Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Towards an affect intensity regulation hypothesis: Systematic review and meta-analyses of the relationship between affective states and alcohol consumption

  • Anna Tovmasyan ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    tovmasan@edgehill.ac.uk

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, United Kingdom, Liverpool Centre for Alcohol Research, Liverpool, United Kingdom

  • Rebecca L. Monk,

    Roles Data curation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, United Kingdom, Liverpool Centre for Alcohol Research, Liverpool, United Kingdom

  • Derek Heim

    Roles Data curation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, United Kingdom, Liverpool Centre for Alcohol Research, Liverpool, United Kingdom

Abstract

While self-medication and positive and negative reinforcement models of alcohol use suggest that there is an association between daily affect and alcohol consumption, findings within the academic literature have been inconsistent. This pre-registered systematic review meta-analytically interrogated the results from studies amongst non-clinical populations that examine the relationship between daily affective states and alcohol consumption volume. PRISMA guided searches of PsychINFO, PsycARTICLES, Science Direct, PubMed, SCOPUS, and JSTOR databases were conducted. When both laboratory and field studies were included, meta-analyses with robust variance estimation yielded 53 eligible studies on negative affect (8355 participants, 127 effect sizes) and 35 studies for positive affect (6384 participants, 50 effect sizes). The significant pooled associations between intra-day affect and alcohol consumption were r = .09, [.03, .14] for negative affect, and r = .17, [.04, .30] for positive affect. A small-to-medium sized effect (d = .275, [.11, .44]) of negative affect on daily alcohol consumption volume was found in laboratory studies (14 studies, 1100 participants). While publication bias was suspected, P-curve analyses suggested that the results were unlikely to be the product of publication bias and p-hacking alone, and selection model analysis revealed no significant differences in results when publication bias was accounted for. For negative affect, using number of drinks as the measure of alcohol consumption was associated with lower effect sizes. For positive affect, the results demonstrated a decline of this observed effect over time. Overall, findings point towards the possibility of developing an affect intensity regulation theory of alcohol use. Conceptualizing the mood-alcohol nexus in terms of affect intensity regulation may afford a more parsimonious explanation of alcohol consumption rather than viewing the behavior as being shaped by either positive or negative affective states.

Introduction

In many Western societies the link between alcohol and mood is deeply rooted, and this cultural knowledge is aptly illustrated by Bukowski’s [1, p. 373] assertion that “If something bad happens you drink in an attempt to forget; if something good happens you drink in order to celebrate; and if nothing happens you drink to make something happen.” Not content with leaving the relationship between mood and alcohol consumption to the writers and philosophers, scientists have, for many years, sought to investigate this association. As a result, a large body of research and theory that seeks to shed light on the extent to which people drink because of their mood has accrued. However, research findings have been mixed and attempts to synthesise this literature meta-analytically have been limited to a study examining negative affect in laboratory settings [2]. This found a small-to-moderate effect of negative mood inductions on alcohol consumption in research that was conducted in contexts that differ markedly from those in which people typically drink. Therefore, the current systematic review and meta-analyses aimed to extend this formative work by focussing on positive as well as negative affect and examining methodically empirical findings garnered from studies carried out in both laboratory and in real world settings.

Both the self-medication hypothesis [3] and the negative reinforcement model of alcohol use [4] theoretically posit a direct association between diminished mental wellbeing and hazardous drinking [5], whereby alcohol is used as a means of improving low mood. While these theoretical models were initially developed to help understand heavy consumption in clinical populations [6,7], they have also been applied to explain excessive drug and alcohol use in non-clinical settings with a view of preventing the development of substance use disorders (e.g., [810]). In a similar vein, tension-reduction [11] and stress-response dampening theories [12] construe the experience of negative affect as a risk factor for problematic consumption. The decision to drink (or to exercise restraint), according to these theories, is a product of people’s affective experiences, and alcohol consumption is understood temporally [1214] as an outcome of their preceding (negative) mood.

