Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Chronic pain precedes disrupted eating behavior in low-back pain patients

  • Yezhe Lin,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York, United States of America

  • Ivan De Araujo,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Neuroscience Department, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Gelsina Stanley,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations The John B. Pierce Laboratory, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Dana Small,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations The John B. Pierce Laboratory, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

  • Paul Geha

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    paul_geha@urmc.rochester.edu

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York, United States of America, The John B. Pierce Laboratory, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America

Abstract

Chronic pain is associated with anhedonia and decreased motivation. These behavioral alterations have been linked to alterations in the limbic brain and could explain the increased risk for obesity in pain patients. The mechanism of these behavioral changes and how they set in in relation to the development of chronic pain remain however poorly understood. Here we asked how eating behavior was affected in low-back pain patients before and after they transitioned to chronic pain, compared to patients whose pain subsided. Additionally, we assessed how the hedonic perception of fat-rich food, which is altered in chronic pain patients, related to the properties of the nucleus accumbens in this patients’ population. We hypothesized that the accumbens would be directly implicated in the hedonic processing of fat-rich food in pain patients because of its well-established role in hedonic feeding and fat ingestion, and its emerging role in chronic pain. Accordingly, we used behavioral assays and structural brain imaging to test sub-acute back pain patients (SBP) and healthy control subjects at baseline and at approximately one-year follow-up. We also studied a sample of chronic low-back pain patients (CLBP) at one time point only. We found that SBP patients who recovered at follow-up (SBPr) and CLBP patients showed disrupted eating behaviors. In contrast, SBP patients who persisted in having pain at follow-up (SBPp) showed intact eating behavior. From a neurological standpoint, only SBPp and CLBP patients showed a strong and direct relationship between hedonic perception of fat-rich food and nucleus accumbens volume. This suggests that accumbens alterations observed in SBPp patients in previous works might protect them from hedonic eating disruptions during the early course of the illness. We conclude that disrupted eating behavior specifically sets in after pain chronification and is accompanied by structural changes in the nucleus accumbens.

Introduction

How chronic pain affects eating behavior and body weight is still poorly understood. The premise that appetitive drives such as eating and chronic pain are mechanistically linked is grounded in the fact that both pain and eating interact with motivational states and affect decision making [15]. This hypothetical mechanistic link is of high clinical significance since chronic pain and obesity are often comorbid [6,7]. In fact, chronic pain is more prevalent in obese individuals and obesity is highly prevalent in different types of chronic pain [810].

According to the "fear-avoidance model," pain patients may become obese due to lack of sufficient physical activity. Specifically, chronic pain leads to aversion to movement in pain patients as a way to avoid exacerbating their pain, and later to "deconditioning", and gradually patients become intolerant of physical activity [11]; However, this hypothesis has been met with underwhelming evidence, as previous studies have failed to detect significant changes in physical activity levels in pain patients (as measured by questionnaires or accelerometers) [1217].

An alternative explanation is that chronic pain and obesity interact at the level of the central nervous system. The brain plays a key role in the regulation of energy intake and expenditure [18], with recent findings emphasizing the role of reward limbic circuits in the current obesity epidemic [19,20]. According to this explanation, interindividual differences in brain response to food cues [3,19] interact with a modern diet environment rich in fats and carbohydrates [21], resulting in excessive caloric intake in segments of the population [22]. On the other hand, recent pre-clinical and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that chronic pain is associated with neuroadaptations in the brain limbic system [23], and that the risk of transition to chronic pain can be predicted from the structural and functional properties of the limbic system [2426]. Consistently, both chronic pain and obesity are characterized by anhedonia [2730], hypo-dopaminergic state in the mesocorticolimbic system [3136], and altered opioid transmission within the limbic system [37,38].

Building on this overlap, we have previously shown [17] that chronic low back pain (CLBP) patients exhibit decreased liking of high-fat puddings but not of high sucrose drinks, suggesting an overlap between circuits underlying CLBP and fat valuation. More importantly, we showed that liking predicted caloric intake from a high-fat pudding offered ad-libitum in healthy controls but not in CLBP patients, indicating the presence of disrupted satiety signals in the latter group [17]. Here we asked how eating behavior was affected in low-back pain patients before and after they recovered or transitioned to chronic pain. Therefore, we studied the eating behavior of sub-acute low-back pain patients and healthy controls at baseline and at long-term follow-up after almost one year. Given the role of the nucleus accumbens (NAc) in the hedonic perception of highly palatable food and subjective liking ratings [3941], we also tested whether NAc volume predicted hedonic ratings and/or caloric intake in our participants. This analysis tested our overall hypothesis of the direct relationship between limbic brain changes in low-back pain and disrupted eating behavior.

Methods

Subjects

All subjects gave written informed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the Yale University Institutional Review Board. We received > 500 responses to our ads from patients complaining of back pain. 68 chronic low-back pain (CLBP) patients completed the full phone screening, 43 (17 males) of whom participated in the study; 97 SBP subjects completed the full phone screening, 51 (31 males) of whom participated in the study; 79 healthy controls were screened, 36 of whom participated in our study. The study consisted of two time points separated by approximately one year (baseline, 1-year follow-up). At each time point, participants completed 2 sessions (Fig 1). 36 healthy controls (HC) (20 males), 51 SBP subjects, and 43 chronic low back pain (CLBP) patients completed the baseline visits and finished session 1 experiments. Of the SBP group, 20 patients were confirmed at follow-up as recovered subacute back pain patients (SBPr) and 16 as persistent subacute back pain patients (SBPp). Patients whose pain dropped by more than 30% [42] at follow-up were considered recovered, otherwise persistent. 41 SBP, 33 CLBP and 30 healthy subjects finished experiments of session 2 at baseline, 20 of whom were SBPr and 16 were SBPp. At follow-up roughly 1 year from baseline, the same experiments were repeated. 16 HC and 29 SBP (16 SBPr and 13 SBPp) subjects completed session 1. 15 SBPr, 12 SBPp and 15 HC subjects completed session 2. Subjects were recruited through flyers in the New Haven area and advertisements on the internet. Subjects were briefly screened at first to check (1) the location of back-pain, (2) if they were otherwise healthy, (3) if they were non-smokers, and (4) their pain duration (between 6 and 12 weeks for SBP and more than one year for CLBP). If they passed this initial brief screen, a more detailed screen was conducted where we assessed demographics, location, possible cause, duration, radiation of the pain, analgesic medication use, medical work-up of the back-pain, substance misuse, recent or past history of opioid medication use, complete medical and psychiatric history, recent or past fluctuations in body weight, history of olfactory or taste impairments, or nasal sinuses surgery. To be included in the study, SBP subjects needed to meet the criteria of having a new-onset 6 to12 weeks low-back pain with no back pain in the year prior to the ongoing episode; CLBP patients had to have a pain duration of at least 1 year. Both SBP and CLBP participants had to (1) fulfill the International Association for the Study of Pain criteria for back pain [43], (2) not be currently, or during the month prior to the study, on any opioid analgesics. We chose to include CLBP patients with at least 1 year of back pain to ensure that they were in the time window when brain reorganization had set in [26,44]. Patients were included if their back pain was below the T-12 thoracic vertebra with or without radiculopathy and was present on more days than not. SBP and CLBP diagnoses were confirmed based on history collected by an experienced clinician (P.G.). Healthy control subjects were screened likewise with the additional exclusion criteria of any current back pain or any history of any pain of more than 6 weeks in duration as an exclusion criterion. Participants had no history of psychiatric disorders, chronic medical conditions (e.g., diabetes, coronary artery disease), loss of consciousness, chemosensory impairment, or food allergies.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schematic diagram showing the experiments performed at both time-points and during each session.

