Figures
Abstract
Estimating population density and population dynamics is essential for understanding primate ecology and relies on robust methods. While distance sampling theory provides a robust framework for estimating animal abundance, implementing a constrained, non-systematic transect design could bias density estimates. Here, we assessed potential bias associated with line distance sampling surveys along roads based on a case study with olive baboons (Papio anubis) in Lake Manyara National Park (Tanzania). This was achieved by comparing density estimates of olive baboons derived from road transect surveys with density estimates derived from estimating the maximum number of social groups (via sleeping site counts) and multiplying this metric with the estimated average size of social groups. From 2011 to 2019, we counted olive baboons along road transects, estimated survey-specific densities in a distance sampling framework, and assessed temporal population trends. Based on the fitted half-normal detection function, the mean density was 132.5 baboons km-2 (95% CI: 110.4–159.2), however, detection models did not fit well due to heaping of sightings on and near the transects. Density estimates were associated with relatively wide confidence intervals that were mostly caused by encounter rate variance. Based on a generalized additive model, baboon densities were greater during the rainy seasons compared to the dry seasons but did not show marked annual trends. Compared to estimates derived from the alternative method (sleeping site survey), distance sampling along road transects overestimated the abundance of baboons more than threefold. Possibly, this overestimation was caused by the preferred use of roads by baboons. While being a frequently used technique (due to its relative ease of implementation compared to spatially randomized survey techniques), inferring population density of baboons (and possibly other species) based on road transects should be treated with caution. Beyond these methodological concerns and considering only the most conservative estimates, baboon densities in LMNP are among the highest across their geographic distribution range.
Citation: Kiffner C, Paciência FMD, Henrich G, Kaitila R, Chuma IS, Mbaryo P, et al. (2022) Road-based line distance surveys overestimate densities of olive baboons. PLoS ONE 17(2): e0263314. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263314
Editor: Stephanie S. Romanach, U.S. Geological Survey, UNITED STATES
Received: August 29, 2021; Accepted: January 14, 2022; Published: February 2, 2022
Copyright: © 2022 Kiffner et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The data underlying the results presented in the study are available from GöttingenResearchOnline https://doi.org/10.25625/6FJPIG.
Funding: SK Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) KN1097/4-1 DZ Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) ZI548/5-1 DFG CK research funds of the The School for Field Studies.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Population size and population trajectories over time are key parameters for understanding the ecology of animal communities and guiding species-specific conservation efforts. In many habitats, particularly in tropical forests, primate species constitute a major component of animal communities and provide multiple ecosystem services as folivores, frugivores, seed dispersers, seed predators, pollinators, and as prey and predators [1, 2].
Primate taxa, such as baboons (Papio spp.), also play key ecological roles in savanna woodland habitats [3–6] and have numerous impacts on the ecosystem. The magnitude of these impacts is likely dependent on their absolute population size. Baboons can be highly effective seed dispersers, which becomes particularly obvious in the case of the invasive Neotropical prickly-pears (Opuntia sp.), which is spreading in many parts of Africa [7–9]. Moreover, the abundance of baboons and their ecological role might change in ecosystems where humans have drastically reduced their predators or competitors. For example, in three protected areas in Ghana, where apex predators such as lions (Panthera leo) and leopards (P. pardus) had been extirpated, the population density of olive baboons has increased manifold. Concomitantly baboons took over the role of mesopredators with negative impacts on smaller ungulates and other taxa [10].
Baboons are listed as Least Concern by the IUCN [11] and they are regionally central to human-wildlife conflicts [12, 13]. Baboons are also reservoir hosts for zoonotic disease agents (e.g., treponematoses [14] and tuberculosis [15]). Risk assessment, simulation, and management of disease outbreaks require realistic data on the absolute population size of reservoir species. Thus, monitoring baboon populations is key for understanding their population dynamics and associated ecological and epidemiological effects.
Lake Manyara National Park (LMNP), is a small protected area in northern Tanzania that is part of the greater Tarangire ecosystem. The park represents a site with available long-term data on the abundance of olive baboons (Papio anubis). Based on time series from 1959 to 2016, the density of olive baboons showed an apparent exponential increase between 2011 and 2016 (see Fig 2 in [16]). However, this manifold density increase (c. 14 fold greater density in the last decade compared to the first decade of the time series) coincided with a change in methodology from attempted total counts from 1959 to 2009 to line distance sampling along roads from 2011 to 2016. Due to this change in monitoring methods, it is unclear whether this apparent shift in population density has indeed occurred or is merely method-related. To better gauge the actual olive baboon density in LMNP, we here attempt to assess potential bias associated with line distance surveys carried out along roads and compare density estimates from this method with an alternative method to approximate the abundance of olive baboons; such comparative assessments are typically recommended when assessing potential biases of wildlife monitoring methods [17, 18].
Line distance sampling is frequently used for monitoring ungulates in East Africa [19, 20], and it is also a common tool for estimating the size of primate populations [21–27]. While this method explicitly accounts for imperfect detectability [28], line distance surveys are occasionally carried out along existing path or road networks due to safety concerns, restrictions on off-road driving in protected areas, or logistical or budgetary constraints associated with walking transects [29]. Extrapolating estimated densities from non-randomly distributed transects to the wider study area could, however, result in substantial sampling bias because roads generally do not represent the entire study area [30].