Empirical findings examining the mood-alcohol nexus have, however, been varied. While some laboratory studies indicate that negative affect is associated with increased alcohol-related attentional bias [15,16], others suggest that it is not related to alcohol consumption [1719]. Similarly, some real-time studies find no association between negative affect and unplanned heavy drinking [20], while others suggest that negative affect is inversely related to drinking onset and further intoxication [2125]. The existing literature therefore provides mixed support for negative affect regulation theories of alcohol use.

In a similar way, there are inconsistent findings with regards to the positive reinforcement theory of alcohol use [26], according to which people drink alcohol to enhance the positive emotions they are experiencing. As such, while real-time studies indicate that positive mood is associated with increased drinking likelihood and breath alcohol concentration later that day [21,2325,27], a study using questionnaire design found that having difficulties with regulating positive emotions was linked with drug but not alcohol misuse [28]. Studies conducted in the laboratory also produced mixed findings: while Stein et al. found that positive mood induction increases consumption [29], VandeVeen et al. found no such effect [30]. Given the variation in support for both negative and positive reinforcement theoretical models, formal interrogation of discrepant findings derived from both laboratory and field settings is required to shed light on the somewhat elusive association between affective states and alcohol consumption. It is therefore necessary to meta-analytically examine whether inconsistent findings may be due to power limitations of individual studies.

In addition to overcoming power concerns by combining effect sizes of studies, theoretical and methodological differences between investigations need to be examined systematically to help clarify inconsistencies in this body of work. The first reason for mixed findings may relate to how affect is conceptualised and measured. Theoretically, there are two perspectives on affective state: while some research operationalises affect as a singular concept and measures this on a continuum that is anchored between positive and negative affect (e.g., [31,32]), in other work current mood is treated as a unidimensional construct in which different affective states are unable to overlap simultaneously (e.g., [33,34]). This has methodological implications as the former conceptualisation of affect necessitates the use of measures that assess mood on a continuous scale, with scope for variability in valence and arousal (e.g., mood circumplex, [35]; UMACL, [36]; affect grid, [37]), while the latter perspective typically uses separate assessments of intensity of negative and positive affect (e.g., PANAS, [38]; VAMS, [39]). These theoretical and methodological considerations are important as they also have consequences for how study findings are interpreted. If affect is understood and measured as a continuous construct, evidence suggesting that positive affect increases consumption (in line with the positive reinforcement model) would contradict the negative reinforcement model. On the other hand, if positive and negative affect are assessed as discrete entities, then evidence for one theory would not necessarily contradict the other and it may be beneficial to combine these approaches into a more parsimonious model. When taking stock of this literature it is therefore essential to consider how measurement choices and theoretical conceptualisations of affect may impact results.

A second potential methodological reason for divergent findings in this research area centres on whether the studies examine distinct emotions or look at overall levels of affect. An important debate in the literature concerns whether emotions ought to be conceptualised along dimensions of valence and arousal [40,41] or as discrete entities [42,43]. In the alcohol literature, the first approach, where researchers measure mood scores (e.g., PANAS, [38]) as an average of various adjectives related to either negative or positive emotions (e.g., [27,4446]), has been more commonly utilised. Yet, an alternative approach, which adopts a discrete model of emotions, analyses each affect item individually. O’Donnell et al., for example, examined how stress and irritation impact consumption [47], while Dvorak and Simons [9] as well as Shadur et al. [33] looked at how anxiety and sadness affect drinking likelihood, and Rohsenow et al. [48] examined how anxiety, anger, and depression influence the number of alcohol units consumed. Considering that emotions differ in terms of arousal [35,49,50] and physiology [43], it is possible that the widely adopted methodology of averaging affective states may have inadvertently contributed to a homogenisation of different facets of emotions. It therefore needs to be examined whether the process of collapsing distinct emotions into positive and negative affect scores may have led researchers to miss the nuanced ways in which these emotions shape alcohol consumption differentially, or whether combining them into a single score for negative and positive affect is appropriate.