Note that CLBP patients were tested once only, while SBP patients and healthy subjects were tested at two time points separated by approximately one year (i.e., baseline and follow-up).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.g001

Stimuli

A set of four pudding samples were prepared with 0%, 3.1%, 6.9%, and 15.6% fat content weight by weight (w/w) [45]. The samples were prepared by mixing instant pudding in heavy cream or milk (Guida’s Dairy) with varying fat content. The sugar content was held constant between the four stimuli at 4.6% (w/w). To maximize liking (i.e., hedonic) ratings, subjects were asked to pick a preferred flavor from a choice of vanilla and chocolate during the pre-study screening interview. A set of four orange or strawberry-flavored Jell-Os with 0, 0.1, 0.56, and 1 Molar (M) sucrose concentration respectively were prepared and represented the sugar stimuli.

Procedures

Fig 1 is a schematic diagram describing the procedures done and the groups undergoing these procedures at baseline and at long-term follow-up (~ 1 year). Subjects were asked to participate in 2 sessions on 2 different days. During session 1, they sampled and rated food stimuli as described below without ingestion. During session 2, they were offered a preload of Mac & Cheese and a pudding to consume ad libitum. The maximum interval between the two sessions was 7 days. We successfully implemented a similar experimental design in our previous study [17].

Session 1.

Subjects presented to the laboratory between 9 am and 3 pm. They were asked to arrive neither hungry nor full and to rate their hunger level upon arrival using a visual analogue scale (VAS: 0 = “I am not hungry at all” and 100 = “I have never been more hungry”). If they rated hunger greater than 30, they were given a small snack and were asked to wait 30 minutes, after which the hunger ratings were repeated. We felt it important to test subjects in the absence of hunger or satiety in order to minimize homeostatic effects on food liking. Before testing, each subject was trained to use the general Labeled Magnitude Scale (gLMS) to rate overall intensity, and sweetness [46], the Labeled Hedonic Scale to rate liking or disliking (LHS) [47], and the VAS to rate hunger, fullness, thirst, oiliness, fattiness, creaminess, and wanting of the stimuli. The gLMS is a computerized psychophysical tool that requires subjects to rate the perceived intensity of a stimulus along a vertical axis lined with adjectives that are spaced semi-logarithmically, based upon experimentally determined intervals to yield ratio-quality data. The LHS was derived using similar methods as the gLMS but asks subjects to rate hedonic liking or disliking [47].

The pudding and Jell-O stimuli were presented in 3 blocks with the order of presentation randomized (i.e., 3 blocks of Jell-O followed by 3 blocks of pudding). Subjects sampled 5 cc of the Jell-O at the tip of a spoon and expectorated without swallowing; likewise, they sampled around 3–5 cc of the pudding at the tip of a spoon without swallowing. After tasting each sample, subjects used the scales to rate their perceptions. They rinsed in between samples and paused for 30 seconds before tasting the next sample. Subjects were blindfolded during the experiment to prevent visual stimulation (i.e., color and consistency of the stimuli) from biasing their ratings. At the end of the session, another set of hunger, fullness, and thirst ratings were obtained.

Session 2.

Subjects presented hungry around lunchtime between 12 and 2 pm. They were asked to eat breakfast and then refrain from eating anything until the time of testing. They were allowed to drink unflavored water up until an hour before the session. First, percent body fat was assessed using air displacement plethysmography (Bod-Pod, COSMED, Italy). Since the percent body fat considered healthy differs in men and women (21–25% range in men and 30–35% range in women) [48,49], we normalized (i.e., divided) the absolute output values from plethysmography by 31% for women and 21% for men. Immediately after, subjects rated hunger, fullness, and thirst, and were offered a set amount of Mac&Cheese as a caloric preload. The preload was intended to mimic a meal and its caloric content was at 25% of their basal metabolic rate. Next, they were offered the pudding to which they gave the highest liking rating during session 1 and instructed to eat as much as they liked. Hunger, fullness and thirst were rated following the ad libitum pudding consumption. Subjects also provided ratings for intensity, liking, sweetness, familiarity, oiliness, fattiness, creaminess, saltiness and wanting after consumption.

Questionnaires.

Subjects were asked to fill out feeding behavior questionnaires, and patients also completed pain related questionnaires. Back-pain was assessed using the following pain questionnaires and scales: VAS for pain intensity, the short-form of the McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ) [50], the Washington Neuropathic Pain Scale (NPS) [51], and the pain DETECT questionnaire [52]. All subjects filled out the Beck’s Anxiety Index (BAI) [53], the Beck’s Depression Index (BDI) [54], and the Edinburgh Inventory Handedness Scale (EIHS). Feeding behavior was assessed in all subjects using the following questionnaires: impulsivity was measured with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (BIS-11) [55]; eating style was measured with the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) [56], the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) [57], the Power of Food Scale (PFS)[58], and the Binge Eating Scale (BES) [59]; reward sensitivity was measured with Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System (BIS/BAS) [60]; hours of physical exercise in the past 7 days was reported using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) [61]; specific food addiction problems were assessed by using Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) [62].

MRI data acquisition parameters

Participants underwent an anatomical T1-weighted scan. Siemens 3.0 T Trio magnet equipped with a 32-channel head-coil was used to acquire the images. MPRAGE 3D T1-weighted acquisition sequence was as follows: TR/TE = 1900/2.52 ms, flip angle, 9°, matrix 256 x 256 with 176-1mm slices.

Calculations of nucleus accumbens volume

Structural data were analyzed with the standard automated processing stream of the Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB) software library (FSL) 5.0.10, which shows high reliability across laboratories [63]. The analysis sequence includes skull extraction, a two-stage linear subcortical registration, and segmentation using Integrated Registration and Segmentation Tool (FIRST) [64], which is part of FMRIB. The volumes of right and left nucleus NAc were calculated for each participant and normalized to standard Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) space. The normalization coefficient was calculated using FSL SIENAX [65].

Statistical analysis.

