Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Direction of attentional focus in prosthetic training: Current practice and potential for improving motor learning in individuals with lower limb loss

  • Szu-Ping Lee ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    szu-ping.lee@unlv.edu

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada, United States of America

  • Alexander Bonczyk ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada, United States of America

  • Maria Katrina Dimapilis ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada, United States of America

  • Sarah Partridge ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada, United States of America

  • Samantha Ruiz ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada, United States of America

  • Lung-Chang Chien ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Nevada, United States of America

  • Andrew Sawers

    Contributed equally to this work with: Szu-Ping Lee, Alexander Bonczyk, Maria Katrina Dimapilis, Sarah Partridge, Samantha Ruiz, Lung-Chang Chien, Andrew Sawers

    Roles Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America

Abstract

Objective

Adopting an external focus of attention has been shown to benefit motor performance and learning. However, the potential of optimizing attentional focus for improving prosthetic motor skills in lower limb prosthesis (LLP) users has not been examined. In this study, we investigated the frequency and direction of attentional focus embedded in the verbal instructions in a clinical prosthetic training setting.

Methods

Twenty-one adult LLP users (8 female, 13 male; 85% at K3 level; mean age = 50.5) were recruited from prosthetic clinics in the Southern Nevada region. Verbal interactions between LLP users and their prosthetists (mean experience = 10 years, range = 4–21 years) during prosthetic training were recorded. Recordings were analyzed to categorize the direction of attentional focus embedded in the instructional and feedback statements as internal, external, mixed, or unfocused. We also explored whether LLP users’ age, time since amputation, and perceived mobility were associated with the proportion of attentional focus statements they received.

Results

We recorded a total of 20 training sessions, yielding 904 statements of instruction from 338 minutes of training. Overall, one verbal interaction occurred every 22.1 seconds. Among the statements, 64% were internal, 9% external, 3% mixed, and 25% unfocused. Regression analysis revealed that female, older, and higher functioning LLP users were significantly more likely to receive internally-focused instructions (p = 0.006, 0.035, and 0.024, respectively).

Conclusions

Our results demonstrated that verbal instructions and feedback are frequently provided to LLP users during prosthetic training. Most verbal interactions are focused internally on the LLP users’ body movements and not externally on the movement effects.

Impact statement

While more research is needed to explore how motor learning principles may be applied to improve LLP user outcomes, clinicians should consider adopting the best available scientific evidence during treatment. Overreliance on internally-focused instructions as observed in the current study may hinder prosthetic skill learning.

Introduction

There are currently more than 2 million people with an amputation living in the United States [1], and approximately 300 to 500 new amputations occur each day [2]. Oftentimes, those who require an amputation have other chronic comorbidities [3]. For example, diabetes is the leading cause of nontraumatic lower limb amputations that accounts for more than half of all amputations in the U.S. [1]. Given the complexity and the chronic nature of causes of amputation, it is important to explore effective rehabilitation strategies for these individuals to address aspects of their disability and to maximize recovery of function after amputation.

Despite the large and increasing number of individuals living with limb loss, current rehabilitation strategies, including prosthetic training, are often inadequate and unstandardized due to a lack of evidence to guide clinical practice [4]. Contemporary research supports the incorporation of motor learning principles to improve motor skill acquisition in clinical settings [5]. Little attention however, has been given to how these concepts can be implemented in prosthetic training [6, 7]. A lack of evidence for effective rehabilitation strategies in regards to post-amputation rehabilitation, and in particular prosthetic skill training, may contribute to suboptimal functional outcomes observed in many lower limb prosthesis (LLP) users despite receiving current standard of care therapy [8].

Empirical evidence from the last two decades have shown how the direction of attentional focus significantly affects motor learning and performance [5, 9]. This theory states that by instructing learners to focus on controlling the motions of body segments or muscle contractions (i.e. internal focus), it may interfere with the natural motor control processes. In contrast, an external focus of attention on the intended movement outcomes allows the learner to adopt a more automatic form of control and is therefore more effective [1012]. For example, Wulf, McNevin, and Shea (2001) found that performance and learning of a balance task was improved when an external attentional focus was adopted [11]. In their study, the internal focus group was instructed to focus on controlling their feet, while the external focus group was told to focus on controlling the movement of the balance platform they stood on. They found that those in the external focus groups not only performed better during training, they also exhibited better retention of the learned skill. Prosthetic researchers have advocated that the theory of attentional focus may be utilized to improve the effectiveness of prosthetic skill training by instructing the LLP users to focus externally (towards the movement task goals) rather than internally (toward body or prosthetic movements) [6]. The application of this theory, however, remains unexplored within the context of prosthetic rehabilitation. In fact, it may be more common for prosthetists to use internally-focused instruction and feedback during gait training, as has been demonstrated in physical therapists treating patients with stroke and in other practice fields [1316].

