Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Assessment of cognitive functioning after living kidney donation: A cross-sectional pilot study

  • Marie Mikuteit ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Marie Mikuteit, Faikah Gueler

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

  • Faikah Gueler ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Marie Mikuteit, Faikah Gueler

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Nephrology and Hypertension, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

  • Iris Pollmann,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Current address: Department of Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Schleswig-Holstein, Kiel, Germany

    Affiliation Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

  • Henning Pflugrad,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

  • Meike Dirks,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

  • Martina de Zwaan ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    dezwaan.martina@mh-hannover.de

    ‡ MZ and KW also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

  • Karin Weissenborn

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ MZ and KW also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany

Abstract

Background

Chronic kidney disease (CKD) has emerged as a risk factor for cognitive impairment. Living kidney donation results in reduction of the donors’ renal function. This is considered acceptable in general but possible associations with cognitive function have not yet been studied.

Methods

Sixty living kidney donors (LKD), who had donated between 2003 and 2012 at Hannover Medical School, underwent neurocognitive testing including attentional and memory testing. In a cross-sectional design results were compared with data of healthy controls (n = 40) and with norm data given in the respective test manuals adjusted for age, sex, and education.

Results

The median age of the LKD was 58 (range 39–70) years and the median time since donation was 7 (range 4–14) years. The LKD did not differ from controls in most of the cognitive test results and a composite attention test sum score. However, LKD did worse than controls in tests of working memory, parallel processing of stimuli, and sustained attention. No differences were found regarding quality of life. In LKD cognitive test results correlated significantly only with educational level but not with time since transplantation, eGFR, somatic comorbidity, quality of life and levels of fatigue, distress, depression, and anxiety.

Conclusions

Our data show a fairly normal performance of LKD in most attentional and memory tests. However, our pilot study also suggests some cognitive impairment in attention tests in LKD which would need to be confirmed in longitudinal prospective studies.

Introduction

Kidney transplantation is the preferential treatment for patients with end stage renal disease (ESRD) [1]. The outcome for the recipients is in general better with a living donation than with deceased donation [2]. Living kidney donors (LKD) rarely experience severe side effects of the nephrectomy such as ESRD or complications like-re-hospitalization [3, 4]. The mortality of LKD does not differ from the general population [1, 5]. In most cases the donors’ renal function remains stable for a long time [2, 57] though at a decreased level compared to before donation. In the long term, between 12 and 25 percent of the LKD develop an estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) of < 60 ml/min [2, 8].

A systematic review revealed that most, but not all, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies suggest an association between cognitive impairment and CKD [9]. Even though cognitive impairment is most likely to occur at eGFRs < 30 ml/min/1.73 m2 [10], it has been demonstrated in community-based studies that cognitive functioning is reduced even in subjects with only moderate CKD, e.g. with an eGFR between 30 and 60 ml/min/1.73 m2 [11, 12]. Cardiovascular and other risk factors have shown to mediate the relationship between CKD in some but not all cognitive tests [12, 13]. Even after controlling for traditional cardiovascular risk factors, patients with CKD showed worse global cognition, visual-spatial orientation, concentration and memory compared to controls suggesting that CKD might be an independent risk factor for cognitive decline [12]. To the best of our knowledge, there are no available data on cognitive functioning in LKD even though their eGFR frequently declines post-donation.

Thus, the goal of the present cross-sectional pilot study was to assess cognitive functioning in LKD and to compare the results in LKD with those of heathy controls and with norm data presented in the respective test manuals. We hypothesized that LKD would show worse cognitive functioning compared to healthy controls. Our secondary hypotheses were that impairment of cognitive function in LKD would be associated with lower eGFR, higher levels of fatigue, depression, anxiety, and general psychological distress and lower levels of quality of life.

Material and methods

Study population

We investigated a convenience sample of 60 LKD out of a total sample of 315 LKD who participated in a follow-up study assessing physical and mental well-being after living kidney donation. Participants had undergone donor nephrectomy at Hannover Medical School between 2003 and 2012 [14, 15]. Exclusion criteria for this neurocognitive sub-study were any neurological disease and any mental disorder, use of central nervous system (CNS) affecting medication, language barrier, and age > 70 years.