Even though baboons are largely terrestrial and also use open habitats, counting baboons via total counts can be a veritable challenge. Due to limited visibility and constrained access, the key assumptions of total counts (i.e. all individuals are detected with certainty and no individuals are counted multiple times) are unlikely met. However, if it would be feasible to estimate the maximum number of social groups and the group size of olive baboons, it should be possible to establish an upper limit of olive baboon abundance in a given area. Olive baboons live in discrete, relatively large (up to 200 individuals) stable multi-male multi-female groups (Fig 1), and home ranges of neighboring groups overlap substantially [31–33]. Generally, baboon groups use tall trees or steep cliffs as sleeping sites to avoid predation [34]. Where cliffs are available, they are often preferred sleeping sites [35, 36]. Usually, a group uses more than one sleeping site within its home range, and the same site can be used several days in a row before switching sites [37–39]. These sites, in particular those in cliffs, are often used over multiple generations. The same sleeping site can be alternately used by more than one group; occasionally two groups use the same site simultaneously. Thus, even at high olive baboon densities, it is reasonable to assume that not all sleeping sites in an area are continuously occupied, i.e., there are usually more sleeping sites than social groups in an area. Therefore, the number of unique sleeping sites should represent the maximum number of social groups. Thus, while the method does not provide a precise estimate of the baboon density in a given area, multiplying the number of sleeping sites—which in the case of LMNP are mostly cliffs and well known by park rangers—with the average group size of baboons in the area should provide an upper limit for the abundance of baboons in LMNP.
In this paper, we first present density estimates of olive baboons based on line distance sampling carried out seasonally along roads from 2011–2019 and contrast these estimates with density estimates approximated by counting sleeping sites and the average size of social groups (the latter estimates should represent an upper limit for olive baboon density). Based on this comparison, we critically discuss monitoring methods for olive baboons and highlight possible implications of biased wildlife population assessments and high baboon densities in protected areas.
Methods
Permission for conducting this research has been granted by the Tanzanian Wildlife Research Institute (TAWIRI) and the Tanzania Commission for Science and Technology (COSTECH; permits 2012-241-NA-2012-57 through 2020-130-NA-2013-191, as well as 2015-87-NA-2014-228, 2015-89-NA-2014-228, 2016-114-NA-2014-228 and 2016-115-NA-2014-228).
Study site
The core area of LMNP (where we conducted our field surveys) is a narrow stretch of land (168 km2 in extent, centered approximately at 3.55°S and 35.78°E) located in the Eastern Rift Valley, in between the escarpment and the alkaline Lake Manyara (Fig 2). The climate is classified as semi‐arid to semi‐humid with a bimodal rainfall pattern characterized by the long rains from February to May, a dry season from June to October, and a short rainy season from November to December [40]; annual precipitation averages 600 mm and ranges from 430 to 1060 mm (data for 2011–2018 from the weather station at the LMNP headquarters). Relative fertile soils (mainly hardpan, plain soil, and clay) and a high groundwater table (facilitated by the water catchment of the Karatu highlands to the west of the park) ensure high vegetative productivity [41]. The vegetation is characterized by a patchwork of habitats including grasslands near the lake, Acacia (Vachellia) woodlands, and escarpment woodlands as well as lush groundwater forests and riverine vegetation [42, 43]. Since the late 1980s, the vegetation density in the bush layer of wooded habitats has increased substantially, and compared to historic baselines, densities of several large-bodied mammals have declined markedly over the last decades [16, 44].
Outline of Lake Manyara National Park (LMNP), positions of transects (straight lines), baboon sleeping sites (black dots), and baboon sightings during surveys from late 2015 to 2019 (red dots). The boundaries of the park shown here depict the boundaries of the park before its extension in the southwest (Marang Forest) in 2012. The inset in the top-left indicates the location of the study area in northern Tanzania. All transects and sleeping sites are located inside the borders of LMNP.
Line distance surveys
As part of a multi-species wildlife monitoring program in the ecosystem [45], we carried out road transect surveys in LMNP from 2011 to 2019. We used the existing road network because dense vegetation and off-road restrictions did not allow for a more systematic distribution of transects throughout the park. However, as the road network crosses the park from north to south, transects cover all major habitat types of LMNP. The roads are primarily used for tourism and the speed limit in the park is 50 km h-1. In general, motorists drive defensively and give wildlife the right of way.
Starting in the short rains 2011, we conducted three surveys per year to capture the main seasons; in 2019 we only conducted surveys during the long and the short rainy season. We conducted long rain surveys from the end of February until April; dry season counts between July and August and short rain counts between the end of September until the beginning of November. Short rains, therefore, mostly refer to the end of the dry season. Transects were 2 km in length and were separated by 500 m (measured by the odometer of the vehicle) to increase independence between transects. We drove along transects at slow speed (5 to 20 km h-1) with open-top vehicles and stopped the vehicle whenever trained observers (N = 3 to 9) detected olive baboons (or other target species). Upon detection, we identified the species, counted baboon individuals in a cluster, and measured the perpendicular distance between the transect and the approximate center of the cluster with a laser range finder (Bushnell Elite 1500).
To use a consistent definition for one animal cluster, we defined a cluster as individuals of the same species that were not separated by more than 50 m from other individuals of the same species; this distance is a common threshold for defining group sizes [46]. We differentiate between the size of a social group and the size of a cluster, whereby a cluster refers to the number of animals fulfilling the above definition and that can be a part of a social group. If olive baboons moved before the vehicle was in a perpendicular position relative to the transect and the animal cluster, we measured the distance to the initial location of the cluster [47]. With minor adjustments due to variable road conditions over time, we used the same transects during each of the 24 surveys (Fig 2). We carried out surveys between 7:30 am and 18:30 and completed each survey within one day using one to five vehicles. When using multiple vehicles simultaneously, each vehicle was assigned a particular, non-overlapping section of the park. Therefore, double counting due to baboon movement is unlikely to have biased density estimates. On a few occasions (n = 6), we carried out up to five surveys within a few days. In those cases, we combined the effort and baboon sightings of the same transect to estimate densities [47]; when repeating surveys within a few days, we varied the sequence of transects. Prior to all surveys, observers were trained in mammal species identification, line distance methodology, and field methodology, with particular emphasis on accurate distance measurements to the center of the animal and the initial location of the cluster and on exact counting of individuals in a cluster [28, 47–49].