While the practice of prosthetics typically centers around designing, fabricating, and fitting prostheses, it often also includes training the users on how to properly use their prosthetic device for motor activities such as walking. The training process can be challenging to adult patients with limb loss because it involves controlling the residual limb-prosthesis interface with altered sensory input and motor output [17]. Such challenge often leads to slower learning as well as increased risk of falls and other injuries during the initial phases of learning to use a LLP. Furthermore, a LLP user may have to relearn and re-adapt when different prosthetic components are introduced (e.g. socket design, prosthetic joints, etc.), or when physical changes occur after surgical revision and atrophy of residual limb muscles over time [18, 19]. Because of this emphasis on skill learning, practice, and motor adaptation, it is generally agreed that motor learning strategies including the adoption of an external focus of attention when delivering instructions or feedback to LLP users could benefit prosthetic training, device acceptance, and improve rehabilitation outcomes after amputation [7, 20]. It may be beneficial for prosthetists and other post-amputation care providers to recognize that the words used in their instructions pertaining to how to operate a prosthetic device can have a tangible impact on the patient’s learning, as has been demonstrated in other disciplines of rehabilitation [2124].

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the verbal interaction between prosthetists and LLP users during prosthetic training. Specifically, we sought to compare the frequency and direction of attentional focus (internal, external, and mixed focus) embedded in the verbal instructions and feedback interactions between prosthetists and LLP users in clinical practice settings. We hypothesized that during prosthetic training, the majority of the verbal instructions would direct LLP users’ attention internally. A secondary purpose of the study was to determine whether the direction of attentional focus embedded in instructions received by the LLP users was related to the treating prosthetists’ experience and/or LLP users’ characteristics such as age, sex, time since amputation, and physical function.

Methods

Participants

Inclusion criteria for LLP user participants included lower limb amputation involving at least one major joint (i.e. ankle, ankle & knee, or above), 18 years of age or older, and current or planned use of a prosthesis. LLP users were recruited from local prosthetic clinics in the Southern Nevada region. Prosthetists had to be certified, actively practicing prosthetists. These criteria were selected to obtain a convenient sample of typical adult LLP users and practicing prosthetists. Neither the LLP users nor the prosthetists were informed about the purpose of the study to eliminate the risk of possibly affecting the interactions. The sample size of 20 LLP users-prosthetist couples was determined based on a previous study examining similar clinical practice behavior [13].

Procedure

After a LLP user was recruited, a researcher asked for his/her permission as well as permission from the treating prosthetist to observe and record the session that involved gait and mobility training with their prostheses. Pre-prosthetic sessions (i.e. sessions before a working prosthesis was available to the LLP user) were excluded from this study. The researcher then explained to the LLP user and the prosthetist that they would be recorded during the training session without any interference or comments. An informed consent form approved by the University of Nevada, Las Vegas Institutional Review Board (IRB) for Biomedical Research was given to each participant to be read and signed prior to data collection.

A smart phone with an audio/video recorder was used for data collection. An external shotgun microphone was connected to the phone to improve audio quality, particularly to help clearly record the verbal exchanges between the prosthetist and LLP user. If the entire session was in one room, the recorder was fixed to a tripod and placed in a corner of the room to minimize intrusions during the prosthetic training. If the prosthetist and LLP user changed locations, the researcher held the recorder and walked behind to minimize disruption.

After obtaining consent, a data collection sheet was used to collect each LLP user’s demographics and medical history, and the treating prosthetist’s years of experience. The LLP user questionnaire was used to record ethnicity, gender, age, date, cause, and level of amputation, use of assistive devices, and MFCL-level (provided by the treating prosthetist). Perceived mobility of LLP users was assessed using the Prosthetic Limb Users Survey of Mobility (PLUS-M) 12-item Short Form [25].