Statistical analysis was performed with STATISTICA 13 (Stat Soft, Inc.). We performed between-group analyses using unpaired t-tests and 2-way mixed measures ANOVAs with stimulus concentration considered as a repeated within subject measure. Ratings were averaged across the 3 presentations for each stimulus concentration. Paired t-tests were used to examine within-group effects (e.g. hunger before and after each session) and regression analyses to investigate the relationships between caloric intake or volume and internal state or psychophysical ratings. Correction for sex is indicated when included. Identification and exclusion of outliers followed the Tukey method [66]. Figs were generated using Sigmaplot 14.0. Bonferroni was used for repeated measurement corrections. Since our starting hypothesis centered around disrupted hedonic ratings in low-back pain patients, differences in liking ratings were not corrected for multiple comparisons and were considered significantly different if p < 0.05. Comparisons for other ratings were corrected for multiple comparisons when reported as significant (i.e., p < 0.05/8 = 0.00625, where 8 is the number of different food attributes we collected). All relevant data are within the manuscript and its S1S7 Figs and S1S10 Tables.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Demographic and clinical data for all participants at both baseline and follow-up are presented in Tables 13. At baseline, the average age, sex distribution, handedness, BMI, percentage body fat, years of education, and anxiety levels did not differ between SBP, CLBP, and HC (Table 1). On average, depression scores were different between the groups (BDI, p = 0.003; ANOVA), with CLBP reporting the highest score (7.61 ± 1.45). However, all three groups had less than minimal depression (BDI score < 9). There was no difference in the eating style or in the average number of hours spent in physical activity during the past 7 days before testing (IPAQ, p = 0.96) between the groups (Table 1). CLBP patients reported significantly higher back-pain intensity on the visual analogue scale (VAS) compared to SBP patients (CLBP = 4.50 ± 0.33; SBP = 3.38 ± 0.29, p = 0.01). SBPr and SBPp patients did not differ from HC on demographic characteristics, eating style, or physical activity at baseline or at follow-up (Tables 2 and 3). SBPr and SBPp patients exhibited different pain qualities. SBPp patients showed lower affective pain (SF-MPQ-Affective; SBPr = 3.6 ± 0.7 versus SBPp = 1.3 ± 0.7, p = 0.03) at baseline but higher sensory pain at follow-up (MPQ-Sensory; SBPr = 2.4 ± 0.5 versus SBPp = 5.7 ± 1.0, p = 0.005). A significantly higher proportion of CLBP patients than SBP patients or HC reported that high-fat and/or high-sugar food are “problematic” for them on the YFAS (S1 Fig). Surprisingly, when we separated the SBP group into SBPp and SBPr patients at baseline and follow-up, we observed that a higher proportion of SBPr patients reported high-fat and/or high-sugar food to be problematic for them than of the HC subjects (S2 and S3 Figs).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of all groups baseline a.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of SBP patients at baseline a.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Demographic and clinical characteristics of SBP patients at follow-up a.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.t003

Internal state and perception of stimuli in low-back pain and healthy subjects at session 1

Internal state.

At baseline, SBP, CLBP and HC subjects did not differ on hunger, fullness or thirst ratings (repeated measures ANOVA, group effects). Only hunger ratings showed time effects (i.e., changes of internal state before compared to after session 1) (F1,105 = 8.00; p = 0.006), and there was no group by time interaction for hunger, fullness, or thirst ratings between the groups. (S4 Fig left and S1 Table). SBPr, SBPp, and HC groups did not differ on hunger, fullness, and thirst ratings during session 1 at baseline. There was neither a time effect nor a group by time effect (Fig 2A–2C left, S2 Table). At follow-up, during session 1, SBPr, SBPp and HC did not differ on hunger, fullness, or thirst ratings. There was no significant time effect or group by time effect except for hunger ratings which showed a significant time effect (F1,40 = 8.27, p = 0.006) (Fig 2A–2C right, S3 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Internal state ratings before and after session 1.

(A) Hunger, (B) Fullness, and (C) Thirst rated by SBPr, SBPp and HC (baseline, left; follow-up, right). There were no time and groups effects in any of the other analyses. Abbreviations: SBPr, recovered subacute back pain patients; SBPp, persistent subacute back pain patients; HC, Healthy Control. *, p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.g002

Pudding ratings.

Puddings containing 4 different concentrations of fat (0%, 3.1%, 6.9% and 15.6% weight by weight) were sampled without ingestion during session 1. At baseline, SBP, CLBP and HC subjects exhibited no difference in pudding liking (Fig 3A left), intensity (S5A Fig left), sweetness, familiarity, fattiness, creaminess, oiliness, or wanting of pudding (mixed-measures ANOVA, group effect, S4 Table). However, the three groups showed a group x concentration interaction (p = 0.042) for ratings of pudding liking, and concentration effects for ratings of liking (p < 0.005) (Fig 3A left, S4 Table). Similarly, SBPr, SBPp and HC subjects did not differ on any of the pudding ratings at baseline (Fig 3B left, S5 Table), but the 3 groups showed a group x concentration interaction (p = 0.005) for ratings of pudding liking driven mostly by a decrease in liking ratings reported by SBPr patients for increasing concentrations of fat (Fig 3B left, S5 Table). In addition, creaminess increased significantly with fat concentration (p < 0.001) (S5 Table). At follow-up, SBPr, SBPp an HC groups differed on pudding liking ratings (p < 0.05), a difference driven mostly by the lower liking ratings of SBPr patients (Fig 3C left, S6 Table). Here, also, creaminess increased significantly with fat concentration (p < 0.005) (S6 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Psychophysical ratings (liking) during session 1 at baseline and follow-up.

(A) shows results for SBP (n = 51), CLBP (n = 43 for pudding and n = 41 for Jello) and healthy control subjects (n = 36) at baseline; (B) shows results for SBPr (n = 20), SBPp (n = 16) and HC (n = 36) at baseline; and (C) for SBPr (n = 15 for pudding and n = 13 for Jello), SBPp (n = 13) and HC (n = 16 for pudding and n = 14 for Jello) at follow-up. (A–C) (Left) Participants’ ratings for each of the 4 fat concentrations of puddings. (Middle) Ratings for each of the 4 sucrose concentrations of flavored Jello. (Far right) Bar plot depicting mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) across all the concentrations for pudding (left) and jello (Right). No significant difference were found among groups. Adding covariates, including pretest hunger rating and sex, did not alter the main results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.g003

Jello ratings.

At baseline, SBP, CLBP and HC subjects did not differ on liking (Fig 3A middle) or any other rating of flavored jello (S5A Fig middle and S4 Table). These results are consistent with our previous findings of normal ratings of sweetened food in CLBP patients [17]. There was a strong increase in ratings of jello attributes with increasing concentration of sucrose, except for oiliness, but no significant group x concentration effect (Fig 3A middle, S5A Fig and S4 Table). Similarly, SBPr, SBPp and HC groups did not differ on any of the flavored Jell-O ratings but showed a strong increase in the ratings of Jell-O attributes with increasing concentration of sucrose at both baseline and follow-up (Fig 3B and 3C middle, S5B and S5C Fig and S5 and S6 Tables)

Low-back pain disrupts the association between subjective liking and caloric consumption

On the day of the ad libitum feeding assessment (session 2), we collected our subjects’ internal state ratings (hunger, fullness, and thirst) before and after consumption and ratings of the food attributes after consumption.

