Figures
Abstract
Health care policies often rely on public cooperation, especially during a health crisis. However, a crisis is also a period of uncertainty and proliferation of health-related advice: while some people adhere to the official recommendations, others tend to avoid them and resort to non-evidence based, pseudoscientific practices. People prone to the latter are often the ones endorsing a set of epistemically suspect beliefs, with two being particularly relevant: conspiratorial pandemic-related beliefs, and the appeal to nature bias regarding COVID-19 (i.e., trusting natural immunity to fight the pandemic). These in turn are rooted in trust in different epistemic authorities, seen as mutually exclusive: trust in science and trust in the “wisdom of the common man”. Drawing from two nationally representative probability samples, we tested a model in which trust in science/wisdom of the common man predicted COVID-19 vaccination status (Study 1, N = 1001) or vaccination status alongside use of pseudoscientific health practices (Study 2, N = 1010), through COVID-19 conspiratorial beliefs and the appeal to nature bias regarding COVID-19. As expected, epistemically suspect beliefs were interrelated, related to vaccination status, and to both types of trust. Moreover, trust in science had both a direct and indirect effect on vaccination status through both types of epistemically suspect beliefs. Trust in the wisdom of the common man had only an indirect effect on vaccination status. Contrary to the way they are typically portrayed, the two types of trust were unrelated. These results were largely replicated in the second study, in which we added pseudoscientific practices as an outcome; trust in science and the wisdom of the common man contributed to their prediction only indirectly, through epistemically suspect beliefs. We offer recommendations on how to make use of different types of epistemic authorities and how to tackle unfounded beliefs in communication during a health crisis.
Citation: Žeželj I, Petrović M, Ivanović A, Kurčubić P (2023) I trust my immunity more than your vaccines: “Appeal to nature” bias strongly predicts questionable health behaviors in the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE 18(2): e0279122. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0279122
Editor: Muhammad Shahzad Aslam, Xiamen University - Malaysia Campus: Xiamen University - Malaysia, MALAYSIA
Received: January 11, 2022; Accepted: November 29, 2022; Published: February 22, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Žeželj et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are available on OSF: https://osf.io/5fuzn/.
Funding: The authors received no specific funding for this work.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
"But if you’re like 21 years old, and you say to me, ‘Should I get vaccinated?’ I’ll go, ‘No. Are you healthy? Are you a healthy person? Like, look, don’t do anything stupid, but you should take care of yourself. If you’re a healthy person, and you’re exercising all the time, and you’re young, and you’re eating well, I don’t think you need to worry about this."
Joe Rogan at The Joe Rogan Experience, April 23, 2021
Introduction
People make hundreds of decisions per day; many of them are directly or indirectly health-related [1]. Furthermore, often they not only decide for themselves, but their decisions impact other people’s health. Implementing public health policies typically requires individual cooperation [2]. Such cooperation, however, is rarely unanimous. There is always a portion of individuals who avoid following the recommended practices; the same people sometimes resort to non-evidence-based traditional, complementary and alternative (TM/CAM) health practices (see [3] for a review). In this paper, we investigate the relations between these two broad types of questionable health behaviors and their psychological roots.
The reason why not adhering to official medical recommendations is detrimental to public health is rather self-evident: it leads to increased mortality rates [4], spread of infectious diseases [5], and economic burden on healthcare systems [6]. The World Health Organization reports that prevalence of non-adherence for chronic diseases is as high as 50% [7], and vaccine hesitancy and antibiotic overuse were listed in top 10 global health risks even before the pandemic [8]. On the other hand, while often perceived as harmless, non-evidence-based TM/CAM practices can be just the opposite: acupuncture may cause infections, alternative diets can cause malnutrition, chiropractic spinal manipulations can cause a stroke, herbal remedies can cause liver damage [9]. The onset of the pandemic only saw an increase of such non-evidence-based pseudoscientific advice [10]. It became so pervasive, that WHO had to set up a “mythbusters” website page devoted to debunking different non-evidence-based practices for combating COVID-19 [11], from constantly sipping liquid, rinsing nose with saline solutions, traditional cures such as consuming garlic, honey or hot peppers, to drinking methanol, ethanol or bleach.
Furthermore, non-evidence-based TM/CAM practices can also be accompanied by delay or avoidance of official treatment: opting for a TM/CAM treatment is related to increased mortality of cancer patients [12]. They can also interact with official treatment, proven risky for cancer patients [13] and HIV patients [14]. There is anecdotal evidence that people prone to these practices are less likely to follow recommended ones when it comes to the COVID-19 pandemic as well: for example, Dr Nestorović, a renown Serbian pulmonologist and a vocal public figure famously revealed that he does not need the COVID-19 vaccine because he keeps his immunity strong with a quince jam and homemade brandy combination [15]; Joe Rogan, the host of one the most popular US podcasts “The Joe Rogan Experience”, was also vocal about recommending young people who “work on their immunity” by exercising regularly, eating healthy and taking supplements not to take the vaccine [16]. We argue that these health behaviors can be traced back to a set of so-called “epistemically suspect” beliefs [17], and we especially focus on two: pandemic-related conspiracy theories and the appeal to nature bias.
Conspiracy theories and health behaviors
As conspiracy theorizing always involves questioning the official versions of events [18], it should also translate to questioning the intentions of the officials, including their health recommendations. There is ample evidence that believing in conspiracy theories is related to less adherence to recommended health practices, such as less frequent physical and dental checkups and use of sunscreen [19]. Exposure to anti-vaccine conspiracy theories has been shown to decrease vaccination intentions [20]. During the 2020–21 COVID-19 pandemic, pandemic-related conspiracy beliefs have repeatedly been linked to vaccine hesitancy (see [21] for a review).