Data analysis

The recorded videos were reviewed and transcribed by one of the two data analysts with the assistance of online transcription services (Rev.com or Otter.ai.). A data analyst cross-checked then analyzed each transcript to identify the frequency of internally focused, externally focused, mixed focus, and unfocused statements embedded in the verbal interactions between the prosthetist and LLP user during each session. These themes were established based on previous research of attentional focus in clinical rehabilitation practice [14]. The thematic analysis procedure was based on the technique described by Pope et al. (2000) which suggested five stages to qualitatively analyze health care interview data (familiarization, identifying a thematic framework, indexing, charting, and interpretation) [26]. In this study, familiarization involved re-watching the recordings and taking note of recurring themes. Identification of a thematic framework was done by identifying key concepts that could be observed in each LLP user-prosthetist pairing. Subsequently, indexing included examining the concept of interest embedded in the recordings (i.e. direction of attentional focus). Summaries of the findings were then arranged in a chart to interpret themes from the data.

Statements were identified as having an internal focus if they directed a LLP user’s attention to one or more body parts, such as their foot, leg, and/or knee. A distinction was made between internally focused comments that were directed towards a LLP user’s body, residual limb, intact limb, and/or those directed towards their prosthesis or prosthetic components. Statements were identified as having an external focus if they directed a LLP user’s attention towards the desired effects of the movement, such as instructing them to walk towards a target or push off against the ground. Statements that included more than one type of focus (mixed focus) and without a specific focus (unfocused) were categorized separately (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Definitions and examples of the attentional focus themes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262977.t001

An analysis matrix was created to outline the different types of tasks, instructions, and feedback given during each recorded training session (see S1 Appendix for an example of an annotated matrix). The matrix was used to quantify the frequency of usage of the five attentional focus types. Because the recordings were of different lengths and contained different numbers of statements, we computed the percentages of the four types of attentional focus statements relative to the total number of statements in each recording. A similar methodology has been used in previous research studies and was shown to be reliable [14, 27]. A reliability study was conducted based on the first five videos collected. The inter-rater reliability of determining the statement types was examined using 2-way random intraclass correlation coefficient models (ICC2,1) for absolute agreement based on independent analysis results from the two analysts [28, 29]. The analysts demonstrated excellent inter-rater reliability for classifying the attentional focus themes of interest (ICC = 0.939 and 0.996 for external and internal categories, respectively).

To compare the percentages of the four types of attentional focus statements delivered by the participating prosthetists to the LLP users, the mean percentage value and 95% confidence interval (95% CI) were computed for each focus type. Normality and homogeneity of variance assumptions were assessed using the Shapiro-Wilks test and White test, respectively. Due to the violation of normality assumption, the nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to determine whether there were significant differences within the six participating prosthetists regarding the percentages of attentional focus statement types delivered. This analysis was conducted to examine if the attentional focus usage pattern can be generalized among the participating clinicians with different clinical training and experiential background.

Multi-variate regression analysis using the Tobit model was conducted to determine whether the prosthetists’ experience and/or LLP users’ characteristics (sex, age, years since amputation, cause of amputation, amputation level, and mobility measured by PLUS-M) were associated with greater use of internal focus instructions and feedback during prosthetic training. The Tobit model was chosen because the dependent variable (i.e. proportion of internal focus statements) was a percentage value bounded by an upper limit of 100% [30]. The estimated coefficients in the Tobit regression model can be interpreted similarly to those in a linear regression model, albeit the association is not on the observed proportions but the uncensored latent variable values [31]. The variance inflation factor was calculated for each predictor to examine multicollinearity of the model. All data analyses were performed using SAS v9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina, USA). The significance level was set to 0.05.

Results

Demographics of LLP users and prosthetists

Six prosthetists (2 female, 4 male) from 3 different prosthetic clinics in the Southern Nevada region participated and recruited their LLP user patients for this study. The participating prosthetists’ years of clinical experience ranged from 4 to 21 years (mean = 10.0 years, SD = 6.2). Five of the prosthetists were certified by the American Board for Certification (ABC) and one by the Board of Certification (BOC), and four received master’s level training in prosthetics and orthotics.