The third methodological variation between studies that may systematically impact results relates to their design. On the one hand, daily diary and ecological momentary assessment (EMA)/experience sampling methods (ESM) studies have enabled researchers to minimise retrospection bias [51,52] and to examine the behaviour in question in naturalistic settings. Here, participants are instructed to record in structured ways events/feelings that occurred during the day. However, such studies occur in uncontrolled environments, and it is therefore possible that findings may be impacted by extraneous factors that are not captured by the research methods used in these studies. Laboratory studies, on the other hand, typically utilise ad-libitum drinking paradigms, where participants can consume as much or as little alcohol as they wish. While having a controlled environment is advantageous, participants may feel obliged to drink alcohol [53,54] or may not be offered their typical beverage of choice (alcoholic or non-alcoholic, [55,56]) and therefore might not accurately reflect real-world drinking behaviours. The potential for study design to moderate the association between affect and alcohol consumption therefore needs to be considered meta-analytically.

Accordingly, the present pre-registered systematic review and meta-analyses aimed to synthesise findings on the impact of affective states on alcohol consumption in non-clinical populations by addressing these gaps, while accounting for potential sources of variability. Specifically, following the self-medication hypothesis and tension-reduction theory, which postulate that increases in negative affect predict substance use within a short timeframe [13,14], we examined the impact of affective states on same day consumption. In consideration of the suggestion that negative and positive affect may be distinct experiences [57,58] separate models were used in the analyses. Laboratory studies examining negative affect were analyzed separately prior to the main analysis (which examined both laboratory and field studies) to test for any causal association. For both negative and positive affect models, alcohol measure (e.g., number of drinks or units), affect conceptualization (i.e., whether studies treated negative and positive affective states as a continuum or separate entities and whether studies looked at distinct emotions or averaged them) and study design (i.e., laboratory or field research) were examined systematically as possible methodological moderators of the affect-alcohol relationship. A series of exploratory analyses of other variables (year of publication, country, study quality) was also undertaken. The results of two meta-analyses (on negative affect and on positive affect) were then compared to establish whether negative and positive affect are differentially associated with alcohol consumption volume.

Methods

Operational definitions

Alcohol consumption is defined as ingesting any beverage containing ethanol. Mood and emotions are distinct but related constructs in that the former tend to be more stable and ‘flat’, while the latter are construed as more vivid and quick [59]. However, studies sometimes use these terms interchangeably. While it is possible that mood and emotions have different effects on alcohol consumption volume over longer periods of time, the current focus was on the effects of within a shorter timeframe, where the distinction between mood and emotions is arguably less important. Therefore, to account for differences in the terminology, the terms ‘affect’, and ‘affective state’ are used in this review as umbrella terms for the experience of mood, emotion, or feeling. We use the term ‘field studies’ for real-time studies, diary studies or studies using telephone interviews.

Eligibility criteria

The literature search was primarily conducted by the lead author. To avoid missing data, the second author conducted a comparative title search using the same criteria to ensure the incorporation of any studies which may have been overlooked in the original review. Full-text papers of any titles and abstracts that were considered relevant were obtained where possible. The relevance of each study was assessed according to the following inclusion criteria (pre-registered on Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/8bngj): peer-reviewed papers; grey literature; focus on the general human population (non-clinical sample); looking at affective states on the day of and prior to consumption; looking at consumption volume during the day (e.g., amount consumed in millilitres, numbers of drinks); papers in English or Russian. The exclusion criteria were as follows: reviews, books, posters, and editorials; literature examining clinical samples (individuals with alcohol use disorders or any other clinical disorder).

Both studies that measured affect as a continuum (i.e., where positive and negative affect are at polar ends of the same assessment spectrum), or as separate entities were included in the review. Furthermore, studies that examined mean levels of affect (i.e., average negative or positive affect) as well as affect facets (i.e., specific emotions, e.g., stress, anger, or happiness) were included. To account for varied methods of assessment, both laboratory and field studies were included. Methodological differences (treating affect as continuum or separate entities, examining mean levels of affect or distinct emotions, alcohol measure used, laboratory or field studies) were included in analysis as moderators.