Conspiracy mentality was also shown to be positively related to the appreciation of alternative medicine in general [22], and people more prone to believing in conspiracy theories endorsed CAM products more than those less prone to it [23,24]. There is also scattered evidence that those who endorse pandemic-related conspiracy beliefs are also more likely to believe that pseudoscientific cures, such as consuming megadoses of garlic or vitamins, sipping ginger potions or drinking colloidal silver can prevent or cure COVID-19 [25]. Not only that, but believing in conspiracy theories about the pandemic also leads to more frequent use of the same practices [26].
Appeal to nature and health behaviors
David Hume famously argued that an ‘ought’ cannot be logically derived from an ‘is’, and ‘ought not’ derived from ‘is not’ [27]. Following this observation, Moore warned that there is no merit in explaining something as being good reductively, evoking its natural properties such as desirable or pleasant, i.e., that doing so would be committing a naturalistic fallacy [28]. It has since been used in a broader sense, as the idea that what is found in nature is necessarily good/beneficial (and what is not is bad/harmful), and sometimes labeled as the “appeal to nature” bias [29]. This bias was observed to be central for a number of anti-scientific attitudes and behaviors, such as rejecting GMO food [30] or opposing in vitro fertilization [31]. There are authors (e.g. [32]) claiming that this bias is even embedded in the official legislation, for example in the EU regulations on organic food products.
The appeal to nature bias is also very present in anti-vaccinal narratives, either through claims that vaccine-induced immunity is inferior to natural immunity, acquired post-disease, or through claims that vaccines are unnecessary if one maintains their immunity by healthy life habits [33,34]. Similar tropes were evoked on social media during the COVID-19 pandemic, with users claiming that natural immunity is “99.9% effective” or their immune system is “99.4% effective” against the virus, alluding to vaccine efficacy [35].
Trust in epistemic authorities
Digging even deeper, these beliefs can be rooted in trust in different epistemic authorities–science, on the one hand, and the so-called “wisdom of the common man” on the other.
Even before the pandemic, there were warnings that growth of science denialism threatens public health [36], and in a similar vein, during the pandemic, distrust in science was repeatedly reported to be a strong predictor of compliance with health guidelines [37,38]. Science skepticism was also correlated with more willingness to spread COVID-19 conspiracies [39] and related misinformation [40].
There is less clarity about the role of trust in the wisdom of the common man in questionable health practices. In the realm of US political science, this construct is sometimes considered a manifestation of anti-intellectualism, in which it represents a substitute for formal knowledge and expertise [41]. In this view, experts are seen as elite, too distanced from average people, exhibiting certain arrogance and patronizing attitudes [42]. Such anti-intellectualism and turning to the plain wisdom of the common man were described as the core of the public’s defiant response to the pandemic [43]. As it is described, however, this is a very (north)American phenomenon, and it has not been cross culturally validated. It is typically contrasted to trust in science, or even measured simply as lack of trust in scientific authorities (as in [43]). The relation between trust in science and trust in folk wisdom could, nevertheless, be more complex. They could be unrelated, but also positively related (even if it seems contradictory), in a so-called polyphasic or “high entropy” representational field [44,45].
A similar, albeit narrower concept of medical folk wisdom has been related to devaluing medical expertise, but not to health behaviors [46], and there is no evidence of predictiveness of trust in common man for pandemic health behavior, neither adhering to recommendations nor resorting to alternative practices.
There is reason to assume that less trust in official authorities would leave people more open to alternative scenarios—conspiratorial beliefs are by definition alternatives to the official ones, and, in the pandemic, the official authorities were clear that relying on natural immunity would not suffice in fighting the virus. These beliefs could, in turn, make it more probable for a person to not adhere to official recommendations, i.e. vaccination, and to resort to other, alternative health practices. As previously discussed, it is less clear what the consequences of more trust in wisdom in common man would be. Drawing from the existing empirical evidence (albeit only from the US and thus maybe not culturally universal), one might assume it could follow a similar path: more trust in the wisdom of the common man could be related to more conspiratorial beliefs and higher endorsement of the appeal to nature bias and thus to more questionable health practices.
We first set out to explore how trust in science and the wisdom of the common man, and the two types of epistemically suspect beliefs–conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19 and the appeal to nature bias–relate to each other, and how they relate to a. the current COVID-19 vaccination status and b. the use of pseudoscientific health practices as protection against COVID-19. We further tested whether these two types of trust impacted vaccination status/pseudoscientific practices through the two types of epistemically suspect beliefs.
Study 1
In the first study, we measured only vaccination status as an outcome. We expected the two types of epistemically suspect beliefs–conspiratorial beliefs and the appeal to nature bias–to be positively related (H1). Based on considerations of the two epistemic authorities and their relationship in previous research, we expected that the two types of trust–in science and in the wisdom of the common man–would be negatively related (H2). Finally, we hypothesized that trust in science and trust in the wisdom of the common man would contribute to prediction of vaccination status through conspiratorial beliefs (H3a) and through the appeal to nature bias (H3b).
Methods
Ethics statement.
The study was approved by the Institutional review board of the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade, Serbia (protocol number: #2021–001); the participants provided their written consent. The protocol strictly followed the Declaration of Helsinki, and the data was collected and stored abiding by the GDPR regulations (for detailed IPSOS data policy consult: https://www.ipsos.com/en/privacy-data-protection)
Sampling and data collection procedure.