A total of 21 prosthetic training sessions from 21 different LLP users were recorded and analyzed. One participant’s video had no sound (#16) so it was excluded from the analysis. The remaining 20 participants consisted of 12 males and 8 females (mean age = 50.2 years, SD = 11.6). Time since amputation ranged from 0.4 years to 27.3 years. PLUS-M T-scores ranged from 21.8 to 71.4 (mean = 47.7, SD = 12.6) indicating a wide range of perceived mobility among the LLP user participants. Causes of amputation varied but the most prevalent was diabetes (n = 7). Table 2 summarizes the characteristics of the LLP users and prosthetists.

thumbnail
Table 2. Characteristics of the LLP users and treating prosthetists.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262977.t002

Direction of attentional focus embedded in prosthetic training instructions and feedback

Length of the training sessions ranged from 6 to 32 minutes for a total of 338 minutes over 20 recorded sessions. Fig 1 shows the relative frequency of attentional focus types in each training session. Nine hundred and four individual statements were transcribed and classified. On average, one verbal instruction/feedback was delivered every 22.1 seconds. Of all statements collected, 48% (436/904) were classified as internal focus of attention directed at the prosthesis or prosthetic component, and 15% (138/904) were classified as internal focus of attention directed at the LLP users’ body, residual limb, and intact limb. Overall, internally focused statements accounted for 64% of all verbal instructions and feedback (95% CI = 60–67%, range = 44–88%). Furthermore, 9% (77/904, 95% CI = 7–10%) of the statements were classified as external focus of attention, 3% (30/904, 95% CI = 2–4%) were classified as mixed focus of attention, and 25% (223/904, 95% CI = 22–27%) were classified as unfocused. Given that the 95% CI of the internal focus proportion was significantly higher and did not overlap with that of any other focus types, we concluded that the participating prosthetists predominantly used instruction/feedback that invoked an internal focus of attention, particularly for directing LLP users’ attention to their prosthetic devices. There were no statistical differences within the six prosthetists regarding the types of attentional focus statements used (p = 0.330–0.945).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Distribution of external and internal focus instruction and feedback during prosthetic training (dark gray = % external focus, medium gray = % internal focus on intact body, and light gray = % internal focus on prosthesis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262977.g001

We diagnosed the normality (p-value = 0.2655) and variance homogeneity assumptions (p-value = 0.6838), showing that neither one was violated in the regression analysis. The regression model was also free from the multi-collinearity problem, where all variance inflation factors were less than 10 (range = 1.34 to 3.77) in the seven included predictors. The multi-variate regression analysis showed that LLP users’ sex, age, and PLUS-M T-scores were significantly associated with the percentage of internal focus instructions and feedback they received (p = 0.006, 0.04, and 0.02, respectively; Table 3). Specifically, female LLP users were more likely to receive internal focus instructions than males (p = 0.006). LLPs with higher PLUS-M T-scores also tend to receive a larger proportion of internally-focused instructions and feedback (a one-point increase in PLUS-M T-score coincided with 0.7% increase in the internal focus instructions received [p = 0.02; Fig 2]). Prosthetists’ years of experience were not significantly associated with the proportion of internally-focused language they used (p = 0.42). Years since amputation, cause of amputation, and amputation level were also not significantly associated with the percentage of internal focus statements received during prosthetic training (p = 0.47, 0.08, and 0.31, respectively; Table 3).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Relationship of the percentages of internal focus statements versus PLUS-M T-scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262977.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of the multivariate analysis of the associations between participant characteristics and percentage of internal focus statements received during prosthetic training.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262977.t003

Discussion

Despite previous research suggesting that incorporating motor learning principles could improve prosthetic training and post-amputation outcomes [6], this is the first study to examine the direction of attentional focus embedded in prosthetic training instructions during clinical practice. Our results demonstrated that the use of instructions and feedback was frequent and ubiquitous during prosthetic training. Our hypothesis was confirmed that most of the verbal interactions delivered by prosthetists to LLP users were focused internally on the movements of the patients’ body and/or prosthesis, rather than externally on the intended movement effects.