Literature review

A comprehensive search was conducted of the following databases: PsychINFO, PsycARTICLES, Science Direct, PubMed, SCOPUS, JSTOR using Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA, [60]) and American Psychological Association’s Meta-Analysis Reporting Standards (MARS; [61,62]) methodologies. The following commands were used for searching: ("alcohol " OR "drinking behavi*r”) AND ("mood" OR "emotions" OR “feelings” OR "affective states") NOT "disorders". The search was conducted on 2nd March 2020. For PsychINFO, after the filters ‘empirical study’ and ‘quantitative study’ were applied, the search yielded 8285 articles for screening. For PsychARTICLES, when the same filters were applied, the search yielded three articles. For Science Direct, as wildcards “*” were not supported, the search terms were ("alcohol " OR "drinking behavior” OR “drinking behaviour”) AND ("mood" OR "emotions" OR “feelings” OR "affective states") NOT "disorders". After the filter ‘research articles’ was applied, the search yielded 2327 articles. For PubMed (3189 citations), SCOPUS (1201 citations), and JSTOR (367 citations), no filters were applied. The citations were loaded to RefWorks software, and the duplicates were removed. Bibliographies from relevant reviews and book chapters, as well as articles that fit the inclusion criteria, were manually searched for additional citations. To ensure that all relevant literature published at the time was covered, a supplementary search was conducted on 29th January 2021, which yielded 3 additional references.

To obtain grey literature, Google Scholar and Open Science Framework were searched. We also contacted the labs that conduct studies on the topics of affect and alcohol consumption. However, only one grey literature study (a study from our own lab) was included in the review, as other available studies did not fit the inclusion criteria (e.g., did not examine same day affect and alcohol consumption).

Quality assessment and data extraction

Study quality was assessed using standard criteria [63], with papers screened by two independent reviewers (Cohen’s Kappa = .71). Each paper was rated on the following criteria (each criterion assessed on a scale from 0 to 2): justification of research question, justification of study deigns, appropriate method of study selection, robustness of the measures, sample size justification, appropriateness of analytic methods, estimation of variance, control for confounds, results being reported in sufficient details, and conclusion being supported by results, with a maximum quality score of 22. Scores of 1–10 were considered to be poor quality, those that scored 11–15 were deemed to be of moderate quality, and studies with scores of 16–20 were classified as being of good quality, with manuscripts scoring 21–22 bring considered to be excellent quality. None of the studies were judged to be of poor quality (and hence none were excluded based on this), while there were 20 studies of moderate quality, 36 that were good quality, and two that were deemed to be of excellent quality.

Following the quality assessment, relevant data were extracted from each study (see Table 1 for full summary). For subset of laboratory studies on negative affect, Cohen’s d statistics was extracted (by calculating the mean difference between the two groups, and then dividing results by pooled standard deviation, or converted from F value, [64]), along with corresponding standard error. Correlation coefficients (r) were extracted either from correlation tables (N = 26 for negative affect, N = 19 for positive affect), obtained from the authors (N = 2 for negative affect, N = 2 for positive affect), or converted from available statistics (N = 24 for negative affect, N = 13 for positive affect), such as standardised beta weights (using formula provided by Peterson and Brown [65]), converted from d obtained from unstandardised beta weight and pooled standard deviation [66] or from sample size, means, and F-test [67]. When necessary, we changed the direction of correlation coefficient to ensure that each effect size reflected the relation between higher levels of affect and higher consumption volume. The standard error for each effect size was calculated using the following formula: SE(r) = √1-r2/N-2 (following [68]), while the variance was obtained by squaring the standard error.

Meta-analyses

Analytical strategy.

Prior to the main analysis, laboratory studies which provided Cohen’s d and its standard error (or other statistics from which these numbers could be calculated) were analysed separately. This was only done for studies examining the impact of negative affect on alcohol consumption (n = 14), as there were only two eligible laboratory studies on positive affect for this analysis. Random effects model was fitted in R Studio [69] version 1.4.1106 using metagen function of the meta [70] package.