We analyzed the data [50] in a distance sampling framework using the program DISTANCE 6.0 [28]. To obtain the required sample size (N = 60–80) for robust modeling of detection functions [47], we combined sightings across surveys and modeled global detection functions for the baboon sightings along road transects. We considered six candidate models to estimate the detectability of baboons: four conventional distance sampling (CDS) models where detectability is a function of perpendicular distance to the transect and two multiple covariate distance (MCDS) models. For the two MCDS models, we hypothesized that the detectability of baboons varies by season (i.e., lower visibility when vegetation is lush vs. dry; season defined as a categorical variable with three levels: dry season; short rains, long rains). For the CDS candidate models, we considered uniform, half-normal, negative exponential, and hazard rate key functions with cosine series expansion; for the MCDS candidate models we considered half-normal and hazard rate key functions with cosine expansion.
To select an appropriate detection model, we compared AIC scores of competing models and also considered the visual fit of the curve near the transect line and overall goodness of fit based on Kolmogorov-Smirnov test statistics [28]. In all models, we truncated 10% of the furthest detections, estimated cluster size based on the mean cluster size during each survey, and used the post-stratification setting to produce season-specific density estimates including associated 95% confidence intervals.
We used R 3.6 [51] for additional data analysis and illustration of results. To assess broad temporal trends, we used the mean density estimates (derived from the chosen distance model) as a response variable in a generalized additive model (GAM), implemented in the mgcv package [52]. In this GAM, we modeled year as a smooth term (dimension of the basis k was set to 3 to allow sufficient wiggliness of the curve) and season as a categorical variable (three levels: dry season; short rains, long rains). To illustrate our results, we predicted the values of the GAM and overlaid the predictions with the observed density estimates using the ggplot2 package [53].
Approximating the abundance maximum of olive baboons and methods comparison
In 2015 and 2016, we systematically recorded the locations of all baboon sleeping sites in LMNP as part of ongoing field research on the behavioral ecology of Treponema pallidum subsp. pertenue (TPE) infected olive baboons [54, 55]. Sleeping sites were surveyed in the evening when groups were already near the sleeping areas and would not travel until the next day. Sites were located with the help of LMNP rangers, who have worked in the park for multiple years. Because sleeping sites of olive baboons are conspicuous and due to the extensive field experience of our field crew and the LMNP rangers, we are confident that we detected all sleeping sites of our study area.
When visibility was good, olive baboons in each social unit were counted by the same two observers. Otherwise, due to limited visibility and constrained accessibility, we assumed group sizes of olive baboons to range between 150 and 200 individuals, which might be at the upper end of group sizes in LMNP. This group size variation was based on total counts of the habituated study troop (170 individuals) and opportunistic observations of other baboon troops over a time period of 24 months in LMNP [54, 55]. We estimated the abundance of olive baboons by multiplying the value of the group size (lower bound 150; mean: 175, upper bound: 200) with the known number of sleeping sites (35, see Fig 2). Given that the number of sleeping sites might be slightly higher than the number of baboon groups and that the assumed group sizes are most likely also at the upper end, the estimated population size might be an overestimation of the “true” population size and thus represents an upper limit of the olive baboon abundance in LMNP.
We compared this abundance maximum with the mean abundance of olive baboons (± 95% confidence intervals) estimated by distance sampling along road transects from 2011 to 2019. Our population density estimate refers to the 168 km2 terrestrial area of LMNP and excludes the lake and the highland area of the Marang Forest [16].
Results
Line distance sampling
We detected clusters of olive baboons in each of the 24 seasonal surveys and detection functions were modeled based on 475 unique detections. Based on the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, none of the tested detection function models fitted the observation data well (Table 1).
Model selection indicated that conventional distance models were more supported than MCDS models. The most supported detection function was the hazard rate key function, followed by negative exponential, half-normal, and uniform detection functions (Table 1).
However, hazard rate and negative exponential key functions–the two most supported models according to AIC scores–overestimated detectability near the transect line (Fig 3A and 3B; note the gap between fitted line and observed data), and estimated strip widths to be narrow (Table 1). Because these detection functions would result in very high (and possibly inflated) density estimates, we chose the half-normal detection model as a more conservative model to estimate seasonal density estimates (this model underestimated detectability near the transect line).
Detection functions (red line) of conventional distance sampling models for olive baboons sighted along road transects in Lake Manyara National Park (Tanzania). The histograms (blue bars) show the observed frequency of sightings in each distance bin; detection functions were modeled using a) hazard rate, b) negative exponential, c) half-normal, and d) uniform key functions with cosine series extensions.
The mean observed cluster size of baboon sightings was 28 (range = 1–190), and the observed cluster size did not vary significantly (p-values of negative binomial regression coefficients >0.092) across seasons (Fig 4); nevertheless, we used survey-specific cluster size estimates for extrapolating from cluster to animal density.
Box-plots (bold midline indicates the median, the upper and lower limits of the box represent the third and first quartiles) of observed baboon cluster sizes during seasonal (LR: Long rains; Dry: Dry season; SR: Short rains) line distance surveys in Lake Manyara National Park (Tanzania). The jittered grey points indicate observed cluster sizes.
Across the survey period, the mean baboon density was 132.5 individuals km-2 (95% CI: 110.4–156.2). Seasonal density estimates fluctuated considerably over time (Fig 5), yet we did not detect a marked yearly trend of baboon densities. Estimated baboon densities were substantially greater during the two rainy seasons than during the dry season counts, whereas this difference was significant for the long rains vs dry season comparison only (Fig 5; and see Table 2 for details of the GAM coefficients).
Confidence intervals associated with seasonal density estimates were relatively wide (Fig 5) and were primarily driven by variation in encounter rates (mean % contribution to estimated variation = 60%), and cluster size (mean % contribution to estimated variation = 39%). Uncertainty associated with the detection model (i.e., CDS detection model with half normal key function) accounted for one percent of the width of the estimated confidence intervals.
Comparing olive baboon densities between survey methods
During fieldwork from 2015 to 2016, we recorded a total of 35 sleeping sites that were regularly used by olive baboon troops (Fig 2). Multiplied with the approximated mean troop size of 175 individuals (lower bound 150, upper bound 200), we estimated that the population maximum of olive baboons in LMNP was around 6,125 (range 5,250–7,000) individuals. This approximation represents the upper limit of olive baboon population size in LMNP, because not every sleeping site was occupied each night. Based on the distance sampling surveys, and extrapolating to the lowland surface area of LMNP (168 km2), the mean abundance of 22,260 baboons (95% CI: 18,547–26,746) was approximately 3.6 times greater than the sleeping site survey estimate.