The first aim of our study was to examine the frequency and direction of attentional focus embedded in the verbal interactions between prosthetists and patients during daily practice. Empirical evidence has accumulated during the last two decades regarding the benefits of adopting an external focus of attention on motor performance and learning as compared to an internal focus [9]. Specifically, previous studies have consistently demonstrated that adopting an external focus before or during the execution of a motor task leads to faster learning, and improved movement effectiveness and neuromuscular efficiency [3234]. This motor learning principle has profoundly impacted the practice of rehabilitation after neurological injuries such as stroke [5, 13, 14]. In the context of post-amputation rehabilitation, the translation of how external versus internal attentional focus affects learning is perhaps even more pertinent because LLP users need to learn to master the use an external device (i.e. a prosthesis) in place of the lost biological limb.

Our results showed a significant but perhaps unsurprising trend that most (i.e. 64%) of verbal instructions and feedback during prosthetic training were directed internally to the movements of prostheses-users’ body and prosthesis. This is comparable to what has been observed in physical therapy for rehabilitation of patients with neurological conditions [13, 14]. For example, Johnson et al. observed eight physical therapy sessions of gait training for patients with stroke and found that 67% of the instructions were internally focused [14]. Previous studies have shown that internally focused instructions can lead to less effective motor learning and performance even when compared to neutral instructions [35, 36]. A possible mechanism underlying motor performance degradation associated with internal focus was the “self-invoking trigger hypothesis” which proposed that frequent evaluation of one’s own movements associated with a task (i.e. self-schema) negatively impacts task learning and performance [35]. An external focus that removes the emphasis of controlling the complex coordination of body movements may activate the more natural “automatic” processes of goal-action coupling and promote task automaticity, which is paramount when learning to walk with a prosthesis [11]. As observed in this study of contemporary clinical practice, prosthetists often use internally-focused language to evaluate a LLP user’s movement or to draw attention to specific errors. Such practice, while well-intended, may hinder LLP users’ performance and learning.

In speculation that LLP users with certain characteristics may be biased to receive a greater proportion of the presumably less effective internally focused instruction and feedback, our secondary analysis examined these associations. We found that LLP users’ sex, age, and perceived mobility were significantly predictive of the percentage of internally-focused instructions and feedback they received. Specifically, women and older LLP users were more likely to receive a higher percentage of internal focus instructions. To the best of our knowledge, no other observational studies to date have reported similar sex and age biases regarding the use of attentional focus in clinical practice. Nevertheless, these biases may potentially impact the training and outcomes of patients, and should be examined further in the future.

Our findings also showed that LLP users with higher PLUS-M T-scores tend to receive a greater proportion of internal focus instructions. It is possible that when treating higher functioning LLP users, prosthetists may choose to focus on addressing subtle body or prosthetic component movements such as symmetry and other specific motor patterns. For example, many prosthetic knee components were designed with the expectation that the user would adopt a pronounced heel-to-toe weight-shifting pattern during the stance phase of gait [3739], particularly during faster walking to allow rapid knee flexion and greater ground clearance [40, 41]. Internally-focused instructions and feedback may be easier and more intuitive to use when teaching these intricate skills to LLP users, leading to its more prevailing use in higher functioning patients. In a previous study by Kal et al. investigating the association between therapists’ attentional focus use in patients with stroke, they found that patients with a longer length of hospital stay tend to receive more external focus instruction and feedback [13]. The authors suggested that the increase in the use of external focus may be a natural progression over the course of rehabilitation to fit the needs, goals, and functioning level of the patients.

Attaining high proficiency in using a prosthesis is important for recovering function after amputation. Prosthetic skill learning may be of even greater importance today owing to the proliferation of sophisticated prosthetic components that are designed and optimized to promote specific and complex movement patterns [42, 43]. While these contemporary prosthetic components facilitate higher levels of functioning, they also demand greater skill and may require additional and more elaborate learning and practice [44]. Compounding the complexity of the learning problem is the varying sensorimotor comorbidities (i.e. reduced proprioception) associated with dysvascular causes of lower limb amputations, which are the current leading reason for acquired amputations. The diminished joint and muscle proprioception may render the traditional modes of feedback about movement quality ineffective (i.e. internal focus instructions emphasizing motion of body segments). For these reasons, we believe that principles of motor learning, in this case the adoption of appropriate external attentional focus associated with prosthetic skill learning, is one of the promising approaches to facilitate rehabilitation of function after amputation.