Forty healthy subjects served as controls. They were recruited in the environment of participating physicians and patients and as a group did not differ with regard to sex distribution, age, and educational level from the LKD sample.

The study complied with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the ethics committee of Hannover Medical School (no. 3252–2016). All patients gave their written informed consent.

Neurocognitive testing

The Test of Attentional Performance (TAP) was used [16, 17]. It is comprised of a collection of methods which allows a differentiated diagnosis of attention deficits. The TAP is a standardized software package that uses simple reaction paradigms in which one has to react to well discriminable, non-verbal stimuli by a simple key press. The performance criteria are the reaction time (RT) in milliseconds (ms) and any mistakes. The present study utilized the following eight subtests: (1) the alertness test (assesses the increase in level of attention when anticipating a stimulus), (2) the working memory task (probes the ability to control information flow and update information in working memory in real time), (3) the crossmodal integration task (examines the ability to detect the pre-specified combination of an acoustic stimulus and a subsequent visual stimulus), (4) the flexible reaction test (a set-shifting task that requires alternating reactions to numbers and letters), (5) the divided attention test (assesses ability to process visual and auditory stimuli in parallel), (6) the Go/No Go task (assesses the ability to suppress an inadequate response and therefore executive attention), (7) the incompatibility test (assesses the effect of contradictory stimulus information on stimulus processing), and (8) the covert shift of attention task (assesses the ability to focus visual attention) [16, 17]. Additionally, the cancelling d test [18] for the assessment of sustained attention was used. Finally, the following tests were utilized to assess memory functioning: the Recurring Figures Test (RFT) [19] and the Word Figure Memory Test (WFMT) [20]. The RFT assesses learning ability and recognition of nonverbal material and the WFMT assesses recognition of words and figures separately.

For the cancelling d test, the TAP battery tests and the RFT, results equal to or below the 10th percentile of norm data were considered abnormal, for the WFMT a z-score ≤ -1.3 compared to norm data was considered as abnormal. The results of the subtests of the TAP battery and the cancelling d test were used for the calculation of a composite attention test sum score, which gives the rate of abnormal test results out of the total number of attention test results achieved (range 0 to 1) and depicts a representative score for each patient’s individual attention ability [2123]. An attention test sum score > 0.4 was considered to represent a clinically relevant cognitive impairment. If a participant was not able to complete a subtest, this was counted as abnormal result. The reasons were lack of comprehension, a very long reaction time and a large number of mistakes. Other putative reasons such as decreased visual acuity were excluded.

To control for unexpected study-inherent confounding factors such as incorrect test instructions, for example, the patients’ test results were compared to the results of 40 concomitantly examined healthy controls in addition to the comparison with pre-defined norm data.

All cognitive assessments were performed by members of the working group according to a predetermined procedure. The cognitive tests were conducted in an undisturbed environment either at the hospital (LKD) or at other places such as private homes or work places (controls). The tests took around 2 hours per participant and were always completed in the same order.

Questionnaires

To assess quality of life the Short-Form 12 Health Survey (SF-12), a short version of the SF-36 Health Survey, was used and adjusted to American standards [24, 25] in participating LKD and in controls.

In LKD the presence and extent of fatigue was assessed using the Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI-2) [26, 27]. Symptoms of depression were assessed with the 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire-Depression Scale (PHQ-9) [28] and symptoms of anxiety were assessed with the 7-item Generalized Anxiety Scale (GAD-7) [29, 30]. The one-dimensional short version of the Symptom Checklist 90 (SCL-9) [31] was applied for assessing general psychological distress (global severity index, GSI).