Internal state.

At baseline, SBP, CLBP and healthy subjects did not differ on hunger, fullness, or thirst (S7 Table). Ratings of hunger and fullness but not thirst changed (p < 0.001) after a preload of Mac & Cheese and ad libitum pudding consumption (time effect) in session 2 (S4AS4C Fig right and S7 Table). Internal state ratings did not differ at baseline or follow-up between SBPr, SBPp and HC subjects (Fig 4A–4C, S7 Table). Hunger and fullness but not thirst ratings changed significantly (p < 0.001) following Mac & Cheese and ad libitum pudding consumption (time effect) at both baseline and follow-up (S7 Table, Fig 4A–4C middle and right).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Internal state ratings before and after the ad libitum consumption of the preferred pudding during session 2.

(A) Hunger, (B) Fullness, and (C) Thirst rated by SBPr, SBPp and HC (baseline, left; follow-up, right). There were no time or group effects in any of the analyses. Hunger and fullness ratings changed significantly after pudding consumption in all groups (A-B); thirst ratings did not differ following between groups or after consumption. There was no group or interaction effect during session 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.g004

Foods ratings.

Ratings of liking, intensity, sweetness, familiarity, saltiness, fattiness, creaminess, oiliness, or wanting for Mac&Cheese and pudding did not differ among CLBP, SBP and HC groups (S8 Table and S6 Fig). These ratings were also not different between SBPp, SBPr and HC groups at baseline or follow-up (S9 and S10 Tables and S6 Fig). CLBP, SBP and HC groups’ total caloric consumption from preferred puddings was not significantly different (p = 0.33) (Table 4). We wanted to investigate whether the magnitude of liking ratings of the preferred pudding collected during session 1 predicted caloric intake when participants came back for pudding consumption during session 2 as we previously studied [17]. Liking of preferred pudding predicted significantly caloric intake in HC (r = 0.60, p = .001) but not in SBP (r = 0.14, p = 0.40) or CLBP patients (r = 0.13, p = 0.49), indicating, as hypothesized, that the relationship between liking of food and caloric ingestion is absent in the patients’ groups (Fig 5A).

thumbnail
Fig 5.

Regression plots of the most preferred pudding liking reported during session 1 and the caloric ingestion from that pudding during session 2 at baseline (A-B) and at follow-up (C). (A) Regression plots for SBP (n = 37), CLBP (n = 32) and healthy control subjects (n = 28) at baseline; (B) Regression plots for SBPr (n = 17) and SBPp (n = 14) groups at baseline; (C) Regression plots for SBPr (n = 15), SBPp (n = 12) and HC (n = 15) subjects at follow-up. **, p < 0.01, *, p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.g005

SBPr, SBPp and HC groups’ total caloric consumption from preferred puddings did not significantly differ at baseline (p = 0.44), or follow-up (p = 0.22) (Table 4). Interestingly, at baseline the prediction of caloric intake from the magnitude of liking ratings of the most preferred pudding sampled during session 1 was close to significance and stronger in the at risk SBPp patients (r = 0.38, p = 0.16, Fig 5B right) than in SBPr patients (r = - 0.14, p = 0.53, Fig 5B middle). This picture persisted at follow-up. As such, the prediction of caloric intake from the magnitude of liking ratings was close to significance in SBPp patients, (r = 0.52, p = 0.08, Fig 5C right) but was weaker and was not statistically significant in SBPr patients (r = 0.21, p = 0.45, Fig 5C, middle). The same relationship remained significant in healthy subjects at follow-up (r = 0.56, p = 0.025, Fig 5C left). It is worth noting that the correlation between liking and caloric intake in SBPr patients showed a trend of recovery between baseline and follow-up becoming stronger in magnitude and changing from negative to positive, and hence becoming more similar to the one observed in HC (Fig 5B and 5C, middle). Finally, we tested for any significant change in BMI or body fat content in SBPp, SBPr, and HC between baseline and follow-up. There were no significant changes or group x time interaction (S7 Fig).

Hedonic perception of high fat pudding is associated with the limbic brain in SBPp and CLBP patients only

The LNAc volume was significantly and strongly correlated with the magnitude of liking ratings of high fat pudding in SBPp and CLBP patients only (Fig 6). While the correlation was large at baseline (r = - 0.68, p < 0.05) and follow-up (r = 0.71, p < 0.05) in SBPp patients, it changed direction from negative to positive respectively and, at follow-up, was consistent in direction with the correlation observed in CLBP patients (Fig 6). The volume of NAc did not predict caloric intake from pudding in any of our groups.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Nucleus accumbens volume in centimeter cubes (cc) predicts highly palatable food liking only in patients at risk for chronic pain (SBPp) and in CLBP patients.

(A-C) Regression (Spearman rank) between LNAc volume and average liking derived from the most liked pudding sampled in session 1 at baseline and in (D-F) at follow-up. (G) Regression between LNAc volume and liking in CLBP patients. *, p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.g006

Discussion

This work adds evidence to our hypothesis [17] that back pain patients’ eating behavior differs significantly from the eating behavior of healthy subjects not in pain, and affects specifically hedonic processing [27,29,30]. The aim of this work is to further understand whether back pain patients diverge from healthy subjects in their eating behavior with the onset of sub-acute back-pain or as the pain becomes chronic. Surprisingly, while at baseline hedonic ratings and the relationship of food liking to caloric intake were mostly preserved in SBPp patients, the relationship of liking to caloric intake was disrupted (absent) in SBPr patients. At follow-up, hedonic blunting worsened in SBPr patients, but their satiety signals started looking more like those of healthy controls, while SBPp patient’s eating behavior remained practically within normal limit. We did not observe hedonic blunting in the CLBP patients but we confirmed the absent relationship between liking of highly palatable fat-rich food and caloric consumption from that food as we have previously shown [17]. We also confirmed that hedonic blunting in SBPp and CLBP patients does not generalize to sweetened food. In addition to the behavioral findings, more CLBP patients reported on the YFAS [67] that high-fat/high carbohydrate foods (e.g. ice cream, cookies) were problematic for them than SBP patients or healthy controls. These reports are consistent with our hypothesis that altered cortico-limbic [25,26,44,6875] pathways in CLBP patients entails altered food choices and eating behavior. Our results suggest, therefore, that eating behavior changes dynamically over the course of development of CLBP, and disruptions set in after the onset of the chronic phase since the SBPp patients show normal eating behavior up to one year after the onset of pain.