Data collection was conducted through Ipsos Serbia’s monthly omnibus research, during May 2021, on a total of 1001 respondents, representative of the adult population of Serbia (95% confidence interval for incidence of 50% on the sample size of 1000 is +/- 3.2%). We used a mixed-mode approach—80% of the data was collected via face-to-face computer-assisted interviews, while the other 20% was collected via online interviews through the Ipsos Online Panel. It was a stratified three-staged probability sample, with the target population of citizens of Serbia aged over 18 years (the sampling frame was based on the data from the 2011 population census). It was stratified by the type of settlement, and by six geo-economical regions. The data was weighted by education to better match the population.
Table 1 details the socio-demographic breakdown of the sample, as well as the official population estimates for Serbia.
Materials
Belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories was assessed via two items: “The so-called COVID-19 pandemic is nothing more than a smokescreen for covert actions of powerful forces” and “It is clear that the pharmaceutical industry is behind this pandemic”. The participants assessed the truthfulness of the items on a 9-point Likert type scale, ranging from 1 (completely false) to 9 (completely true). The score was calculated as a mean of answers on the two items. We had additionally included a negatively reflected item “I think the public is correctly informed about the origins of the coronavirus”, however, we excluded it from the final score since it had a poor corrected item-total correlation (r = .159). Upon exclusion, the internal consistency of the scale was good α = .75.
Appeal to nature bias regarding COVID-19. The appeal to nature bias was assessed with two items: “COVID-19 can be beaten with natural remedies and nutrition.” and “The coronavirus is harmless for those with a strong immunity”. The participants indicated the truthfulness of the items on a scale ranging from 1 (completely false) to 9 (completely true). The internal consistency of the two items (α = .66), allowed us to average them in the final score.
Trust in science. Trust in science was assessed with one item: “When it comes to dealing with the coronavirus and the knowledge about it, how much trust do you have in science and scientific findings?”. The participants answered using a 9-point scale, from 1 (I do not trust it at all) to 9 (I trust it completely).
Trust in the wisdom of the common man. Trust in the common man was assessed with the item “When it comes to dealing with the coronavirus and the knowledge about it, how much trust do you have in the wisdom of the common man?”. Answers were given on a scale ranging from 1 (I do not trust it at all) to 9 (I trust it completely).
Vaccination status. We assessed vaccination status by asking the participants “Did you get vaccinated against COVID-19 (with at least one dose)?”. Participants answered using a scale from 1 (No, and I will surely not get vaccinated), 2 (No, and I will probably not get vaccinated), 3 (No, and I am not sure if I will get vaccinated or not), 4 (No, but I will probably get vaccinated), 5 (No, but I will surely get vaccinated), 6 (No, but I have signed up for vaccination1) to 7 (Yes). (1 At this point in time, it was required to sign up to get vaccinated in Serbia in order to get a vaccination appointment.) We additionally included a category for those for whom the vaccine is not recommended. This category made up 5.8% of the sample and we coded these answers as system missing. We opted for a 7-point scale rather than a binary one given that the study was done mid-year 2021 when the vaccines were still becoming more widely available. As such, those who had a strong intent to get vaccinated might not have logistically been able to do so yet, and could not thus be lumped together with those who strongly opposed getting vaccinated against COVID-19. It was important to have those differences among the ones not (yet) vaccinated reflected in the measure given the particular context. We label the measure “vaccination status” to more precisely illustrate its nature, given that it is a combination of a behavioral and intentional measure.
Results
All materials, data and syntax are available on Open Science Framework (OSF) project page: https://osf.io/5fuzn/.
Endorsement of COVID-19-related epistemically suspect beliefs
We first examined the population prevalence of the epistemically suspect beliefs (Table 2 details the distribution of answers).
Conspiracy theories about the pandemic were endorsed by a significant proportion of the population, with more than a third finding them more likely to be true than not. Similarly, the appeal to nature bias, while slightly less present than conspiratorial beliefs, was also observed in more than a third of the population. Overall, this suggests that, while not prevalent, epistemically suspect beliefs are far from marginalized.
Trust in science and the wisdom of the common man
Table 2 also details the distribution of answers regarding trust in two different epistemic authorities—science and the wisdom of the common man.
Overall, trust in science was high, with a vast majority of the population indicating that they trust that science is able to deal with the pandemic, and only 12 percent indicating the opposite. Trust in the wisdom of the common man was more evenly distributed: 40% claimed to trust it, whilst 33% did not. Additionally, the pattern of results suggested that the appeal to nature bias was more present in younger people, who also tended to trust science less (for full breakdown consult S1 Appendix).
Relations between epistemically suspect beliefs, trust and vaccination status
Next, we examined the relations between belief in COVID-19 related conspiracy theories, the appeal to nature bias, trust in science and the wisdom of the common man and vaccination status (Table 3). We transformed all of the variables using the Rankit rank-based normalization for subsequent analyses [47], but we report non-transformed descriptives for ease of interpretation.
As Table 3 suggests, vaccination status was meaningfully related to all other constructs. As expected, it was most strongly related to the appeal to nature bias and trust in science: negatively to the former and positively to the latter; it was negatively albeit very weakly related to trust in the wisdom of the common man. Additionally, also in line with expectations (H1), belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories and the appeal to nature bias were moderately positively related, whilst both were correlated with the two types of trust: negatively with science and positively with the wisdom of the common man. Contrary to our expectations (H2), the two types of trust were not related to each other.