In light of our findings and the motor learning knowledge regarding the potential benefits of external focus, we propose that prosthetic practitioners (i.e. prosthetists, physical therapists, occupational therapists, and physiatrists) should be mindful about their choice of words when training LLP users on how to perform movement tasks with their prosthesis. As an example, we observed many cases where the prosthetist instructed LLP users to bend or straighten their knees and to kick the feet out and contact the ground with heels during walking. We understand that it is difficult to instruct movements completely without referring to the patient’s body or prosthesis, however improved outcomes may be attained if the practitioner can use instructions such as “walk like you would crush a bug with the heel of your shoes with every step” (to promote a heel first gait pattern) or using external targets such as markers placed on the floor to promote larger steps and faster gait [45]. It may be beneficial for prosthetic component manufacturers to develop evidence-guided instructional videos aimed at addressing common movement errors and promoting movement patterns that maximize the performance of the prosthesis and the patient [6]. Further research is needed to identify specific movement goals that are most prevalent in prosthetic training, which can guide the development of motor learning guidelines for improving patient outcomes during prosthetic rehabilitation.

Limitations

Although 338 minutes of video data were collected and analyzed systematically, a limitation of the present study was that only 20 LLP users and 6 prosthetists from 3 clinics in Southern Nevada region were included. Therefore, the results may be influenced by regional practice trends. Larger scale studies involving a wider range of practice regions and countries are needed to improve generalizability. A second limitation was the presence of a researcher and an audio and video recorder in the room during each session. Even though the prosthetists and LLP user participants were unaware of the purpose of the study, the presence of an observer in the room could have potentially impacted their choice of words and actions. Other factors that can affect instruction and feedback, such as LLP user’s level of education, prosthesis use proficiency, and other psychological factors, were not examined in the current study.

Clinical relevance

While experimental research into the benefits of an external focus of attention during prosthetic skill training is pending, evidence from other clinical models have shown that adopting an external focus can enhance motor performance and benefit long-term learning. Clinicians should adopt the best available scientific evidence of motor learning when treating individuals with lower limb amputation. Overreliance on internally focused instructions may interrupt goal-action coupling and hinder prosthetic skill learning in individuals with limb loss.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Example of a completed analysis matrix: Focus of attention.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262977.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participating LLP users and prosthetists from Prosthetic Center of Excellence, POP Prosthetics, Nevada Orthotics and Prosthetics for accommodating the data collection of this study, particularly during the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020.