Discussion
By comparing two methods for estimating the density of olive baboons in LMNP, we show that method choice can result in manifold (~360%) differences in absolute density estimates of a common primate species. In the following, we will discuss the methodological aspects of our study and highlight the implications of our results.
Monitoring primate populations
In line with Buckland et al. [47], we implemented a consistent field protocol for defining distinct cluster sightings (i.e., defined by a threshold distance of 50 m between individuals) and used laser range finders to estimate perpendicular distances between transect and the center of a baboon cluster. However, none of the tested detection functions were able to accurately describe the observed data near the transect line (see Fig 3 for poor fit of all four tested models at close perpendicular distances). This points to possible problems associated with the detection process and with the representativeness of the road transects for the wider study area. We assume that the observed spike in baboon detections on and near the transect is related to two, mutually non-exclusive reasons. First, when faced with (large) animal clusters near the transect, it may be difficult to accurately measure to the center of the cluster and it could be that observers then assigned those sightings as zero meters from the transect (in particular if baboons were detected on both sides of the road). Theoretically, measuring the perpendicular distance to each individual could overcome this issue [47], yet this may pose logistic challenges given the large group sizes and mobile nature of baboons. Beyond possible measurement issues, heaping of baboon sightings on or near the transect line (in our case, the road) could not be a measurement error but be caused by preferential use of roads by baboons. Indeed, high-resolution movement analyses of olive baboons in Laikipia (Kenya) suggest that baboons frequently followed roads and avoided dense vegetation [56]. As large parts of our study area were covered with very dense understory vegetation [16], the baboons’ propensity to use roads in LMNP may be even greater than in the relatively open savannah vegetation of Laikipia [56]. Thus, the high frequency of baboon sightings near the road transect may not necessarily be a measurement error but a consequence of the baboons’ spatial preference for moving along roads (Fig 1). While potential predators of olive baboons (e.g. leopards, lions, and spotted hyenas Crocuta crocuta [57–60]) may also frequently use unpaved roads in protected areas, these large carnivores are primarily nocturnal in LMNP [61]. As we carried out the line distance surveys between 8 am and 6 pm, olive baboons likely perceived moving along roads during these times not only as convenient but also as relatively safe.
As a consequence, such apparent local attraction to the road network may also partially explain the more than threefold discrepancy between road-based distance sampling estimates and the maximum olive baboon abundance based on the sleeping site count.
Beyond the relative local preference of roads, the use of roads as sampling transects could yield additional bias that operates at larger spatial scales. For example, if roads are not randomly distributed relative to habitat features, sleeping sites, and other areas that are preferred (or avoided) by olive baboons [56], extrapolating densities estimated along roads to the wider study area may further bias density estimates. Indeed, the largest component contributing to the confidence intervals of our density estimates was variation in encounter rates, contributing to 60% of the observed variance (range across surveys: 35 to 81%), suggesting that, during a given survey, baboon sightings occurred relatively unevenly across the surveyed area. While such spatial differences in local densities are to be expected in a group-living species at a given snapshot in time, mapping of olive baboon observations (Fig 2) from late 2015 to 2019, suggests that olive baboons principally used all (surveyed) parts of LMNP across the 2015–2019 period. In principle, if spatial information for all baboon sightings were available, one could model the seasonal density distribution of olive baboons as a function of environmental covariates and this could result in more accurate and more precise estimates [62]. Unfortunately, this was not possible because coordinates of baboon sightings were not recorded systematically during surveys prior to the short rain survey in 2015.
Overall, these considerations suggest that extrapolating olive baboon densities estimated along road-transects to the wider area overestimated olive baboon densities in our case study. Our findings further corroborate other comparative work on primate survey methods and highlight the magnitude of method-related differences for density estimates [e.g., [63, 64]]. In light of these potential sources of bias associated with density estimates derived from road counts, we assume that the density estimate of the sleeping site count (i.e., 31.3–41.7 baboons/km2) is a more realistic approximation for the absolute number of olive baboons in LMNP. While the sleeping site count method is also associated with substantial levels of uncertainty, this estimation method likely presents an upper limit for the baboon density in LMNP because the “true” number of social units may be somewhat lower than the number of sleeping sites. Camera trap distance sampling (implemented via a systematic sampling design) might help here to increase the accuracy of the estimates of baboon abundance. This method does not require the identification of individual baboons and proved to produce unbiased density estimates for other semi-arboreal and group living primate species [65].
Estimation bias associated with sampling along roads is not restricted to primates. For example, while sampling along roads yielded similar agouti (Dasyprocta prymnolopha) densities as sampling along systematically distributed transects, models fitted to road surveys differed in variance contribution, had lower detection probability and poorer fit [66]. For white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus), distance sampling along roads yielded 3.0–7.6 times greater density estimates than systematic aerial surveys [17]. In the case of LMNP, contrasting road-based, distance sampling estimates with (likely more robust) estimates derived by either total count surveys (wildebeest Connochates taurinus and zebra Equus quagga) or mark-resight models (buffalo Syncerus caffer, elephant Loxodonta africana, and giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis) suggests that densities from road surveys can be biased low (e.g. for buffalo) or high (e.g. for wildebeest, zebra, giraffe, and elephant) [16]. Collectively, these comparisons suggest that naively extrapolating densities from road-based distance sampling to the wider study area can yield substantial bias.
While road-based surveys are potentially associated with biased absolute density estimates, temporal trends are unlikely to be affected by a road-based transect design, because we generally used the same transects across the study period and thus kept potential design-based bias constant. Thus, we are confident that the olive baboon population size in LMNP remained relatively constant from 2011 to 2019 (Fig 5).
Olive baboon densities in context.