Sociodemographic and clinical data

The survey also contained investigator-generated questions related to personal data of the donor and donation-specific variables such as age, sex, educational level, somatic comorbidities and date of the donation. Additionally, kidney function (eGFR CKD-EPI) of the 60 donors was assessed at the time of the study. Two LKD refused blood sampling at the time of study assessment.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 24. Depending on data distribution, either the Student’s t test or the Mann-Whitney U test was used to conduct between-group comparisons (LKD versus controls). Chi-square tests were conducted for categorical data. Effect sizes were calculated with Cohen’s d and phi [32]. Regarding Cohen’s d, 0.2 expresses a small effect, 0.5 a medium, and 0.8 a large effect. Regarding phi (df = 1), 0.1 expresses a small effect, 0.3 a medium, and 0.5 a large effect. Medians and the 25th and 75th percentiles and number of participants and the percentages of participants are given for individual variables.

In the LKD group we performed regression analyses to examine putative associations between the attention test sum score and sociodemographic and clinical variables. Univariate linear regression analyses were performed with the attention test sum score as the dependent variable and age, sex, educational level, time since donation, current eGFR, change in eGFR from pre-to post donation, the scores of the MFI, PCS, MCS, PHQ-9, GAD-7, and SCL-9, and the presence of hyperlipidemia, coronary heart disease, diabetes, hypertension, and hypothyroidism as independent variables.

A p-value <0.05 was regarded as significant. Since the study was primarily exploratory, we did not correct for multiple testing.

Results

Baseline characteristics of donors and controls

The comparison between LKD and controls are summarized in Table 1. Thirty-four (56.7%) of the 60 donors who participated in our study were female. Median age at the time of assessment was 58 years in the LKD group and 55.5 years in the control group. The median time since donation was 7 (range 4–14) years. Donors had attended school for 11 (range 8–13) years in median, controls for 12 (range 9–13) years. Regarding sex, age and years of education there were no significant differences between LKD and healthy controls. Significantly more LKD than controls were diagnosed with hypertension and hyperlipidemia. The frequency of hypothyroidism, coronary heart disease and diabetes mellitus did not differ between groups. Additionally, LKD and controls showed scores within the normal range for quality of life.

thumbnail
Table 1. Comparison between living kidney donors (LKD) and controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264284.t001

Comparison between participants and non-participants

Of the 315 LKD who participated in the follow-up study, 184 LKD met inclusion criteria for our neurocognitive pilot study. The 60 LKD who participated in the neurocognitive study did not differ with regard to age, sex, time since donation, and the scores of the assessment instruments for health-related quality of life, mood, and fatigue from the non-participants (data not shown). The 60 participating LKD reported a higher educational level compared to the non-participants: median 11 years (25th; 75th percentile 10.0; 12.8) versus median 10 years (25th; 75th percentile 9.0; 12.0) (p = .034).

Attention and memory testing

LKD and controls did not differ regarding the attention test sum score (Table 2). There were also no differences between the two groups in most of the subtests. However, donors showed significantly more misses in the subtest working memory than controls, had slower reaction times (RT) in the subtests divided attention and incompatibility and were less successful in all results of the cancelling d test. The effect sizes ranged from small to large with a large effect size for the difference in misses in the “working memory” test (d = .824). There was no difference between LKD and controls in most of the memory tests. However, donors exhibited worse results for nonsense figures than controls in the RFT (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparison of cognitive test results between living kidney donors (LKD) and controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264284.t002

Regarding the number of abnormal test results, 8 (13.3%) of the donors and 3 (7.5%) of the controls had an attention test sum score above 0.4 which was considered as clinically relevant impaired attention; this difference was not statistically significant (Table 3). With regard to the individual tests, the comparison of the number of abnormal test results fits well with the comparison of the raw values between LKD and controls (Table 2). A higher percentage of LKD than of controls exhibited errors and misses in the working memory subtest and cancelling d test. In the subtest divided attention, donors responded more often abnormally slow towards auditory stimuli, but made less often mistakes. They also had more often a prolonged RT in the “valid” condition of the subtest covert shift of attention. In terms of memory function, donors and controls obtained comparable numbers of abnormal results in all subtests (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Number and percentage of LKD and controls with abnormal test results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264284.t003

