiers for facial affect recognition. PLoS One. 2020;15: 1–17. pmid:32330178
- 61. Valstar MF, Mehu M, Bihan Jiang, Pantic M, Scherer K. Meta-Analysis of the first facial expression recognition challenge. IEEE Trans Syst Man, Cybern Part B. 2012;42: 966–979. pmid:22736651
- 62. Ko BC. A brief review of facial emotion recognition based on visual information. Sensors. 2018;18. pmid:29385749
- 63. Calvo MG, Lundqvist D. Facial expressions of emotion (KDEF): Identification under different display-duration conditions. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40: 109–115. pmid:18411533
- 64. Palermo R, Coltheart M. Photographs of facial expression: Accuracy, response times, and ratings of intensity. Behav Res Methods, Instruments, Comput. 2004;36: 634–638. pmid:15641409
- 65. Eisenbarth H, Alpers GW, Segrè D, Calogero A, Angrilli A. Categorization and evaluation of emotional faces in psychopathic women. Psychiatry Res. 2008;159: 189–195. pmid:18423614
- 66. Stinson C. Algorithms are not neutral. AI Ethics. 2022. pmid:35128540
- 67. Giovanola B, Tiribelli S. Beyond bias and discrimination: redefining the AI ethics principle of fairness in healthcare machine-learning algorithms. AI Soc. 2022. pmid:35615443
- 68. Buolamwini J, Gebru T. Gender Shades: Intersectional Accuracy Disparities in Commercial Gender Classification. Conference on Fairness, Accountability, and Transparency. 2018. pp. 77–91.
- 69. Wieczorek M, Silka J, Wozniak M, Garg S, Hassan MM. Lightweight Convolutional Neural Network Model for Human Face Detection in Risk Situations. IEEE Trans Ind Informatics. 2022;18: 4820–4829.
- 70. Keltner D, Cordaro DT. Understanding multimodal emotional expression: Recent advances in basic emotion theory. Sci facial Expr. 2017; 57–75.
- 71. Sauter DA. The nonverbal communication of positive emotions: An emotion family approach. Emot Rev. 2017;9: 222–234. pmid:28804510
- 72. Scherer KR, Mortillaro M, Mehu M. Understanding the mechanisms underlying the production of facial expression of emotion: A componential perspective. Emot Rev. 2013;5: 47–53.
- 73. Scherer KR, Mortillaro M, Rotondi I, Sergi I, Trznadel S. Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2018;114: 358–379. pmid:29461080
- 74. Neumann R, Schulz SM, Lozo L, Alpers GW. Automatic facial responses to near-threshold presented facial displays of emotion: Imitation or evaluation? Biol Psychol. 2014;96: 144–149. pmid:24370542
- 75. Eisenbarth H, Gerdes ABM, Alpers GW. Motor-incompatibility of facial reactions. J Psychophysiol. 2011;25: 124–130.
- 76. Gerdes ABM, Wieser MJ, Alpers GW, Strack F, Pauli P. Why do you smile at me while I’m in pain?—Pain selectively modulates voluntary facial muscle responses to happy faces. Int J Psychophysiol. 2012;85: 161–167. pmid:22705169