Even when considering the lowest approximation of the maximum olive baboon density (31.3 baboons km-2) or a sleeping site/social group ratio of 0.75 (this would yield an estimated range of 23.5–31.3 baboons km-2), baboon densities in LMNP appear high, particularly in the context of olive baboon densities in other parts of their geographic range (Table 3). However, comparing species-specific density estimates requires due caution because estimates have been derived with different methods and have been extrapolated to different spatial scales (i.e., home range scale vs. landscape scale).
While providing a frame for comparison, please consider that density estimates are based on different methods, and often, the respective spatial scales to which the estimates refer (e.g., the home range of a primate group vs. landscape scale) are not always clear.
As indicated above, accurate information on baboon abundance is not only important in the ecological and conservation context, but also in the context of zoonoses. A higher population density is associated with increased intraspecific and interspecific contact rates and an increased level of competition for resources. This could favor the spread of communicable diseases and, if olive baboons move outside the boundaries of protected areas (e.g. in search for food resources), increase the risk for spillover transmissions to humans. Monitoring of these ecological processes is vital for risk assessment and pandemic preparedness. When census data of host species are combined with key epidemiological data (e.g. prevalence rates) the spread of infectious diseases such as the currently ongoing TPE epidemic [54, 76] within and across baboon groups in LMNP and other places in Sub-Saharan Africa can be simulated more accurately. Such studies are of fundamental importance for the identification of disease reservoirs [77] and in the case of LMNP, could provide further support for the elimination of yaws in nonhuman primates, the disease caused by TPE [78]. Finally, the census estimates from LMNP will help to understand the long-term consequences of treponematoses in its primate host. It is, for example, unclear whether LMNP’s olive baboon population is negatively affected by T. pallidum infections, which is a chronic disfiguring disease in humans and baboons [79].
Hypothesized causes of high baboon densities
Unfortunately, uncertainty associated with both total counts conducted between 1959 to 2009 and with more recent olive baboon surveys prevents elucidating with certainty whether the olive baboon population has recently increased from 4.5 to 13.1 individuals km-2 (based on total counts [16, 44]) to around 30 individuals km-2 during the 2010s [this study]. Indeed, it is possible that these earlier “total counts” underestimated actual baboon abundance, as previous chief wardens of LMNP mentioned an “overpopulation” of olive baboons at least in the groundwater forest of LMNP during the late 1970s [80].
However, olive baboon density increases of several 100% within a few years are possible and have been observed in protected areas of Ghana. These increases have been linked to local extinctions of apex predators, consistent with predictions of the mesopredator release hypothesis [10]. As a recent camera trap study provides evidence that lion, leopard, and spotted hyena are relatively widely distributed across LMNP [81], it is unlikely that release from top-down control could have been the main driver of the apparent high population density of olive baboons in LMNP. Instead, we suggest that a combination of bottom-up (e.g., a favorable resource base) and top-down forces (e.g., persecution by humans outside the park) could explain the current (i.e. 2011–2019) high density of olive baboons in LMNP. Over the last decades, the boundaries of LMNP were gradually extended, adding more habitat and greater protection to baboons. For example, the southern extension to the park (annexed to the park in 1990) used to be a sugar cane plantation [82] and is now woodland habitat. At the same time, however, the human population grew substantially around LMNP, leading to an expansion of human settlements, agricultural areas, and substantial loss of natural habitat outside LMNP [83–85]. Given the vermin status of olive baboons in Tanzania, one may safely assume that in the past baboons were (and still are) persecuted by humans in communal lands. Collectively, this may have caused (1) an increase in baboon populations in areas where baboons were released from human persecution, and (2) a shift of olive baboon distribution away from human-dominated areas to the more secure national park [86]. Concurrent with shifts in mega-herbivore populations inside LMNP, the vegetation inside the park also changed considerably. In recent years, the understory of most bushland or woodland portions of LMNP is characterized by dense vegetation, which is in stark contrast to the previous predominantly open habitats of LMNP until the mid-1980s [16]. Likely, this bush encroachment creates very favorable conditions for olive baboons by providing sufficient food resources. In sum, these aspects provide a plausible, though not necessarily exhaustive, explanation for the observed high baboon densities in LMNP.
Conclusions
This comparative baboon monitoring study provides evidence that road-based distance sampling overestimates densities of baboons, possibly because baboons are attracted to roads. Given the methodological challenges associated with monitoring primates (and other taxa) [48, 87], comparing monitoring methods with alternative estimation methods seems essential to assess, and ideally correct for potential biases. Beyond these methodological considerations, our monitoring suggests that LMNP is a stronghold for baboons.
Acknowledgments
We sincerely thank all SFS students and staff who participated in the field surveys from 2011 to 2019. We warmly acknowledge the close collaboration with the Conservation Science (Ecology) Department of Tanzanian National Parks (TANAPA) at Lake Manyara National Park as well as the Tanzania Wildlife Research Institute (TAWIRI). Finally, we would like to thank Dr. Alex Piel and another reviewer for their constructive and helpful comments.
References
- 1.
Chapman CA, Gautier-Hion A, Oates JF, Onderdonk DA. African primate communities: Determinants of structure and threats to survival. In: Fleagle JG, Janson C, Reed KEeditors. Primates Communities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999. p. 1–37.