Associations between cognitive test results and other variables in LKD

Overall, donors’ mean eGFR decreased from 96.2 (± 10.1) ml/min/1.73m2 pre-donation to 56.8 (± 8.8) ml/min/1.73m2 immediately post-donation, then increased to 59.9 (± 10.9) ml/min/1.73m2 at first visit 2–6 weeks after donation and to 61.3 (± 9.6) ml/min/1.73m2 at the time of assessment. At the time of testing 28 LKD had eGFR values < 60 (48.3%) and 30 (51.7%) of ≥ 60 ml/min/1.73m2; the values of two LKD were missing.

In univariate linear regression analyses there were no associations between the attention test sum score and sex, age, time since donation, kidney function (eGFR: β = 0.07; 95% CI = -.004 to .007, p = .59 and delta eGFR pre- to post-donation: β = -0.08; 95% CI = -0.01 to 0.01, p = .54), fatigue, quality of life and levels of distress, depression and anxiety, or any concomitant disease. Years of school attendance was the only significantly associated factor for the attention test sum score (β = -0.43; 95% CI = -0.09 to -0.03, p = .001). The longer LKD had attended school, the lower (more normal) were their attention test sum scores. Years of education alone explained 17.5% of the variance of the attention test sum score (corrected R2). Since we did not find significant associations other than educational level we did not perform multivariate regression models.

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first study assessing cognitive functioning in German LKD several years after donation. Our hypothesis was that LKD would show impaired cognitive functioning compared to norm data and to healthy controls which would correlate with kidney function and mental status. Sixty donors who had undergone kidney donation at Hannover Medical School (MHH) between 2003 and 2012 and were representative for the respective cohort of donors at MHH participated in the study.

There were no differences between LKD and controls in most of the individual results of the cognitive tests applied and specifically in the composite attention test sum score. Eight out of 60 donors as compared to three out of 40 controls achieved an abnormal attention test sum score of >.0.4 which is considered to indicate a clinically relevant cognitive impairment. However, the level of performance of LKD was below normal (compared to norms) and significantly different from controls in some results of the TAP tests working memory, divided attention, covert shift of attention, incompatibility, and in the cancelling d test. This might suggest some low-grade cognitive impairment, but considering the large number of tests and the lack of difference in the attentional composite score our data do not support the presence of severe cognitive impairment in LKD compared to healthy controls. Regarding the pattern of abnormal cognitive test results, we found similarities and differences between our sample of LKD and individuals with CKD in general population samples. General and visual attention and concentration were impaired in the LKD in our study similarly to individuals with CKD in population samples. However, in contrast to the LKD in our study, individuals with CKD frequently also exhibit memory impairment [1012, 34].

With regard to the association between eGFR and cognitive functioning, cross-sectional population studies have reported conflicting results. Hailpern et al. [33] showed that moderate CKD (eGFR of 30–59 ml/min/1.73m2) was associated with poorer results in tests on visual attention and learning and concentration. In another study, subjects with a eGFR <60 ml/min/1.73m2 performed worse in tests of visual and spatial organization and memory as well as scanning and tracking compared to subjects with a eGFR ≥60 ml/min/1.73m2 [11, 12], while working memory and verbal episodic memory were not impaired. In contrast, Davey et al. [34] showed in a long-term investigation, that with a clinically significant decline in eGFR (>3ml/year) subjects performed worse in the global composite of cognitive performances and in similarities and verbal episodic memory. In the Brain in Kidney Disease Study [10] participants with a eGFR between 30 and 59 ml/min/1.73m2 performed 0.2 to 0.5 standard deviations below norms in all tested domains of attention and memory. However, consistent with the results of the present study, others did not observe associations between performance on cognitive tests and eGFR or measured GFR in general population samples [3537]. In the general population, low GFR may be a sign of systemic atherosclerosis. However, LKD develop reduced kidney function through a different mechanism, and their clinical and prognostic significance remains uncertain [2].