Mediation analyses. All mediation analyses were run using JASP version 0.16 [48]. We used the maximum likelihood estimator, after performing a listwise deletion of the missing data, given that it was not missing at random. We estimated the confidence intervals using the bootstrap bias-corrected percentile method.
In the first model (Fig 1), we tested whether the two types of trust predict vaccination status through belief in conspiracy theories. Epistemically suspect beliefs only partially mediated the relationship between trust in science and vaccination status, partially in line with our expectations (H3a). The relation of trust in the wisdom of the common man and vaccination status was, as expected (H3b), fully mediated by epistemically suspect beliefs. Trust in science contributed both directly (β = .400, 95% CI [.320, .475]; p < .001) and indirectly through belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories (β = .074, 95% CI [.048, .104]; p < .001) to the prediction of vaccination status. On the other hand, trust in the wisdom of the common man had no direct effect on vaccination status (β = -.040, 95% CI [-.108, .024]; p = .220), but contributed to its prediction via belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories (β = -.052, 95% CI [-.077, -.033]; p < .001). The model explained a total of 19% of variance in the vaccination status.
Note. The paths are shown with standardized estimates. Non-significant paths are shown with dashed lines.
When we entered the appeal to nature bias as the mediator in the model (Fig 2), a similar pattern of results emerged. Trust in the wisdom of the common man contributed to the prediction solely through the appeal to nature bias (β = -.099, 95% CI [-.130, -.069]; p < .001), while trust in science contributed both directly (β = .363, 95% CI [.284, .441]; p < .001) and indirectly (β = .111, 95% CI [.081, .149]; p < .001). In total, the model explained 25% of variance in vaccination status.
Note. The paths are shown with standardized estimates. Non-significant paths are shown with dashed lines.
Study 2
As our main goal was to explore psychological roots of questionable health practices, we wanted to replicate the findings of the first study, and extend it by including another health outcome in a new sample. To this end, we assessed the frequency of use of different non-evidence-based treatments that were advertised to build protection against the COVID-19. As these types of remedies are often sold as “natural solutions” and/or “traditional cures” [9,49], its use is even more likely to be related to the appeal to nature bias, and further to trust in the wisdom of the common man as an epistemic authority.
We once again expected trust in science and the wisdom of the common man to have an indirect effect on vaccination status through both conspiratorial beliefs (H1a) and the appeal to nature bias (H1b). As for the use of pseudoscientific practices, we hypothesized that both types of trust would contribute to its prediction through conspiratorial beliefs (H2a) and the appeal to nature bias (H2b). Finally, we expected the appeal to nature bias to be a stronger predictor of the use of pseudoscientific practices than the endorsement of conspiratorial beliefs (H3).
Methods
Sampling and data collection procedure.
We followed the same sampling and data collection procedure as in Study 1. The sample was representative of the adult population of Serbia with a total of N = 1010 participants; the data was collected in June 2021. Table 4 details the socio-demographic breakdown of the sample, alongside official population estimates for 2021.
Materials.
Use of pseudoscientific practices. The use of pseudoscientific practices was assessed via the frequency of engagement in three types of pseudoscientific practices for protection against the coronavirus: (1) eating garlic, (2) drinking healing concoctions (such as ginger tea, lemon with baking soda, or similar drinks) and (3) eating honey or other bee products. These practices were selected because they were among the most common ones in Serbia based on a previous study [26]. Participants indicated how often they partook in these activities during the pandemic in order to protect themselves from the coronavirus on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). We calculated the score by taking the mean of answers on all three items. The scale’s internal consistency was satisfactory α = .65.
We also assessed belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories (α = .75), the appeal to nature bias regarding COVID-19 (α = .61) and vaccination status using the same measures as in Study 1.
Results
All materials, data and syntax are available on the OSF project page: https://osf.io/5fuzn/ https://osf.io/5fuzn/?view_only=7ad27e0f55364bc28498ec6a6a234087.
Endorsement of COVID-19-related epistemically suspect beliefs
As in Study 1, conspiracy beliefs were more present in the population compared to the appeal to nature bias, but both types of beliefs were slightly less endorsed than in Study 1 (Table 5).
Trust in science and the wisdom of the common man
Table 5 also details the percentage of the population that has (vs. the percentage that does not have) trust in science and the wisdom of the common man.
Compared to Study 1, trust in science remained high, while trust in the wisdom of the common man was slightly less prevalent, with only a third of the population indicating that they trust it when it comes to dealing with the pandemic.
Similar to Study 1, we found that the appeal to nature bias mostly decreases with age, while the opposite is true for trust in science (S1 Appendix).
Use of pseudoscientific practices as protective measures
While the use of pseudoscientific practices was not widespread in the population, there was still a significant portion of people who resorted to it (Table 6). More than 50 percent of the participants indicated that they at least sometimes ate garlic or bee products to protect themselves from the virus, while the least prevalent practice was the consumption of healing concoctions, (42% of the population used them at least sometimes). However, the majority still claimed they have never resorted to any of the three pseudoscientific practices.
Relations between epistemically suspect beliefs, trust, pseudoscientific practices and vaccination status
Table 7 shows means, standard deviations and correlations between epistemically suspect beliefs, trust, pseudoscientific practices and vaccination status. We again transformed all of the variables using the Rankit rank-based transformation and reported the non-transformed ones for descriptives.