- 2. Chapman CA, Dunham AE. Primate seed dispersal and forest restoration: an African perspective for a brighter future. Int J Primatol. 2018; 39(3): 427–442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-018-0049-3
- 3. Kunz BK, Linsenmair KE. The role of the olive baboon (Papio anubis, Cercopithecidae) as seed disperser in a savanna-forest mosaic of West Africa. J Trop Ecol. 2008a; 24(3): 235–246. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0266467408005014
- 4. Liebermann D, Hall JB, Swaine MD, Liebermann M. Seed dispersal by baboons in the Shai Hills, Ghana. Ecology. 1979; 60: 65–75. https://doi.org/10.2307/1936469
- 5. Tew E, Landman M, Kerley GIH. The contribution of the chacma baboon to seed dispersal in the eastern Karoo, South Africa. Afr J Wildl Res. 2018; 48(2): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3957/056.048.023002
- 6. Timóteo S, Correia M, Rodríguez-Echeverría S, Freitas H, Heleno R. Multilayer networks reveal the spatial structure of seed-dispersal interactions across the Great Rift landscapes. Nat Comm. 2018; 9(1): 140. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02658-y
- 7. Dudenhoeffer M, Hodge AMC. Opposing forces of seed dispersal and seed predation by mammals for an invasive cactus in central Kenya. Afr J Ecol. 2018; 56: 149–184. https://doi.org/10.1111/aje.12504
- 8. Foxcroft LC, Rejmánek M. What helps Opuntia stricta invade Kruger National Park, South Africa: Baboons or elephants? Appl Veg Sci. 2007; 10: 265–270. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1654-109X.2007.tb00525.x
- 9. Strum SC, Stirling G, Mutunga SK. The perfect storm: Land use change promotes Opuntia stricta’s invasion of pastoral rangelands in Kenya. J Arid Environ. 2015; 118: 37–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaridenv.2015.02.015
- 10.
Brashares JS, Prugh LR, Stoner CJ, Epps CW. Ecological and conservation implications of mesopredator release. In: Terborgh J, Estes JAeditors. Trophic Cascades. Predators, Prey, and the Changing Dynamics of Nature. Washington, DC: Island Press; 2010. p. 221–240.
- 11. Wallis J. Papio anubis. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T40647A17953200. https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-2.RLTS.T40647A17953200.en
- 12. Hill CM. Conflict of interest between people and baboons: Crop raiding in Uganda. Int J Primatol. 2000; 21(2): 299–315. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005481605637
- 13.
Warren Y, Higham JP, MacLarnon AM, Ross C. Crop-raiding and commensalism in olive baboons: The costs and benefits of living with humans. In: Sommer V, Ross Ceditors. Primates of Gashaka. Socioecology and Conservation in Nigeria’s Biodiversity Hotspot New York: Springer; 2011. p. 307–332.
- 14. Knauf S, Gogarten JF, Schuenemann VJ, De Nys HM, Düx A, Strouhal M, et al. Nonhuman primates across sub-Saharan Africa are infected with the yaws bacterium Treponema pallidum subsp. pertenue. Emerg Microbes Infect. 2018; 7: 157. pmid:30228266
- 15. Sapolsky RM, Else JG. Bovine tuberculosis in a wild baboon population: epidemiological aspects. J Med Primatol. 1987; 16: 229–235. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0684.1987.tb00331.x pmid:3305954
- 16. Kiffner C, Rheault H, Miller E, Scheetz T, Enriquez V, Swafford R, et al. Long-term population dynamics in a multi-species assemblage of large herbivores in East Africa. Ecosphere. 2017; 8(12): e02027. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecs2.2027
- 17. Beaver JT, Harper CA, Kissell RE Jr, Muller LI, Basinger PS, Goode MJ, et al. Aerial vertical-looking infrared imagery to evaluate bias of distance sampling techniques for white-tailed deer. Wildl Soc Bull. 2014; 38(2): 419–427. https://doi.org/10.1002/wsb.410
- 18. Vink DN, Stewart FA, Piel AK. Comparing methods for assessing chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) party size: Observations, camera traps, and bed counts from a savanna–woodland mosaic in the Issa Valley, Tanzania. Int J Primatol. 2020; 41: 901–915. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-020-00142-x
- 19. Caro TM. Densities of mammals in partially protected areas: the Katavi ecosystem of western Tanzania. J Appl Ecol. 1999; 36(2): 205–217. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2664.1999.00392.x
- 20. Ogutu JO, Bhola N, Piepho HP, Reid R. Efficiency of strip-and line-transect surveys of African savanna mammals. J Zool. 2006; 269(2): 149–160. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00055.x
- 21.
Plumptre AJ, Sterling EJ, Buckland ST. Primate census and survey techniques. In: Sterling EJ, Bynum N, Blair MEeditors. Primate Ecology and Conservation. A Handbook of Techniques. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2013. p. 10–26.
- 22. Araldi A, Barelli C, Hodges K, Rovero F. Density estimation of the endangered Udzungwa red colobus (Procolobus gordonorum) and other arboreal primates in the Udzungwa Mountains using systematic distance sampling. Int J Primatol. 2014; 35(5): 941–956. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-014-9772-6
- 23. Bapureddy G, Santhosh K, Jayakumar S, Kumara HN. Estimate of primate density using distance sampling in the evergreen forests of the central Western Ghats, India. Curr Sci. 2015; 108(1): 118–123. https://www.jstor.org/stable/24216184
- 24. Cavada N, Barelli C, Ciolli M, Rovero F. Primates in human-modified and fragmented landscapes: the conservation relevance of modelling habitat and disturbance factors in density estimation. PLoS ONE 2016; 11(2), e0148289. pmid:26844891
- 25. Morgan D, Sanz C, Onononga JR, Strindberg S. Ape abundance and habitat use in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. Int J Primatol. 2006; 27(1): 147–179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-005-9013-0
- 26. Paddock CL, Bruford MW, McCabe GM. Estimating the population size of the Sanje mangabey (Cercocebus sanjei) using acoustic distance sampling. Am J Primatol. 2020; 82(2): e23083. pmid:31912545
- 27. Wuesthoff EF, Fuller TK, Sutherland C, Kamilar JM, Ramanankirahina R, Rakotondravony R, et al. Differential habitat use by sympatric species of mouse lemurs across a mangrove–dry forest habitat gradient. J Mammal. 2021; 102(5): 1266–1278. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmammal/gyab082
- 28. Thomas L, Buckland ST, Rexstad EA, Laake JL, Strindberg S, Hedley SL, et al. Distance software: Design and analysis of distance sampling surveys for estimating population size. J Appl Ecol. 2010; 47(1): 5–14. pmid:20383262
- 29. Waltert M, Meyer B, Shanyangi MW, Balozi JJ, Kitwara O, Qolli S, et al. Foot surveys of large mammals in woodlands of Western Tanzania. J Wildl Manag. 2008; 72(3): 603–610. https://doi.org/10.2193/2006-456
- 30. Marques TA, Buckland ST, Borchers DL, Tosh D, McDonald RA. Point transect sampling along linear features. Biometrics. 2010; 66(4): 1247–1255. pmid:20105157
- 31.