LKD with abnormal attention test sum score differed in terms of educational level from LKD with normal scores although norms adjusted for education were used for scoring if applicable. Former studies have shown that a higher educational level might have a protective effect on cognitive function in patients with CKD [11]. This concept has been pursued in patients with metabolic disorders such as CKD or liver cirrhosis, but has also been discussed for example for Alzheimer’s dementia where intelligence and education have been considered supporting cognitive reserve [38]. Further research will be necessary to confirm the positive effect of education on attention and memory in LKD.

We hypothesised that an impaired cognitive function was correlated to higher fatigue scores, lower quality of life, as well as higher levels of distress, depression and anxiety. Depression is known to affect cognitive functioning [39, 40]. In our study; however, there was no difference regarding symptoms of fatigue, distress, depression, and anxiety between the patients with and without cognitive impairment. In our study we excluded participants with a diagnosed mental disorder; thus, in both groups the mean scores for the scales were clearly within population norms, indicating low symptom severity which might explain the lack of association with cognitive functioning.

Strengths and limitations of the study

This is the first pilot study investigating cognitive functioning in LKD using a comprehensive state-of-the-art cognitive test battery for the assessment of attention and memory and using a methodology that has been shown to be reliable in other patient populations [2123]. The test results were compared to norms adjusted for age, sex and education and we also included a healthy control group.

However, there are several limitations. Firstly, due to the cross-sectional design of our pilot study without data on cognitive functions before donation, we do not know if cognitive functioning changed after nephrectomy in our LKD. There is evidence that LKD are usually physically and mentally healthier compared to the general population before donation [7, 41]. This might also be true for neurocognitive functioning. Ultimately, only prospective studies will allow to determine whether LKD are confronted with post-operative cognitive decline.

The groups were fairly small and there could have been a selection bias, because we tested a convenience sample of 60 LKD out of 184 participants who met inclusion criteria for this sub-study. Also, the healthy control group was not matched. Because LKD are a selected sample of especially healthy individuals, the comparison with normative samples has been criticized [41].

Conclusions

In summary, 4 to 14 years after the kidney donation, we found some evidence that cognitive functioning might show some impairment in LKD compared to healthy controls even though a composite attention test sum score did not show significant differences. Test results were not associated with age, sex, kidney function, time since transplantation, quality of life, and mental status. These findings need further evaluation especially in longitudinal studies that address the intra-individual change of cognitive function from before to after donation. Also, a comparison of LKD and patients with modest early-stage CKD may help understanding the mechanism how kidney function affects attention and memory functions.

Acknowledgments

This paper is dedicated to Faikah Güler, who died on March 20, 2020. Faikah Güler was dedicated to her clinical and scientific work, but especially to her patients. We will keep her in fond memory.

We also thank Karl-Heinz Heiringhoff for excellent IT support and Hanna Prominski for help with management of the study.