Relations between variables were of similar magnitude as in Study 1. Pseudoscientific practices show relatively weak relations to most variables, with the exception of trust in science and vaccination status. It was most strongly correlated with the appeal to nature bias. The two types of trust were once again not related. Vaccination status showed the strongest correlation with the appeal to nature bias.
Mediation analyses. We ran a total of four mediation analyses, two where the outcome variable was vaccination status and two where we used pseudoscientific practice use as the outcome. In all four models, the predictors were trust in science and the wisdom of the common man. For each of the outcome variables, we fit one model with belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories as a mediator, and one with the appeal to nature bias as a mediator. We used the same analyses properties as in Study 1.
Vaccination status as the outcome. Firstly, we tested the same models as in Study 1 (Figs 3 and 4). Partially in line with our expectations (H1a), when belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories was entered as a mediator, trust in science contributed both directly (β = .357, 95% CI [.277, .439]; p < .001) and through belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories (β = .087, 95% CI [.060, .122]; p < .001). Similarly, trust in the wisdom of the common man had both a significant direct (β = -.094, 95% CI [-.170, -0.029]; p = .008) and indirect effects (β = -.077, 95% CI [-.111, -0.052]; p < .001) on vaccination status. The model explained 20 percent of variance of the outcome variable.
Note. The paths are shown with standardized estimates. Non-significant paths are shown as dashed lines.
Note. The paths are shown with standardized estimates. Non-significant paths are shown as dashed lines.
Next, we tested the same model, but with the appeal to nature bias as the mediator (Fig 4, H1b), and fully replicated the pattern observed in Study 1. Trust in science contributed both directly (β = .315, 95% CI [.237, .394]; p < .001) and indirectly (β = .129, 95% CI [.097, .173]; p < .001) to predicting vaccination status. In turn, wisdom of the common man contributed to the prediction through the appeal to nature bias (β = -.120, 95% CI [-.153, -0.090]; p < .001), but not directly (β = -.051, 95% CI [-.112, .018]; p = .139). In total, the model explained 25% of variance in vaccination status.
Use of pseudoscientific practices as the outcome. We proceeded to test the same models, but with use of pseudoscientific practices as the outcome.
When belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories was entered as the mediator (Fig 5), the model explained only a total of two percent of the variance of the outcome variable. None of the direct paths was significant, however, as expected (H2a), both trust in science (β = -.045, 95% CI [-.073, -.020]; p < .001) and trust in the wisdom of the common man (β = .041, 95% CI [.018, .068]; p < .001) had an indirect effect on the use of pseudoscientific practices through belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories.
Note. The paths are shown with standardized estimates. Non-significant paths are shown as dashed lines.
Finally, we tested the same model, but with the appeal to nature bias as the mediator (Fig 6). As with the previous model and as we expected (H2b) the relation between trust in science and use of pseudoscientific practices was fully mediated by the appeal to nature bias (β = -.055, 95% CI [-.082, -.027]; p < .001), as was the relation between trust in the wisdom of the common man and the use of pseudoscientific practices (β = .053, 95% CI [.024, .080]; p < .001), explaining three percent of the variance of the use of pseudoscientific practices.
Note. The paths are shown with standardized estimates. Non-significant paths are shown as dashed lines.
In line with our prediction (H3), the appeal to nature bias had a slightly stronger correlation to the use of pseudoscientific practices than conspiratorial beliefs. The model with the appeal to nature bias as a mediator explained more variance as well, however this difference was minor.
Discussion
Across two studies on nationally representative samples, we explored how trust in science and trust in the wisdom of the common man contributed to the prediction of questionable health practices through epistemically suspect beliefs.
We repeatedly observed high trust in the ability of science to deal with the ongoing pandemic, with more than 70 percent of the population indicating they trust science more than they do not, whilst wisdom of the common man was trusted by around one-third. When evaluating this finding, one should take into account that these were self-reported measures, thus potentially sensitive to socially desirable responding—especially relevant for trust in science. However, epistemically suspect beliefs were endorsed by a significant proportion of the population as well: conspiratorial beliefs by 40 percent, and the appeal to nature bias regarding COVID-19 by more than 30 percent. In line with previous research, the two types of beliefs were moderately positively correlated, indicating that they tend to cluster together [17,23]. Our results testify to the role of these beliefs, i.e. to the fact that, although important, trust in science is neither only nor solely a direct precursor of pandemic-related health behaviors.
First, the appeal to nature bias had the strongest correlation to both vaccination status and the use of pseudoscientific practices. The youngest participants who were most prone to appeal to nature were also the ones least vaccinated. Second, trust in science both directly and indirectly predicted the vaccination status, corroborating recent findings [50], whilst trust in the wisdom of the common man did so only indirectly. As for their relation to the use of pseudoscientific practices to protect oneself from COVID-19, it was fully mediated by conspiratorial beliefs and the appeal to nature bias. This might be due to the fact that trust in science is a better-defined construct and has more content overlap with vaccination status. Even though it was widely used in public discourse, the term “wisdom of the common man” is more vague, and can encompass different areas of knowledge. We also found no significant correlation between trust in science and trust in the wisdom of the common man, implying that relying on folk wisdom does not necessarily lead to the devaluation of experts and science and that it cannot be equated to anti-intellectualism. The idea of their potential coexistence has already been raised in the literature [51]: the two types of knowledge were assumed to be drawn upon separately or even simultaneously, depending on the situation.