References

  1. 1. Ziegler-Graham K, MacKenzie EJ, Ephraim PL, Travison TG, Brookmeyer R. Estimating the prevalence of limb loss in the United States: 2005 to 2050. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2008;89(3):422–9. pmid:18295618
  2. 2. Sheehan TP, Gondo GC. Impact of limb loss in the United States. Physical medicine and rehabilitation clinics of North America. 2014;25(1):9–28. pmid:24287236
  3. 3. Wurdeman SR, Stevens PM, Campbell JH. Mobility Analysis of AmpuTees II: Comorbidities and Mobility in Lower Limb Prosthesis Users. American journal of physical medicine & rehabilitation / Association of Academic Physiatrists. 2018;97(11):782–8. pmid:29781963
  4. 4. Webster JB, Crunkhorn A, Sall J, Highsmith MJ, Pruziner A, Randolph BJ. Clinical Practice Guidelines for the Rehabilitation of Lower Limb Amputation An Update from the Department of Veterans Affairs and Department of Defense. Am J Phys Med Rehab. 2019;98(9):820–9. pmid:31419214
  5. 5. Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23(5):1382–414. pmid:26833314
  6. 6. Sawers A, Hahn ME, Kelly VE, Czerniecki JM, Kartin D. Beyond componentry: How principles of motor learning can enhance locomotor rehabilitation of individuals with lower limb loss—a review. Journal of rehabilitation research and development. 2012;49(10):1431–42. pmid:23516048
  7. 7. Parr JVV, Wright DJ, Uiga L, Marshall B, Mohamed MO, Wood G. A scoping review of the application of motor learning principles to optimize myoelectric prosthetic hand control. Prosthetics and orthotics international. 2021. pmid:34932512
  8. 8. Gailey R, Gaunaurd I, Raya M, Kirk-Sanchez N, Prieto-Sanchez LM, Roach K. Effectiveness of an Evidence-Based Amputee Rehabilitation Program: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial. Physical therapy. 2020;100(5):773–87. pmid:31951260
  9. 9. Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. Int Rev Sport Exer P. 2013;6(1):77–104.
  10. 10. Richer N, Saunders D, Polskaia N, Lajoie Y. The effects of attentional focus and cognitive tasks on postural sway may be the result of automaticity. Gait Posture. 2017;54:45–9. pmid:28259038
  11. 11. Wulf G, McNevin N, Shea CH. The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology A, Human experimental psychology. 2001;54(4):1143–54. pmid:11765737
  12. 12. Poolton JM, Maxwell JP, Masters RS, Raab M. Benefits of an external focus of attention: common coding or conscious processing? Journal of sports sciences. 2006;24(1):89–99. pmid:16368617
  13. 13. Kal E, van den Brink H, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Goossens PH, van Bennekom C, et al. How physical therapists instruct patients with stroke: an observational study on attentional focus during gait rehabilitation after stroke. Disabil Rehabil. 2018;40(10):1154–65. pmid:28637152
  14. 14. Johnson L, Burridge JH, Demain SH. Internal and external focus of attention during gait re-education: an observational study of physical therapist practice in stroke rehabilitation. Physical therapy. 2013;93(7):957–66. pmid:23559523
  15. 15. Yamada M, Higgins LQ, Raisbeck L. How external and internal focus are used in the field: A review. Int J Sports Sci Coa. 2022;17(3):647–54.
  16. 16. Durham K, Van Vliet PM, Badger F, Sackley C. Use of information feedback and attentional focus of feedback in treating the person with a hemiplegic arm. Physiotherapy research international: the journal for researchers and clinicians in physical therapy. 2009;14(2):77–90. pmid:19107706
  17. 17. Wurdeman SR, Myers SA, Jacobsen AL, Stergiou N. Adaptation and prosthesis effects on stride-to-stride fluctuations in amputee gait. PloS one. 2014;9(6):e100125. pmid:24956384
  18. 18. Lee Childers W, Prilutsky BI, Gregor RJ. Motor adaptation to prosthetic cycling in people with trans-tibial amputation. Journal of biomechanics. 2014;47(10):2306–13. pmid:24818794
  19. 19. Tantua AT, Geertzen JH, van den Dungen JJ, Breek JK, Dijkstra PU. Reduction of residual limb volume in people with transtibial amputation. Journal of rehabilitation research and development. 2014;51(7):1119–26. pmid:25437771
  20. 20. Parr JVV, Vine SJ, Wilson MR, Harrison NR, Wood G. Visual attention, EEG alpha power and T7-Fz connectivity are implicated in prosthetic hand control and can be optimized through gaze training. Journal of neuroengineering and rehabilitation. 2019;16(1):52. pmid:31029174
  21. 21. Kim GJ, Hinojosa J, Rao AK, Batavia M, O’Dell MW. Randomized Trial on the Effects of Attentional Focus on Motor Training of the Upper Extremity Using Robotics With Individuals After Chronic Stroke. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2017;98(10):1924–31. pmid:28652064
  22. 22. Landers MR, Hatlevig RM, Davis AD, Richards AR, Rosenlof LE. Does attentional focus during balance training in people with Parkinson’s disease affect outcome? A randomised controlled clinical trial. Clinical rehabilitation. 2016;30(1):53–63. pmid:25697454