DeVore I, Washburn SL. Baboon ecology and human evolution. In: Howell FC, Bourlière Feditors. African Ecology and Human Evolution. Chicago: Aldine; 1963. p. 335–367. pmid:14087561
- 32. Rowell TE. Forest living baboons in Uganda. J Zool. 1966; 149: 344–364. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1966.tb04054.x
- 33.
Ransom TW. Beach Troop of the Gombe. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press. 1981.
- 34. Bidner LR, Matsumoto-Oda A, Isbell LA. The role of sleeping sites in the predator-prey dynamics of leopards and olive baboons. Am J Primatol. 2018; 80(12): e22932. pmid:30537388
- 35. Barton R, Whiten A, Strum S, Byrne R, Simpson A. Habitat use and resource availability in baboons. Anim Behav. 1992; 43: 831–844. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80206-4
- 36.
Anandam MV, Bennett EL, Davenport TRB, Davies NJ, Detwiler KM, Engelhardt A, et al. Species accounts of Cercopithecidae. In: Mittermeier RA, Rylands AB, Wilson DEeditors. Handbook of the Mammals of the World. Vol. 3. Primates. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions; 2013. p. 628–753.
- 37. Hamilton WJ. Baboon sleeping site preferences and relationships to primate grouping patterns. Am J Primatol. 1982; 3: 41–53. pmid:31991989
- 38. Hausfater G, Meade BJ. Alternation of sleeping groves by yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) as a strategy for parasite avoidance. Primates. 1982; 23: 287–297. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381167
- 39. Markham AC, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Haven for the night: sleeping site selection in a wild primate. Behav Ecol. 2016; 27(1): 29–35. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv118
- 40. Prins HHT, Loth PE. Rainfall patterns as background to plant phenology in northern Tanzania. J Biogeogr. 1988; 15: 451–463. https://doi.org/10.2307/2845275
- 41. Prins HHT. Plant phenology patterns in Lake Manyara National Park. J Biogeogr. 1988; 15: 465–480. https://doi.org/10.2307/2845276
- 42. Greenway PJ, Vesey-Fitzgerald DF. The vegetation of Lake Manyara National Park. J Ecol. 1969; 57: 127–149. https://doi.org/10.2307/2258212
- 43. Loth PE, Prins HHT. Spatial patterns of the landscape and vegetation of Lake Manyara National Park. ITC Journal. 1986; 2: 115–130.
- 44. Prins HHT, Douglas-Hamilton I. Stability in a multi-species assemblage of large herbivores in East Africa. Oecologia 1990; 83(3): 392–400. pmid:28313013
- 45. Kiffner C, Binzen G, Cunningham L, Jones M, Spruiell F, Kioko J. Wildlife population trends as indicators of protected area effectiveness in northern Tanzania. Ecol Indic. 2020; 110: 105903. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2019.105903
- 46. Kasozi H, Montgomery RA. Variability in the estimation of ungulate group sizes complicates ecological inference. Ecol Evol. 2020; 10(14): 6881–6889. pmid:32760498
- 47.
Buckland ST, Anderson DR, Burnham KP, Laake JL, Borchers DL. Introduction to Distance Sampling. Estimating Abundance of Biological Populations. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2001.
- 48. Buckland ST, Plumptre AJ, Thomas L, Rexstad EA. Design and analysis of line transect surveys for primates. Int J Primatol. 2010; 31(5): 833–847. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-010-9431-5
- 49.
Buckland ST, Rexstad EA, Marques TA, Oedekoven CS. Distance Sampling: Methods and Applications. Cham: Springer. 2015.
- 50.
Zinner D. Baboon Census Data LMNP. Göttingen Research Online / Data, 2021. https://doi.org/10.25625/6FJPIG
- 51.
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2016. http://www.r-project.org/
- 52.
Wood S. Package ‘mgcv’. Mixed GAM Computation Vehicle with Automatic Smoothness Estimation. 2021. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/mgcv/mgcv.pdf
- 53.
Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-98141-3
- 54. Paciência FMD, Rushmore J, Chuma IS, Lipende IF, Caillaud D, Knauf S, et al. Mating avoidance in female olive baboons (Papio anubis) infected by Treponema pallidum. Sci Adv. 2019; 5(12): eaaw9724. pmid:31840059
- 55. Paciência FMD, Chuma IS, Lipende IF, Knauf S, Zinner D. Female post-copulatory behavior in a group of olive baboons (Papio anubis) infected by Treponema pallidum. PLoS ONE. 2022; 17(1): e0261894. pmid:35051197
- 56. Strandburg-Peshkin A, Farine DR, Crofoot MC, Couzin ID. Habitat and social factors shape individual decisions and emergent group structure during baboon collective movement, eLife. 2017; 6: e19505. pmid:28139196
- 57. Havmøller RW, Jacobsen NS, Havmøller LW, Rovero F., Scharff N, Bohmann K. DNA metabarcoding reveals that African leopard diet varies between habitats. Afr J Ecol 2021; 59(1): 37–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/aje.12817
- 58. Hayward MW, Kerley GIH. Prey preferences of the lion (Panthera leo). J Zool. 2005; 267(3): 309–322. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952836905007508
- 59. Hayward MW. Prey preferences of the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta) and degree of dietary overlap with the lion (Panthera leo). J Zool. 2006; 270(4): 606–614. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00183.x
- 60. Hayward MW, Henschel P, O’Brien J, Hofmeyr M, Balme G, Kerley GIH. Prey preferences of the leopard (Panthera pardus). J Zool. 2006; 270(2): 298–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00139.x
- 61. Clauss M, Scriba M, Kioko J, Ganzhorn JU, Kiffner C. Camera-trap data do not indicate scaling of diel activity and cathemerality with body mass in an East African mammal assemblage. Ecol Evol. 2021; 11(20): 13846–13861. pmid:34707822
- 62. Miller DL, Burt ML, Rexstad EA, Thomas L. Spatial models for distance sampling data: recent developments and future directions. Methods Ecol Evol. 2013; 4(11): 1001–1010. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12105
- 63. Brugiere D, Fleury MC. Estimating primate densities using home range and line transect methods: A comparative test with the black colobus monkey Colobus satanas. Primates. 2000; 41(4): 373–382. pmid:30545201
- 64. Mitani JC, Struhsaker TT, Lwanga J. Primate community dynamics in old growth forest over 23.5 years at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda: Implications for conservation and census methods. Int J Primatol. 2000; 21(2): 269–286. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005477504728
- 65. Cappelle N, Després-Einspenner M, Howe E, Boesch C, Kühl H. Validating camera trap distance sampling for chimpanzees. Am J Primatol. 2019; 81(3): e22962. pmid:30811079
- 66. Chiarello AG, Arruda LN. Unpaved roads are not adequate surrogates of true transects for sampling agoutis. Mammalia. 2017; 81(5): 489–501. https://doi.org/10.1515/mammalia-2015-0134
- 67. Nagel U. A comparison of anubis baboons, hamadryas baboons and their hybrids at a species border in Ethiopia. Folia Primatol. 1973; 19(2): 104–165. https://doi.org/10.1159/000155536
- 68. Dunbar RIM, Dunbar EP. Ecological relations and niche separation between sympatric terrestrial primates in Ethiopia. Folia Primatol. 1974; 21: 36–60. pmid:4210698
- 69.
DeVore I, Hall KRL. Baboon ecology. In: DeVore I editor. Primate Behavior. Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1965. p. 20–52.
- 70. Harding RSO. Ranging patterns of a troop of baboons (Papio anubis) in Kenya. Folia Primatol. 1976; 25: 143–185. https://doi.org/10.1159/000155711
- 71. Plumptre AJ, Reynolds V. The effect of selective logging on the primate populations in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. J Appl Ecol. 1994; 31: 631–641. https://doi.org/10.2307/2404154
- 72. Hall JS, White LJT, Williamson EA, Inogwabini BI, Omari I. Distribution, abundance, and biomass estimates for primates within Kahuzi-Biega lowlands and adjacent forest in Eastern DRC. Afr Primates. 2003; 6(1&2): 35–42.
- 73. Kunz BK, Linsenmair KE. The disregarded West: Diet and behavioural ecology of olive baboons in the Ivory Coast. Folia Primatol. 2008b; 79(1): 31–51. pmid:17855793
- 74. Sinsin B, Tehou AC, Daouda I, Saidou A. Abundance and species richness of larger mammals in Pendjari National Park in Benin. Mammalia. 2002; 66(3): 369–380. https://doi.org/10.1515/mamm.2002.66.3.369
- 75. Green AA. Density estimate of the larger mammals of Arli National Park, Upper Volta. Mammalia. 1979; 43(1): 59–70. https://doi.org/10.1515/mamm.1979.43.1.59
- 76. Chuma IS, Roos C, Atickem A, Bohm T, Collins DA, Grillová L, et al. Strain diversity of Treponema pallidum subsp. pertenue suggests rare interspecies transmission in African nonhuman primates. Sci Rep. 2019; 9: 14243. pmid:31578447
- 77. Hallmaier-Wacker LK, Munster VJ, Knauf S. Disease reservoirs: from conceptual frameworks to applicable criteria. Emerg Microbes Infect. 2017; 6: e79. pmid:28874791
- 78. Marks M, Mitjà O, Vestergaard LS, Pillay A, Knauf S, Chen CY, et al. Challenges and key research questions for yaws eradication. Lancet Infect Dis. 2015; 15: 1220–1225. pmid:26362174
- 79.
Knauf S. Clinical Manifestation and Aetiology of a Genital Associated Disease in Olive Baboons (Papio hamadryas anubis) at Lake Manyara National Park, Tanzania. Giessen: VVB Laufersweiler. 2011.
- 80.
Mungure SA. Manyara Monthly Report, Tanzania National Parks. 1980.
- 81. Steinbeiser CM, Kioko J, Aresi A, Kaitilia R, Kiffner C. Relative abundance and activity patterns explain method-related differences in mammalian species richness estimates. J Mammal. 2019; 100(1): 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmammal/gyy175
- 82.
Prins HHT. Ecology and Behavior of the African Buffalo. Social Inequality and Decision Making. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. 1996.
- 83. Msoffe FU, Said MY, Ogutu JO, Kifugo SC, de Leeuw J, van Gardingen P, et al. Spatial correlates of land-use changes in the Maasai- Steppe of Tanzania: Implications for conservation and environmental planning. Int J Biodivers Conserv. 2011; 3(7): 280–290. https://doi.org/10.5897/IJBC.9000125
- 84. Newmark WD. Insularization of Tanzanian parks and the local extinction of large mammals. Conserv Biol. 1996; 10(6): 1549–1556. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1523-1739.1996.10061549.x
- 85. Kiffner C, Wenner C, LaViolet A, Yeh K, Kioko J. From savannah to farmland: Effects of land-use on mammal communities in the Tarangire-Manyara ecosystem, Tanzania. Afr J Ecol. 2015; 53(2): 156–166. https://doi.org/10.1111/aje.12160
- 86. Taylor RA, Ryan SJ, Brashares JS, Johnson LR. Hunting, food subsidies, and mesopredator release: the dynamics of crop-raiding baboons in a managed landscape. Ecology. 2016; 97(4): 951–960. pmid:27220211
- 87. Plumptre A, Cox D. Counting primates for conservation: primate surveys in Uganda. Primates. 2006; 47(1): 65–73. pmid:16132166