An important finding in this work is the observation that the presence of back-pain in the sub-acute phase and the early-chronic phase is not sufficient by itself to explain the abnormal eating behavior in SBP patients. While SBPr patients showed signs of blunted hedonic ratings and disrupted satiety signals (i.e., absent relationship of liking to caloric intake), SBPp patients showed normal eating behavior. Both groups reported similar average low-back pain intensity at baseline, but SBPr patients’ average back-pain was only 1.5, while SBPp patients’ average back-pain was 3.5 at follow-up. SBPr patients’ cortico-limbic pathways have shown signs of resilience according to neuroimaging studies. For example, SBPr patients showed widespread increased fractional anisotropy in major white matter bundles even higher than in healthy controls [76]; in addition, in a previous report on the same SBPr patients studied here, we found no difference in their nucleus accumbens volume or activity compared to healthy controls and a significant increase in their amygdala volume at baseline compared to SBPp patients, [26] which did not persist at follow-up. A functional accumbens is needed in animals to associate aversive or rewarding cues with outcomes [77]. A normal accumbens in SBPr patients allows therefore low-back pain to disrupt the relationship between hedonic perception and caloric intake. A dysfunctional accumbens in SBPp patients, on the other hand, protects them from this disruption, where hedonic ratings become strongly tied to the nucleus accumbens properties as in CLBP patients. This is evidenced by the strong correlation between LNAc volume and hedonic perception in SBPp and CLBP patients only. Hence, plastic changes in cortico-limbic pathway associated with resilience to pain could initially disrupt eating behavior in SBPr patients, which then recovers with time as back-pain subsides. It is notable also that the relationship between hedonic ratings and NAc volume changes direction in all groups from baseline to follow-up. We ascribe this change to learning mechanisms where a first exposure leads to memory formation which is then re-activated at the follow-up experiment. The fact that this change in direction is across the board indicates that it is not a confounding effect.

The lack of blunting of hedonic rating in the CLBP cohort or in the SBPp patients at follow-up observed in this work is inconsistent with our previous report [17]. This discrepancy may be explained by important changes in the experimental testing procedures; in this work, all participants were blinded while tasting food stimuli in session 1. In our previous work, we blinded participants only when they were tasting the chocolate flavored pudding and unblinded them when they were tasting vanilla flavored pudding because we were able to make all vanilla flavored stimuli look alike. It seems, therefore, that the cognitive-perceptual visual processing of offered stimuli has a sizable effect on how patients rate their hedonic experience. This also implies that SBPr patients could show further hedonic blunting were we to shift back to the previous testing procedures.

Others and we have shown that structural and functional plasticity in the NAc is directly involved in CLBP [26,44,68,70]. As such, a smaller accumbens and an increased accumbens to medial prefrontal cortex functional connectivity are associated with a higher risk of transitioning to CLBP [26,44]; in addition, the accumbens exhibits a signature loss of low frequency fluctuations in CLBP that is not observed during the sub-acute phase [26]. On the other hand, the accumbens role in mediating hedonic reactions to food [40,78] and fat intake via μ-opioid binding is well established [79]. The blunted hedonic reactions and disrupted satiety signals affecting highly palatable fat-rich foods in back-pain patients are therefore consistent with the overlapping neurobiology of back-pain and pleasure systems in the brain. Notably, in both this work and our previous report [17], we did not observe any group differences pertaining to the sensory experience of the presented stimuli or the hedonic ratings of sweetened food, consistent with the described role of μ-opioid binding in the accumbens in selectively enhancing fat intake [80]. The normal eating behavior concerning sweetened foods is also consistent with a recent rodent study [81] showing that rats with neuropathic pain have normal hedonic and motivational responses to sweetened food (sucrose water and sucrose pellets). Future studies will need to test whether μ-opioid receptor activation or blockade alters hedonic responses in CLBP patients by, for example, using an opioid agonist like morphine or an opioid antagonist like naloxone.

Increased anhedonia predicts long-term increase in BMI [82,83] in individuals not suffering from clinical pain. We did not observe a change in BMI or adiposity in SBPr, SBPp and HC groups at follow-up. It remains to be determined, therefore, whether abnormal hedonic signals accounts for the association between obesity and CLBP long-term. Nevertheless, BMI and adiposity were matched between the groups and are, therefore, unlikely to account for the observed differences in eating behavior. Substance misuse is another condition associated with motivational disruptions, and anhedonia is associated with increased substance use. Consistent with our current and previous findings [17], Garland et al. [27], have reported that chronic pain patients exhibit anhedonia independently of opioid prescription status, but that chronic pain patients misusing opioids exhibit significantly higher anhedonia than medication free or opioid-treated patients who were not misusing their prescription. Our results are complementary to these findings and offer a novel behavioral approach to directly measure anhedonia which is usually assessed by questionnaires [84].

In summary, we uncovered a complex relationship between hedonic perception of highly palatable fat-rich food and the development of CLBP and reproduced our previous finding [17], namely that, unlike healthy controls, hedonic responses to fat-rich food fail to predict caloric intake in low-back pain patients. We also find a strong and dynamic relationship between hedonic perception and the NAc volume in at-risk SBPp patients as they transition from the sub-acute to the chronic phase of back pain. Future longitudinal studies of longer duration than ours are needed to understand the long-term impact of altered eating behavior on BMI and adiposity of CLBP patients.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Percentage of participants from each group reporting “problems” with dietary items listed in YFAS.

A higher proportion of CLBP subjects reported problems with food high in sugar or fat (eg. ice cream, p = 0.04; cookies, p = 0.02; hamburger, p = 0.02; rice, and p = 0.02; Pearson Chi-square test) than HC. The proportion of participants reporting no problem with different food items was significantly higher in HC than CLBP. The proportion of SBP patients was somewhere in the middle for several items (e.g. ice cream, chocolate). Abbreviations: CLBP, chronic low back pain; SBP, subacute back pain; HC, Healthy Control; YFAS, Yale Food Addiction Scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Problematic food listed in YFAS for SBP groups and HC at baseline.

SBPr, subacute back pain recovered; SBPp, subacute back pain persistent; HC, Healthy Control; YFAS, Yale Food Addiction Scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Fig. Problematic food listed in YFAS for SBP groups and HC at follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Fig. Internal ratings of SBP, CLBP and HC subjects during session 1 (left) and 2 (right).

(A) Hunger rated by SBP, CLBP and HC participants; (B) Fullness; (C) Thirst.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Fig. Psychophysical ratings (intensity) during session 1 at baseline and follow-up.

A for SBP (n = 51), CLBP (n = 43 for pudding and n = 41 for Jello) and healthy control subjects (n = 36) at baseline; B for SBPr (n = 20), SBPp (n = 16) and HC (n = 36) at baseline and C for SBPr (n = 15 for pudding and n = 13 for Jello), SBPp (n = 13) and HC (n = 16 for pudding and n = 14 for Jello) at follow-up. (A–C) (Left) Participants’ ratings for each of the 4 fat concentrations of puddings. (Middle) Ratings for each of the 4 sucrose concentrations of orange-flavored Jello. (Far right) Bar plot depicting mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) across all the concentrations for pudding (left) and juice (right). No group effect, concentration effect or interaction was found. Adding covariates, including pretest hunger rating and sex did not affect the main results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s005

(DOCX)

S6 Fig. Psychological ratings during session 2 at baseline and follow-up.