Our results also revealed a more nuanced relation between the two types of questionable health practices. They were not negatively related as we expected, but unrelated. In addition, while the use of pseudoscientific practices was related to trust in the wisdom of the common man, it was not related to trust in science. Finally, taken together, our models were better at predicting non-adhering to recommended health practices (vaccination) than predicting resorting to non-evidence-based practices. Whether this pattern of results could be better explained by the overall image of the TM/CAM practices or by our specific choice of these practices remains to be tested. Namely, TM/CAM practices as a whole are often described as healthier and with less side effects in comparison to the practices recommended by conventional medicine [52]. To capture enough variance in the population, in this research we deliberately focused on practices low in cost, both in terms of resources and risk, but potentially high in benefit. We did observe their relatively widespread use, with 50 percent of the population reporting that, in order to protect themselves from the virus, they consumed garlic or honey at least sometimes. It could thus be the case that these particular practices were perceived as especially harmless and might have been practiced regardless of whether or not a person is adhering to official recommendations, as an added safeguard against the virus.
Implications
Although we did observe relatively high reported trust in science across our two samples, that does not necessarily imply that our participants are well equipped to evaluate scientific information, assess the evidence base for certain claims, and disentangle the official versus unofficial sources of information, i.e., that they hold a high level of “epistemic vigilance” [53,54]. During the pandemic, this was an especially difficult task, as certain voices from the scientific community spread conspiracy theories about the virus. Long-term efforts in building scientific and digital literacy could equip laypeople with a skillset to address these issues.
However, a pandemic is an emergency that requires more imminent solutions as well. For example, the epistemic authority of the wisdom of the common man could be better exploited in public communication by tying it to scientific content instead, for example via interventions where “the common man” either endorses science and scientists (“It was always wise to listen to people who know more than you”; “I would always take my car to a mechanic for a repair”), or is asked to be a spokesperson for a scientific consensus, testifying to their adherence to recommended practices.
Similarly, although the appeal to nature bias was repeatedly publicly debunked by health experts, it does not seem to be enough—thus, more complex interventions aiming to dispel the idea of natural or strong immunity as a protective factor against the virus should be devised (for example, direct debunking could be complemented with personal narratives [55]); media reports on young and otherwise healthy people suffering from COVID-19 could serve the same purpose.
Limitations and future research
Our results point to the role of the appeal to nature bias regarding COVID-19 in predicting important health outcomes in the COVID-19 pandemic. Since exploring the latent structure of this construct was out of the scope of this research, we assessed it via only two items. Future studies should include a broader set of items to understand it better, as well as items that are not specifically tied to COVID-19. For example, it is possible that it encompasses two more specific subdimensions, one which reflects the naturalistic fallacy (i.e., all natural things are good), and the other which reflects its consequences (i.e., beliefs that natural immunity is invincible and/or necessarily better than relying on conventional medicine).
Due to the fact that the study was resource-demanding, we measured other constructs with shorter scales, as well. This led to somewhat lower reliability than the standard. It should be noted, however, that the reliability is still adequate given the number of items used, and that despite this, the results seem to be robust across two large, representative samples.
Although we replicated the findings about the predictiveness of COVID-19 related conspiracy theories for health behavior in the pandemic, the appeal to nature bias was a stronger predictor. It could be due to the specificity of conspiratorial content: if the conspiracies were more health-related or even vaccine-related, their predictiveness and the overall predictiveness of the model could be higher. This would be in line with research pointing to different predictiveness of different types of conspiracy theories [56].
We choose two prominent candidates to represent epistemically suspect beliefs. To illustrate there are more such beliefs forming a mindset, future studies could add more and test their interrelatedness. For example, the role of proneness to type 1 error bias, i.e., minimizing the false positives as they are costlier to make [26], or the role of “doublethink”, i.e., proneness to simultaneously holding incompatible beliefs [45] could be tested. In addition to testing a broader set of this type of beliefs which makes people more vulnerable to avoiding recommended behaviors and resorting to non-evidence-based ones, the design could also incorporate potentially protective constructs that could make people more resistant to these practices. That could be more factual knowledge about the virus or general proneness to analytical thinking.
Future studies should also aim to capture a more diverse range of pseudoscientific practices and include ones more likely to cause side effects or divert from the official treatment, as well as ones more costly in terms of resources. Additionally, focusing on specific populations that might be more prone to extreme pseudoscientific practices could provide better insight as to how these practices relate to each other, to epistemically suspect beliefs and the reliance on conventional medicine in general.
Our results reveal that “wisdom of the common man” is a somewhat vague and culturally sensitive construct. To map it more precisely, and test its invariance, it should be assessed cross-culturally, along with potentially overlapping constructs such as thinking styles, ideological left-right preference, anti-paternalism and individualism/collectivism. It would also be informative to include other epistemic sources, such as religious authorities, and contrast them with the two assessed in our studies.
Finally, the data from these two studies is correlational so it cannot speak of causality. Experimental studies should therefore explore the effects of manipulating trust in epistemic authorities, especially in the wisdom of the common man on a range of health behaviors, and the proposed mediation of the effect by epistemically suspect beliefs.
Conclusion
This study lends further support to a growing body of work suggesting that conspiratorial beliefs shape pandemic-related health behavior [21], and adds the appeal to nature bias as another epistemically suspect belief that is relevant in this context. Furthermore, it speaks to the importance of epistemic authorities as sources of information: besides once again flagging trust in science, it brings new evidence about the role of trust in the wisdom of the common man. The results reveal the relation between the two is sensitive to the cultural context, and that they need not be mutually exclusive. In addition to recommending long-term efforts aimed at nurturing trust in science and a better understanding of the scientific process, we also offer recommendations for tailoring short-term messaging from public health and media professionals.