References

  1. 1. Segev DL, Muzaale AD, Caffo BS, Mehta SH, Singer AL, Taranto SE, et al. Perioperative mortality and long-term survival following live kidney donation. JAMA. 2010;303(10):959–966. pmid:20215610
  2. 2. Garg AX, Muirhead N, Knoll G, Yang RC, Prasad GV, Thiessen-Philbrook H, et al. Proteinuria and reduced kidney function in living kidney donors: A systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression. Kidney Int. 2006;70(10):1801–1810. pmid:17003822
  3. 3. Reese PP, Boudville N, Garg AX. Living kidney donation: outcomes, ethics, and uncertainty. Lancet. 2015;385(9981):2003–2013. pmid:26090646
  4. 4. Mjøen G, Øyen O, Holdaas H, Midtvedt K, Line PD. Morbidity and mortality in 1022 consecutive living donor nephrectomies: benefits of a living donor registry. Transplantation. 2009;88(11):1273–1279. pmid:19996926
  5. 5. Schold JD, Goldfarb DA, Buccini LD, Rodrigue JR, Mandelbrot D, Heaphy EL, et al. Hospitalizations following living donor nephrectomy in the United States. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2014;9(2):355–365. pmid:24458071
  6. 6. Janki S, Klop KW, Dooper IM, Weimar W, Ijzermans JN, Kok NF. More than a decade after live donor nephrectomy: a prospective cohort study. Transpl Int. 2015;28(11):1268–1275. pmid:25865340
  7. 7. Gross CR, Messersmith EE, Hong BA, Jowsey SG, Jacobs C, Gillespie BW, et al. Health-related quality of life in kidney donors from the last five decades: results from the RELIVE study. Am J Transplant. 2013;13(11):2924–2934. pmid:24011252
  8. 8. Reimer J, Rensing A, Haasen C, Philipp T, Pietruck F, Franke GH. The impact of living-related kidney transplantation on the donor’s life. Transplantation. 2006;81(9):1268–1273. pmid:16699453
  9. 9. Etgen T, Chonchol M, Förstl H, Sander D. Chronic kidney disease and cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Nephrol. 2012;35(5):474–482. pmid:22555151
  10. 10. Murray AM, Bell EJ, Tupper DE, Davey CS, Pederson SL, Amiot EM, et al. The Brain in Kidney Disease (BRINK) Cohort Study: Design and Baseline Cognitive Function. Am J Kidney Dis. 2016;67(4):593–600. pmid:26744128
  11. 11. Elias MF, Elias PK, Seliger SL, Narsipur SS, Dore GA, Robbins MA. Chronic kidney disease, creatinine and cognitive functioning. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2009;24(8):2446–2452. pmid:19297357
  12. 12. Torres RV, Elias MF, Seliger S, Davey A, Robbins MA. Risk for cognitive impairment across 22 measures of cognitive ability in early-stage chronic kidney disease. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2017;32(2):299–306. pmid:28186575
  13. 13. Kurella Tamura M, Xie D, Yaffe K, Cohen DL, Teal V, Kasner SE, et al. Vascular risk factors and cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease: the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2011;6(2):248–256. pmid:20930087
  14. 14. Pollmann I, Gueler F, Mikuteit M, Nöhre M, Richter N, Weissenborn K, et al. Adaptive personality traits and psychosocial correlates among living kidney donors. Front Psychiatry. 2017;8:210. pmid:29109691
  15. 15. Nöhre M, Pollmann I, Mikuteit M, Weissenborn K, Gueler F, de Zwaan M. Partnership satisfaction in living kidney donors. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:353. pmid:30123146
  16. 16. Zimmermann P, Fimm B: Neuropsychologische Testbatterie zur Erfassung von Aufmerksamkeitsdefiziten—Revidierte Fassung. Psytest Psychologische Testsysteme: Vera Fimm, Psychologisches Institut der Universität Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany; 1989.
  17. 17. Psytest Webpage. https://www.psytest.de/index.php?page=TAP-2-2&hl=de_DE. last accessed 03-12-2021.
  18. 18. Brickenkamp R: Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (Test d2). Hogrefe, Göttingen Toronto Bern Seattle; 2002. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2002-19861 pmid:11823950
  19. 19. Rixecker H, Hartje W: Kimura’s Recurring-Figures-Test: a normative study. J Clin Psychol. 1980;36(2):465–467. pmid:7372815
  20. 20. Weissenborn K, Rückert N, Brassel F, Becker H, Dietz H. A proposed modification of the Wada test for presurgical assessment in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroradiology. 1996;38(5):422–429. pmid:8837083
  21. 21. Prell T, Dirks M, Arvanitis D, Braun D, Peschel T, Worthmann H, et al. Cerebral patterns of neuropsychological disturbances in hepatitis C patients. J Neurovirol. 2019;25(2):229–238. pmid:30610739