  23. 23. Durham KF, Sackley CM, Wright CC, Wing AM, Edwards MG, van Vliet P. Attentional focus of feedback for improving performance of reach-to-grasp after stroke: a randomised crossover study. Physiotherapy. 2014;100(2):108–15. pmid:23796803
  24. 24. Chua LK, Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Choose your words wisely: Optimizing impacts on standardized performance testing. Gait Posture. 2020;79:210–6. pmid:32442896
  25. 25. Hafner BJ, Gaunaurd IA, Morgan SJ, Amtmann D, Salem R, Gailey RS. Construct Validity of the Prosthetic Limb Users Survey of Mobility (PLUS-M) in Adults With Lower Limb Amputation. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2017;98(2):277–85. pmid:27590443
  26. 26. Pope C, Ziebland S, Mays N. Qualitative research in health care. Analysing qualitative data. Bmj. 2000;320(7227):114–6. pmid:10625273
  27. 27. Haidet KK, Tate J, Divirgilio-Thomas D, Kolanowski A, Happ MB. Methods to improve reliability of video-recorded behavioral data. Res Nurs Health. 2009;32(4):465–74. pmid:19434651
  28. 28. Erratum to "A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research" [J Chiropr Med 2016;15(2):155–163]. J Chiropr Med. 2017;16(4):346. pmid:29276468
  29. 29. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15(2):155–63. pmid:27330520
  30. 30. Long JS. Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1997.
  31. 31. Mcdonald JF, Moffitt RA. The Uses of Tobit Analysis. Rev Econ Stat. 1980;62(2):318–21.
  32. 32. Wulf G, Dufek JS, Lozano L, Pettigrew C. Increased jump height and reduced EMG activity with an external focus. Human movement science. 2010;29(3):440–8. pmid:20409600
  33. 33. Wulf G, Dufek JS. Increased jump height with an external focus due to enhanced lower extremity joint kinetics. Journal of motor behavior. 2009;41(5):401–9. pmid:19846388
  34. 34. Chiviacowsky S, Wulf G, Wally R. An external focus of attention enhances balance learning in older adults. Gait Posture. 2010;32(4):572–5. pmid:20850325
  35. 35. McKay B, Wulf G, Lewthwaite R, Nordin A. The self: Your own worst enemy? A test of the self-invoking trigger hypothesis. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2015;68(9):1910–9. pmid:25565349
  36. 36. Porter JM, Nolan RP, Ostrowski EJ, Wulf G. Directing attention externally enhances agility performance: a qualitative and quantitative analysis of the efficacy of using verbal instructions to focus attention. Front Psychol. 2010;1:216. pmid:21833271
  37. 37. Eberly VJ, Mulroy SJ, Gronley JK, Perry J, Yule WJ, Burnfield JM. Impact of a stance phase microprocessor-controlled knee prosthesis on level walking in lower functioning individuals with a transfemoral amputation. Prosthetics and orthotics international. 2014;38(6):447–55. pmid:24135259
  38. 38. Kaufman KR, Frittoli S, Frigo CA. Gait asymmetry of transfemoral amputees using mechanical and microprocessor-controlled prosthetic knees. Clinical biomechanics. 2012;27(5):460–5. pmid:22221344
  39. 39. Maaref K, Martinet N, Grumillier C, Ghannouchi S, Andre JM, Paysant J. Kinematics in the terminal swing phase of unilateral transfemoral amputees: microprocessor-controlled versus swing-phase control prosthetic knees. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2010;91(6):919–25. pmid:20510984
  40. 40. Mohamed A, Sexton A, Simonsen K, McGibbon CA. Development of a Mechanistic Hypothesis Linking Compensatory Biomechanics and Stepping Asymmetry during Gait of Transfemoral Amputees. Appl Bionics Biomech. 2019;2019:4769242. pmid:30863460
  41. 41. Shojaei I, Hendershot BD, Wolf EJ, Bazrgari B. Persons with unilateral transfemoral amputation experience larger spinal loads during level-ground walking compared to able-bodied individuals. Clinical biomechanics. 2016;32:157–63. pmid:26682630
  42. 42. Franzke AW, Kristoffersen MB, Bongers RM, Murgia A, Pobatschnig B, Unglaube F, et al. Users’ and therapists’ perceptions of myoelectric multi-function upper limb prostheses with conventional and pattern recognition control. PloS one. 2019;14(8):e0220899. pmid:31465469
  43. 43. Kristoffersen MB, Franzke AW, Bongers RM, Wand M, Murgia A, van der Sluis CK. User training for machine learning controlled upper limb prostheses: a serious game approach. Journal of neuroengineering and rehabilitation. 2021;18(1):32. pmid:33579326
  44. 44. Alcaide-Aguirre RE, Morgenroth DC, Ferris DP. Motor control and learning with lower-limb myoelectric control in amputees. Journal of rehabilitation research and development. 2013;50(5):687–98. pmid:24013916
  45. 45. Jie LJ, Goodwin V, Kleynen M, Braun S, Nunns M, Wilson M. Analogy learning in Parkinson’s disease: A proof-of-concept study. Int J Ther Rehabil. 2016;23(3):123–30.