(A) Ratings of MacCheese (left) and pudding (right) for SBP, CLBP and HC participants at baseline; (B) Ratings of MacCheese (left) and pudding (right) for SBPr, SBPp and HC participants at baseline; (C) Ratings of MacCheese (left) and pudding (right) for SBPr, SBPp and HC at follow-up. CLBP, chronic low back pain; SBP, subacute back pain; SBPr, subacute back pain recovered; SBPp, subacute back pain persistent; HC, Healthy Control. No significant differences were found among groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s006

(DOCX)

S7 Fig. Body fat change from baseline to follow-up.

No changes in any of the groups were found from baseline to 1-year follow-up. SBPr, subacute back pain recovered; SBPp, subacute back pain persistent; HC, Healthy Control.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s007

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Internal state ratings for SBP and CLBP patients, and for healthy subjects for session 1 at baseline a.

SBP (42), CLBP (37), HC (29). a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Calculated by repeated measure ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s008

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Internal state ratings for SBPr and SBPp patients, and for healthy subjects for session 1 at baseline a.

a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Calculated by repeated measure ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s009

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Internal state ratings for SBPr and SBPp patients, and for healthy subjects for session 1 at follow-up a.

a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Calculated by repeated measure ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s010

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Comparison of SBP, CLBP patients’ and healthy subjects’ ratings of puddings and jello during session 1 a.

a F Values are results of a mixed 2-way ANOVA where group (SBP vs CLBP vs healthy) was a factor and stimulus concentration the repeated measure. * p < .05, ** p < .005, *** p < .001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s011

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Comparison of SBPr, SBPp patients’ and healthy subjects’ ratings of puddings and jello during session 1 baseline a.

a F Values are results of a mixed 2-way ANOVA where group (SBPr vs SBPp vs healthy) was a factor and stimulus concentration the repeated measure. * p < .05, ** p < .005, *** p < .001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s012

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Comparison of SBPr, SBPp patients’ and healthy subjects’ ratings of puddings and jello during session 1 follow-up a.

a F Values are results of a mixed 2-way ANOVA where group (SBPr vs SBPp vs healthy) was a factor and stimulus concentration the repeated measure. * p < .05, ** p < .005, *** p < .001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s013

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Internal state ratings for session 2 at baseline and follow-up a.

a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Calculated by repeated measure ANOVA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s014

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Consumed food attribute ratings for CLBP and SBP patients, and healthy subjects at for session 2 at baseline a.

a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Results of a one-way ANOVA among groups (SBP vs CLBP vs healthy).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s015

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Consumed food attribute ratings for SBPr, SBPp patients and healthy subjects for session 2 at baseline a.

a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Results of a one-way ANOVA among groups (SBP vs CLBP vs healthy).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s016

(DOCX)

S10 Table. Ratings for SBPr, SBPp and HC groups at session 2 at follow-up.

a Values are expressed as mean ± SEM. b Results of a one-way ANOVA among groups (SBP vs CLBP vs healthy).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263527.s017