References
- 1.
Sahakian BJ, LaBuzetta JN. Bad moves: how decision making goes wrong, and the ethics of smart drugs. First edition. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2013. 167 p.
- 2. Weaver RK. Compliance Regimes and Barriers to Behavioral Change: Compliance Regimes and Behavioral Change. Governance. 2014 Apr;27(2):243–65.
- 3. Chavda VP, Sonak SS, Munshi NK, Dhamade PN. Pseudoscience and fraudulent products for COVID-19 management. Environ Sci Pollut Res. 2022 Jul 14.
- 4. Christensen AJ, Ehlers SL. Psychological factors in end-stage renal disease: an emerging context for behavioral medicine research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70(3):712–24. pmid:12090378
- 5. Benecke O, DeYoung SE. Anti-vaccine decision-making and measles resurgence in the United States. Glob Pediatr Health. 2019 Jan;6:2333794X1986294. pmid:31384629
- 6. Cutler RL, Fernandez-Llimos F, Frommer M, Benrimoj C, Garcia-Cardenas V. Economic impact of medication non-adherence by disease groups: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2018 Jan;8(1):e016982. pmid:29358417
- 7.
Sabaté E, World Health Organization, editors. Adherence to long-term therapies: evidence for action. World Health Organization; 2003. 198 p.
- 8. WHO. Ten threats to global health in 2019. [Internet]. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/spotlight/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019.
- 9.
Ernst E. Alternative medicine: a critical assessment of 150 modalities. New York, NY: Springer; 2019.
- 10. Freckelton QC I. COVID-19: Fear, quackery, false representations and the law. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2020 Sep;72:101611. pmid:32911444
- 11. WHO. Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) advice for the public: mythbusters [Internet]. 2021. Available from: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/advice-for-public/myth-busters.
- 12. Johnson SB, Park HS, Gross CP, Yu JB. Use of alternative medicine for cancer and its impact on survival. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2018 Jan 1;110(1):121–4. pmid:28922780
- 13. Werneke U, Earl J, Seydel C, Horn O, Crichton P, Fannon D. Potential health risks of complementary alternative medicines in cancer patients. Br J Cancer. 2004 Jan;90(2):408–13. pmid:14735185
- 14. Ladenheim D, Horn O, Werneke U, Phillpot M, Murungi A, Theobald N, et al. Potential health risks of complementary alternative medicines in HIV patients. HIV Med. 2008 Sep;9(8):653–9. pmid:18631258
- 15.
Alo. Dr Nestorović otkriva: kafa, rakija i slatko od dunje su moj recept za jak imunitet [Dr Nestorovic reveals: coffee, homemade brandy and quince jam are my recipe for strong immunity][Internet]. 2021. Available from: https://www.alo.rs/zena/zdravlje/537327/dr-nestorovic-otkriva-kafa-rakija-i-slatko-od-dunje-su-moj-recept-za-jak-imunitet/vest.
- 16. Rogan J. #1639 –Dave Smith. The Joe Rogan Experience [Internet]. 2021. Available from: https://open.spotify.com/episode/7taqki4fGUkcXESbaUzjgh?si=Njqe6imhTU2rbSFnZ_LDFA.
- 17. Rizeq J, Flora DB, Toplak ME. An examination of the underlying dimensional structure of three domains of contaminated mindware: paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes. Think Reason. 2021 Apr 3;27(2):187–211.
- 18. Douglas KM, Sutton RM. Why conspiracy theories matter: a social psychological analysis. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2018 Jan;29(1):256–98.
- 19. Oliver JE, Wood T. Medical conspiracy theories and health behaviors in the United States. JAMA Intern Med. 2014 May 1;174(5):817. pmid:24638266
- 20. Jolley D, Douglas KM. The Effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. Tripp R, editor. PLoS ONE. 2014 Feb 20;9(2):e89177. pmid:24586574
- 21. van Mulukom V, Pummerer LJ, Alper S, Bai H, Čavojová V, Farias J, et al. Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: A systematic review. Social Science & Medicine. 2022 May 1;301:114912. pmid:35354105
- 22. Lamberty P, Imhoff R. Powerful Pharma and its marginalized alternatives?: effects of individual differences in conspiracy mentality on attitudes toward medical approaches. Soc Psychol (Gott). 2018 Sep;49(5):255–70.
- 23. Lobato E, Mendoza J, Sims V, Chin M. Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population: relationship between unwarranted beliefs. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2014 Sep;28(5):617–25.
- 24. Mijatović N, Šljivić J, Tošić N, Conić L, Petrović M, Žeželj I. Big Suppla: Challenging the Common View of the Supplements and Herbs Industry Affects the Willingness to Try and Recommend Their Products. Studia Psychologica. 2022 Mar 16;64(1):91–103.
- 25. Pavela Banai I, Banai B, Mikloušić I. Beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories, compliance with the preventive measures, and trust in government medical officials. Curr Psychol. 2021 May 26. pmid:34075284
- 26. Teovanović P, Lukić P, Zupan Z, Lazić A, Ninković M, Žeželj I. Irrational beliefs differentially predict adherence to guidelines and pseudoscientific practices during the COVID ‐19 pandemic. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2021 Mar;35(2):486–96. pmid:33362344
- 27.
Hume D. David Hume: A Treatise of Human Nature. 2nd edition. Selby-Bigge LA, Nidditch PH, editors. Oxford University Press; 1978.