  22. 22. Dirks M, Haag K, Pflugrad H, Tryc AB, Schuppner R, Wedemeyer H, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in hepatitis C patients resemble those of patients with autoimmune liver disease but are different from those in hepatitis B patients. J Viral Hepat. 2019;26(4):422–431. pmid:30120896
  23. 23. Dirks M, Pflugrad H, Haag K, Tillmann HL, Wedemeyer H, Arvanitis D, et al. Persistent neuropsychiatric impairment in HCV patients despite clearance of the virus?! J Viral Hepat. 2017;24(7):541–550. pmid:28117537
  24. 24. Gandek B, Ware JE, Aaronson NK, Apolone G, Bjorner JB, Brazier JE, et al. Cross-validation of item selection and scoring for the SF-12 Health Survey in nine countries: results from the IQOLA Project. International Quality of Life Assessment. J Clin Epidemiol. 1998;51(11):1171–1178. pmid:9817135
  25. 25. Ware JE, Kosinski M, Keller SD. SF-12: How to Score the SF-12 Physical and Mental Health Summary Scales. In. Second Edition, The Health Institute, New England Medical Center, Boston, MA; 1995.
  26. 26. Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, De Haes JC. The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res. 1995;39(3):315–325. pmid:7636775
  27. 27. Schwarz R, Krauss O, Hinz A: Fatigue in the general population. Onkologie 2003, 26(2):140–144. pmid:12771522
  28. 28. Löwe B, Kroenke K, Herzog W, Gräfe K. Measuring depression outcome with a brief self-report instrument: sensitivity to change of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9). J Affect Disord. 2004;81(1):61–66. pmid:15183601
  29. 29. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(10):1092–1097. pmid:16717171
  30. 30. Löwe B, Decker O, Müller S, Brähler E, Schellberg D, Herzog W, et al. Validation and standardization of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Screener (GAD-7) in the general population. Med Care. 2008;46(3):266–274. pmid:18388841
  31. 31. Prinz U, Nutzinger DO, Schulz H, Petermann F, Braukhaus C, Andreas S. Comparative psychometric analysis of the SCL-90-R and its short versions in patients with affective disorders. BMC Psychatry. 2013;13: pmid:23537095
  32. 32. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  33. 33. Hailpern SM, Melamed ML, Cohen HW, Hostetter TH. Moderate chronic kidney disease and cognitive function in adults 20 to 59 years of age: Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III). J Am Soc Nephrol. 2007;18(7):2205–2213. pmid:17554148
  34. 34. Davey A, Elias MF, Robbins MA, Seliger SL, Dore GA. Decline in renal functioning is associated with longitudinal decline in global cognitive functioning, abstract reasoning and verbal memory. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2013;28(7):1810–1819. pmid:23166308
  35. 35. Småbrekke S, Schirmer H, Melsom T, Solbu MD, Eriksen BO. Low-grade impairments in cognitive and kidney function in a healthy middle-aged general population: a cross-sectional study. BMC Nephrol. 2019;20:166. pmid:31088493
  36. 36. Martens RJ, Kooman JP, Stehouwer CD, Dagnelie PC, van der Kallen CJ, Koster A, et al. Estimated GFR, albuminuria, and cognitive performance: The Maastricht Study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2017;69(2):179–191. pmid:27291486
  37. 37. Joosten H, Izaks GJ, Slaets JP, de Jong PE, Visser ST, Bilo HJ, et al. Association of cognitive function with albuminuria and eGFR in the general population. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2011;6(6):1400–1409. pmid:21566108
  38. 38. Le Carret N, Lafont S, Mayo W, Fabrigoule C. The effect of education on cognitive performances and its implication for the constitution of the cognitive reserve. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;23(3):317–337. pmid:12740188
  39. 39. Ahern E, Semkovska M. Cognitive functioning in the first-episode of major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. 2017;31:52–72. pmid:27732039
  40. 40. Liu J, Liu B, Wang M, Ju Y, Dong Q, Lu X, et al. Evidence for progressive cognitive deficits in patients with major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:627695. pmid:33664684
  41. 41. Kroencke S, Fischer L, Nashan B, Herich L, Schulz KH. A prospective study on living related kidney donors’ quality of life in the first year: choosing appropriate reference data. Clin Transplant. 2012;26(4):E418–427. pmid:22882697