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Fields H. State-dependent opioid control of pain. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5(7):565–75. pmid:15208698.
  2. 2. Porreca F, Navratilova E. Reward, motivation, and emotion of pain and its relief. Pain. 2017;158 Suppl 1:S43–S9. pmid:28106670; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5350036.
  3. 3. Kenny PJ. Reward mechanisms in obesity: new insights and future directions. Neuron. 2011;69(4):664–79. pmid:21338878
  4. 4. van Steenbergen H, Eikemo M, Leknes S. The role of the opioid system in decision making and cognitive control: A review. Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience. 2019;19(3):435–58. Epub 2019/04/10. pmid:30963411; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6599188.
  5. 5. Laurent V, Morse AK, Balleine BW. The role of opioid processes in reward and decision-making. Br J Pharmacol. 2015;172(2):449–59. pmid:24930675; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4292959.
  6. 6. Shiri R, Karppinen J, Leino-Arjas P, Solovieva S, Viikari-Juntura E. The association between obesity and low back pain: a meta-analysis. AmJEpidemiol. 2010;171(2):135–54. pmid:20007994
  7. 7. Brady SRE, Urquhart DM, Hussain SM, Teichtahl A, Wang Y, Wluka AE, et al. High baseline fat mass, but not lean tissue mass, is associated with high intensity low back pain and disability in community-based adults. Arthritis research & therapy. 2019;21(1):165. Epub 2019/07/07. pmid:31277706; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6612201.
  8. 8. Wilson AC, Samuelson B, Palermo TM. Obesity in children and adolescents with chronic pain: associations with pain and activity limitations. ClinJPain. 2010;26(8):705–11. pmid:20664337
  9. 9. Allen SA, Dal Grande E, Abernethy AP, Currow DC. Two colliding epidemics—obesity is independently associated with chronic pain interfering with activities of daily living in adults 18 years and over; a cross-sectional, population-based study. BMC public health. 2016;16(1):1034. Epub 2016/10/08. pmid:27716147; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5045593.
  10. 10. Okifuji A, Hare BD. The association between chronic pain and obesity. J Pain Res. 2015;8:399–408. Epub 2015/07/24. pmid:26203274; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4508090.
  11. 11. Verbunt JA, Seelen HA, Vlaeyen JW, van de Heijden GJ, Heuts PH, Pons K, et al. Disuse and deconditioning in chronic low back pain: concepts and hypotheses on contributing mechanisms. EurJPain. 2003;7(1):9–21. pmid:12527313
  12. 12. Bousema EJ, Verbunt JA, Seelen HA, Vlaeyen JW, Knottnerus JA. Disuse and physical deconditioning in the first year after the onset of back pain. Pain. 2007;130(3):279–86. pmid:17467902
  13. 13. van Weering MG, Vollenbroek-Hutten MM, Tonis TM, Hermens HJ. Daily physical activities in chronic lower back pain patients assessed with accelerometry. EurJPain. 2009;13(6):649–54. pmid:18752975
  14. 14. Verbunt JA, Westerterp KR, van der Heijden GJ, Seelen HA, Vlaeyen JW, Knottnerus JA. Physical activity in daily life in patients with chronic low back pain. ArchPhysMedRehabil. 2001;82(6):726–30. pmid:11387574
  15. 15. Lin CW, McAuley JH, Macedo L, Barnett DC, Smeets RJ, Verbunt JA. Relationship between physical activity and disability in low back pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain. 2011;152(3):607–13. Epub 2011/01/22. pmid:21251757.
  16. 16. Patel KV, Dansie EJ, Turk DC. Impact of chronic musculoskeletal pain on objectively measured daily physical activity: a review of current findings. Pain Manag. 2013;3(6):467–74. Epub 2014/03/25. pmid:24654901; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4006369.
  17. 17. Geha P, Dearaujo I, Green B, Small DM. Decreased food pleasure and disrupted satiety signals in chronic low back pain. Pain. 2014;155(4):712–22. pmid:24384160.
  18. 18. Berthoud HR, Munzberg H, Morrison CD. Blaming the Brain for Obesity: Integration of Hedonic and Homeostatic Mechanisms. Gastroenterology. 2017;152(7):1728–38. Epub 2017/02/14. pmid:28192106; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5406238.
  19. 19. de Araujo IE, Schatzker M, Small DM. Rethinking Food Reward. Annual review of psychology. 2020;71:139–64. Epub 2019/09/29. pmid:31561741.
  20. 20. Vainik U, Garcia-Garcia I, Dagher A. Uncontrolled eating: a unifying heritable trait linked with obesity, overeating, personality and the brain. The European journal of neuroscience. 2019;50(3):2430–45. Epub 2019/01/23. pmid:30667547.
  21. 21. Finkelstein EA, Ruhm CJ, Kosa KM. Economic causes and consequences of obesity. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:239–57. pmid:15760288.
  22. 22. Hall KD. Did the Food Environment Cause the Obesity Epidemic? Obesity. 2018;26(1):11–3. Epub 2017/12/22. pmid:29265772; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5769871.
  23. 23. Elman I, Borsook D. Common Brain Mechanisms of Chronic Pain and Addiction. Neuron. 2016;89(1):11–36. pmid:26748087.
  24. 24. Baliki MN, Petre B, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nature neuroscience. 2012;15(8):1117–9. Epub 2012/07/04. pmid:22751038; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3411898.
  25. 25. Vachon-Presseau E, Tetreault P, Petre B, Huang L, Berger SE, Torbey S, et al. Corticolimbic anatomical characteristics predetermine risk for chronic pain. Brain. 2016;139(Pt 7):1958–70. pmid:27190016; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4939699.
  26. 26. Makary MM, Polosecki P, Cecchi GA, DeAraujo IE, Barron DS, Constable TR, et al. Loss of nucleus accumbens low-frequency fluctuations is a signature of chronic pain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2020:201918682. pmid:32312809
  27. 27. Garland EL, Trostheim M, Eikemo M, Ernst G, Leknes S. Anhedonia in chronic pain and prescription opioid misuse. Psychol Med. 2019:1–12. Epub 2019/08/20. pmid:31422776.
  28. 28. Keranen AM, Rasinaho E, Hakko H, Savolainen M, Lindeman S. Eating behavior in obese and overweight persons with and without anhedonia. Appetite. 2010;55(3):726–9. pmid:20801180.
  29. 29. Case LK, Ceko M, Gracely JL, Richards EA, Olausson H, Bushnell MC. Touch Perception Altered by Chronic Pain and by Opioid Blockade. eNeuro. 2016;3(1). Epub 2016/03/30. pmid:27022625; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4785385.
  30. 30. Boehme R, van Ettinger-Veenstra H, Olausson H, Gerdle B, Nagi SS. Anhedonia to Gentle Touch in Fibromyalgia: Normal Sensory Processing but Abnormal Evaluation. Brain Sci. 2020;10(5). Epub 2020/05/24. pmid:32443443; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7288027.
  31. 31. Stice E, Yokum S, Zald D, Dagher A. Dopamine-based reward circuitry responsivity, genetics, and overeating. CurrTopBehavNeurosci. 2011;6:81–93.
  32. 32. Hagelberg N, Forssell H, Aalto S, Rinne JO, Scheinin H, Taiminen T, et al. Altered dopamine D2 receptor binding in atypical facial pain. Pain. 2003;106(1–2):43–8. pmid:14581109
  33. 33. Hagelberg N, Forssell H, Rinne JO, Scheinin H, Taiminen T, Aalto S, et al. Striatal dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in burning mouth syndrome. Pain. 2003;101(1–2):149–54. pmid:12507709
  34. 34. Hagelberg N, Jaaskelainen SK, Martikainen IK, Mansikka H, Forssell H, Scheinin H, et al. Striatal dopamine D2 receptors in modulation of pain in humans: a review. Eur J Pharmacol. 2004;500(1–3):187–92. pmid:15464032
  35. 35. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. NatNeurosci. 2010;13(5):635–41. pmid:20348917
  36. 36. van Galen KA, Ter Horst KW, Booij J, la Fleur SE, Serlie MJ. The role of central dopamine and serotonin in human obesity: lessons learned from molecular neuroimaging studies. Metabolism: clinical and experimental. 2018;85:325–39. Epub 2017/10/04. pmid:28970033.
  37. 37. Nummenmaa L, Saanijoki T, Tuominen L, Hirvonen J, Tuulari JJ, Nuutila P, et al. mu-opioid receptor system mediates reward processing in humans. Nature communications. 2018;9(1):1500. Epub 2018/04/18. pmid:29662095; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5902580.
  38. 38. Karlsson HK, Tuominen L, Tuulari JJ, Hirvonen J, Parkkola R, Helin S, et al. Obesity is associated with decreased mu-opioid but unaltered dopamine D2 receptor availability in the brain. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35(9):3959–65. Epub 2015/03/06. pmid:25740524; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6605573.
  39. 39. Kuhn S, Gallinat J. The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness. Neuroimage. 2012;61(1):289–94. pmid:22406357
  40. 40. Smith KS, Berridge KC. Opioid limbic circuit for reward: interaction between hedonic hotspots of nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum. JNeurosci. 2007;27(7):1594–605. pmid:17301168