- 28.
Moore GE. Principia ethica. 2nd edition. Baldwin T, editor. Cambridge [England]; New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press; 1993. 313 p.
- 29.
Baggini J. Making sense: philosophy behind the headlines. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. 296 p.
- 30. Moldovan A. On appeals to nature and their use in the public controversy over genetically modified organisms. IL. 2018 Sep 14;38(3):409–37.
- 31. Lindahl MG, Linder C. What’s natural about nature? Deceptive concepts in socio-scientific decision-making. European J Sci Math Ed. 2015 Jul 15;3(3):250–64.
- 32. Mulet JM. The appeal-to-nature fallacy: Homeopathy and biodynamic agriculture in official EU regulations. Mètode. 2017 Dec 18;(8).
- 33. Kata A. Anti-vaccine activists, Web 2.0, and the postmodern paradigm–an overview of tactics and tropes used online by the anti-vaccination movement. Vaccine. 2012 May;30(25):3778–89. pmid:22172504
- 34. Stolle LB, Nalamasu R, Pergolizzi JV, Varrassi G, Magnusson P, LeQuang J, et al. Fact vs fallacy: the anti-vaccine discussion reloaded. Adv Ther. 2020 Nov;37(11):4481–90. pmid:32965654
- 35. Wong LP, Lin Y, Alias H, Bakar SA, Zhao Q, Hu Z. COVID-19 anti-vaccine sentiments: analyses of comments from social media. Healthcare (Basel). 2021 Nov 9;9(11):1530. pmid:34828576
- 36. McKee M, Diethelm P. How the growth of denialism undermines public health. BMJ. 2010 Dec 14;341(dec14 1):c6950–c6950. pmid:21156741
- 37. Dohle S, Wingen T, Schreiber M. Acceptance and adoption of protective measures during the covid-19 pandemic: the role of trust in politics and trust in science. Soc Psychol Bulletin. 2020 Dec 23;15(4):1–23.
- 38. Plohl N, Musil B. Modeling compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: the critical role of trust in science. Psychol Health Med. 2021 Jan 2;26(1):1–12. pmid:32479113
- 39. Lobato EJC, Powell M, Padilla LMK, Holbrook C. Factors predicting willingness to share covid-19 misinformation. Front Psychol. 2020 Sep 24;11:566108. pmid:33071894
- 40. Roozenbeek J, Schneider CR, Dryhurst S, Kerr J, Freeman ALJ, Recchia G, et al. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. R Soc Open Sci. 2020 Oct;7(10):201199. pmid:33204475
- 41.
Hofstadter R. Anti-intellectualism in American life. New York: Knopf; 1963.
- 42.
Harris L. The next American Civil War: the populist revolt against the liberal elite. 1st ed. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2010. 248 p.
- 43. Merkley E, Loewen PJ. Anti-intellectualism and the mass public’s response to the COVID-19 pandemic. Nat Hum Behav. 2021 Jun;5(6):706–15. pmid:33911228
- 44. Jovchelovitch S. The rehabilitation of common sense: social representations, science and cognitive polyphasia. J Theory Soc Behav. 2008 Dec;38(4):431–48.
- 45. Petrović MB, Žeželj I. Thinking inconsistently: development and validation of an instrument for assessing proneness to doublethink. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2021 Aug 17;1015–5759/a000645.
- 46. Motta M, Callaghan T. The pervasiveness and policy consequences of medical folk wisdom in the U.S. Sci Rep. 2020 Dec;10(1):10722. pmid:32612260
- 47. Solomon SR, Sawilowsky SS. Impact of rank-based normalizing transformations on the accuracy of test scores. J Mod App Stat Meth. 2009 Nov 1;8(2):448–62.
- 48.
JASP team. JASP. 2021.
- 49. Lazić A, Petrović M, Branković M, Žeželj I. Quick natural cure-alls: Portrayal of traditional, complementary, and alternative medicine in Serbian online media [Internet]. PsyArXiv; 2022 Jul. Available from: https://osf.io/rz2h9.
- 50. Čavojová V, Šrol J, Mikušková EB. With the little help of science understanding: examining the direct and indirect role of scientific reasoning and trust in science in normative health behaviour during pandemic [Internet]. PsyArXiv; 2021 Jun [cited 2021 Dec 22]. Available from: https://osf.io/xahdj.
- 51.
Jovchelovitch S, Priego-Hernández J. Cognitive polyphasia, knowledge encounters and public spheres. In: Sammut G, Andreouli E, Gaskell G, Valsiner J, editors. The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2015. p. 163–78.
- 52. Welz AN, Emberger-Klein A, Menrad K. Why people use herbal medicine: insights from a focus-group study in Germany. BMC Complement Altern Med. 2018 Dec;18(1):92. pmid:29544493
- 53. Blancke S, Boudry M, Braeckman J. Whence pseudoscience?An epidemiological approach. Mètode. 2017 Dec 18;(8).
- 54. Sperber D, Clément F, Heintz C, Mascaro O, Mercier H, Origgi G, et al. Epistemic Vigilance. Mind & Language. 2010 Aug 20;25(4):359–93.
- 55. Lazić A, Žeželj I. A systematic review of narrative interventions: Lessons for countering anti-vaccination conspiracy theories and misinformation. Public Underst Sci. 2021 Aug;30(6):644–70. pmid:34006153
- 56. Imhoff R, Lamberty P. A bioweapon or a hoax? the link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2020 Nov;11(8):1110–8.