  41. 41. Bartra O, McGuire JT, Kable JW. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage. 2013;76:412–27. Epub 2013/03/20. pmid:23507394; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3756836.
  42. 42. Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Farrar JT, Haythornthwaite JA, Jensen MP, Katz NP, et al. Core outcome measures for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain. 2005;113(1–2):9–19. pmid:15621359.
  43. 43. Treede R-D. The International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: as valid in 2018 as in 1979, but in need of regularly updated footnotes. Pain Rep. 2018;3(2):e643–e. pmid:29756089.
  44. 44. Baliki MN, Petre B, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. NatNeurosci. 2012. pmid:22751038
  45. 45. Mennella JA, Finkbeiner S, Reed DR. The proof is in the pudding: children prefer lower fat but higher sugar than do mothers. International journal of obesity. 2012;36(10):1285–91. pmid:22546773; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3429629.
  46. 46. Green BG, Dalton P, Cowart B, Shaffer G, Rankin K, Higgins J. Evaluating the ’Labeled Magnitude Scale’ for measuring sensations of taste and smell. ChemSenses. 1996;21(3):323–34. pmid:8670711
  47. 47. Lim J, Wood A, Green BG. Derivation and evaluation of a labeled hedonic scale. ChemSenses. 2009;34(9):739–51.
  48. 48. Snitker S. Use of body fatness cutoff points. Mayo Clinic proceedings Mayo Clinic. 2010;85(11):1057; author reply -8. pmid:21037049; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2966372.
  49. 49. De Lorenzo A, Bianchi A, Maroni P, Iannarelli A, Di Daniele N, Iacopino L, et al. Adiposity rather than BMI determines metabolic risk. International journal of cardiology. 2013;166(1):111–7. pmid:22088224.
  50. 50. Melzack R. The short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Pain. 1987;30(2):191–7. pmid:3670870
  51. 51. Galer BS, Jensen MP. Development and preliminary validation of a pain measure specific to neuropathic pain: the Neuropathic Pain Scale. Neurology. 1997;48(2):332–8. pmid:9040716
  52. 52. Freynhagen R, Baron R, Gockel U, Tolle TR. painDETECT: a new screening questionnaire to identify neuropathic components in patients with back pain. CurrMedResOpin. 2006;22(10):1911–20. pmid:17022849
  53. 53. Beck A, Steer R. Manual for the Beck Anxiety Inventory. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1993.
  54. 54. Beck A, Steer R. Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1993.
  55. 55. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J ClinPsychol. 1995;51(6):768–74. pmid:8778124
  56. 56. Van ST, Rookus MA, Bergers GP, Frijters JE, Defares PB. Life events, emotional eating and change in body mass index. IntJ Obes. 1986;10(1):29–35.
  57. 57. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J PsychosomRes. 1985;29(1):71–83. pmid:3981480
  58. 58. Lowe MR, Butryn ML, Didie ER, Annunziato RA, Thomas JG, Crerand CE, et al. The Power of Food Scale. A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment. Appetite. 2009;53(1):114–8. pmid:19500623.
  59. 59. Gormally J, Black S, Daston S, Rardin D. The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addictive behaviors. 1982;7(1):47–55. pmid:7080884.
  60. 60. CS C, TL W. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation,and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:319–33.
  61. 61. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2003;35(8):1381–95. pmid:12900694.
  62. 62. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale food addiction scale. Appetite. 2009;52(2):430–6. pmid:19121351
  63. 63. Nugent AC, Luckenbaugh DA, Wood SE, Bogers W, Zarate CA Jr, Drevets WC. Automated subcortical segmentation using FIRST: test–retest reliability, interscanner reliability, and comparison to manual segmentation. Human brain mapping. 2013;34(9):2313–29. pmid:22815187
  64. 64. Patenaude B, Smith SM, Kennedy DN, Jenkinson M. A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. Neuroimage. 2011;56(3):907–22. pmid:21352927
  65. 65. Smith SM, Zhang Y, Jenkinson M, Chen J, Matthews P, Federico A, et al. Accurate, robust, and automated longitudinal and cross-sectional brain change analysis. Neuroimage. 2002;17(1):479–89. pmid:12482100
  66. 66. Tukey JW. Exploratory data analysis. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co.; 1977. xvi, 688 p. p.
  67. 67. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite. 2009;52(2):430–6. pmid:19121351.
  68. 68. Kaneko H, Zhang S, Sekiguchi M, Nikaido T, Makita K, Kurata J, et al. Dysfunction of Nucleus Accumbens Is Associated with Psychiatric Problems in Patients with Chronic Low Back Pain—A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2016. pmid:27755492.
  69. 69. Mao C, Wei L, Zhang Q, Liao X, Yang X, Zhang M. Differences in brain structure in patients with distinct sites of chronic pain: A voxel-based morphometric analysis. Neural Regen Res. 2013;8(32):2981–90. pmid:25206618; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4146206.
  70. 70. Baliki MN, Geha PY, Fields HL, Apkarian AV. Predicting value of pain and analgesia: nucleus accumbens response to noxious stimuli changes in the presence of chronic pain. Neuron. 2010;66(1):149–60. pmid:20399736
  71. 71. Xie P, Qin B, Song G, Zhang Y, Cao S, Yu J, et al. Microstructural Abnormalities Were Found in Brain Gray Matter from Patients with Chronic Myofascial Pain. Front Neuroanat. 2016;10:122. pmid:28066193; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5167736.
  72. 72. Geha PY, Baliki MN, Harden RN, Bauer WR, Parrish TB, Apkarian AV. The brain in chronic CRPS pain: abnormal gray-white matter interactions in emotional and autonomic regions. Neuron. 2008;60(4):570–81. pmid:19038215; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2637446.
  73. 73. Kong J, Spaeth RB, Wey HY, Cheetham A, Cook AH, Jensen K, et al. S1 is associated with chronic low back pain: a functional and structural MRI study. Molecular pain. 2013;9:43. pmid:23965184; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3765748.
  74. 74. Apkarian AV, Sosa Y, Sonty S, Levy RE, Harden R, Parrish T, et al. Chronic back pain is associated with decreased prefrontal and thalamic gray matter density. J Neurosci. 2004;24:10410–5. pmid:15548656
  75. 75. Loggia ML, Kim J, Gollub RL, Vangel MG, Kirsch I, Kong J, et al. Default mode network connectivity encodes clinical pain: an arterial spin labeling study. Pain. 2013;154(1):24–33. pmid:23111164; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3534957.
  76. 76. Mansour AR, Baliki MN, Huang L, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Schnitzer TJ, et al. Brain white matter structural properties predict transition to chronic pain. Pain. 2013;154(10):2160–8. pmid:24040975; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3799881.
  77. 77. West EA, Carelli RM. Nucleus accumbens core and shell differentially encode reward-associated cues after reinforcer devaluation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36(4):1128–39. pmid:26818502
  78. 78. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron. 2015;86(3):646–64. Epub 2015/05/08. pmid:25950633; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4425246.
  79. 79. Baldo BA, Pratt WE, Kelley AE. Control of Fat Intake by Striatal Opioids. 2010.
  80. 80. Zhang M, Gosnell BA, Kelley AE. Intake of high-fat food is selectively enhanced by mu opioid receptor stimulation within the nucleus accumbens. JPharmacolExpTher. 1998;285(2):908–14. pmid:9580643
  81. 81. Okun A, McKinzie DL, Witkin JM, Remeniuk B, Husein O, Gleason SD, et al. Hedonic and motivational responses to food reward are unchanged in rats with neuropathic pain. Pain. 2016;157(12):2731–8. Epub 2016/08/23. pmid:27548047; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5108682.
  82. 82. Cho J, Goldenson NI, Pester MS, Khoddam R, Bello MS, Dunton GF, et al. Longitudinal Associations Between Anhedonia and Body Mass Index Trajectory Groups Among Adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2018;63(1):81–7. Epub 2018/05/08. pmid:29731318; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6067955.
  83. 83. Ibrahim M, Thearle MS, Krakoff J, Gluck ME. Perceived stress and anhedonia predict short-and long-term weight change, respectively, in healthy adults. Eat Behav. 2016;21:214–9. Epub 2016/03/24. pmid:27002703; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4851568.
  84. 84. Snaith RP, Hamilton M, Morley S, Humayan A, Hargreaves D, Trigwell P. A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science. 1995;167(1):99–103. pmid:7551619.