Pearson’s r correlation coefficients were used as the effect size for the main meta-analyses, with generic inverse-variance pooling to combine correlations from different studies into one pooled correlation estimate. As Pearson’s r is not normally distributed, effect sizes were first converted to Fisher’s z using the following formula: (z = ½ ln ((1+r)/(1-r)). After the analysis, the coefficients were converted back to Pearson’s r (following [68]).

Some data sets provided multiple correlations between constructs of interest (e.g., the correlation between sadness and consumption and anger and consumption, or both within- and between-person associations; N = 25 for negative affect, N = 16 for positive affect). Given that including more than one effect size from a study violates the assumption of independence, we used the robust variance estimation (RVE; [71]) method to control for dependencies between effect sizes. Because correlations between the effect sizes reported within each study were not known, we assumed a Spearman’s rho (ρ) of .80 [71]. We also performed a series of sensitivity analyses by testing different values of ρ in intervals of .10. This did not affect inferences about effect sizes; therefore, these results are not reported in the paper. Correlated effects model with small-sample corrections was fitted in R Studio [69] using robumeta [72] package. Heterogeneity was assessed using I2 and τ2 statistics.

To assess potential publication bias, we conducted the Egger’s test [73], which was performed by regressing effect estimates against their standard errors. If the slope for the regression line is significant, that would suggest publication bias. Additionally, selection model analysis was performed using JASP ([74], following Bartoš et al. [75]). P-curve analysis was also performed in the online app (http://p-curve.com/) to assess potential p-hacking [76].

Several categorical moderators were examined: study quality, country, study design (laboratory vs field), alcohol consumption measure (e.g., number of millilitres consumed during the day, number of drinks consumed during the day), whether studies examined distinct emotions or averaged them, and whether study considered affective state to be a continuum or not. The effects of categorical moderators (i.e., country and study design) were assessed using meta-regression approach, as suggested by Harrer et al. [77]. Additionally, year of publication was a continuous moderator, which was examined using metatest [78] package. While examining the differences between the sample types was initially planned, it was deemed inappropriate due to inconsistent reporting—for example, many studies included anyone who was not diagnosed with alcohol use disorder, while others only included heavy drinkers. Thus, there is a potential overlap between sample types of different studies. As mean AUDIT scores were not reported consistently, we decided not to include sample type as a moderator.

The results of meta-analyses of negative and positive affect were then compared using Cohen’s q statistic [79] by imputing the obtained correlations to online calculator (https://www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html, Lenhard & Lenhard. [Unpublished]). This method transforms correlation coefficients to z scores and then subtracts them.

Data and R scripts for the analyses are available on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/fe9au/files/.

Results

Quantity of research available

Electronic and hand search identified 15372 articles, which, once duplicates were removed, left 2472 unique citations to be screened for inclusion (Fig 1). Their titles and abstracts were assessed for their relevance to the review, resulting in 22 potential articles being retained. The full texts of all but three studies were obtained. After applying exclusion criteria for the remaining full-text papers, nine articles were excluded; the most common reason for exclusion was that the studies did not look at affective state on the day and prior to consumption. After that, full texts of eligible articles were screened to obtain additional citations. This resulted in screening 264 additional articles. All but 22 were retrieved. After applying exclusion criteria for the remaining full-text papers, 206 were excluded; the most common reason for exclusion was that studies did not examine the variables of interest. Additionally, one study from our laboratory which is currently in preparation was included. Following the supplementary search, two additional articles were included. Overall, 58 studies were eligible for systematic review. Fifty-five studies were eligible for meta-analysis on negative affect, however, two did not allow for effect size extraction, leaving 53 studies. For the meta-analysis on positive affect, 35 studies were eligible and included in analysis. The PRISMA flow diagram summarises the included studies for both negative and positive affect (see Fig 1).

Included studies were published between 1975 and 2021 and were conducted in various countries: Australia (n = 2), Canada (n = 4), China (n = 1), France (n = 1), Netherlands (n = 3), the USA (n = 42), and the UK (n = 5). Most of the studies were either conducted in the laboratory and looked ad libitum consumption, or utilized experienced sampling method (EMA), or similar (diary study, telephone interviews throughout several days). Study characteristics and effect sizes are provided in Table 1.

Association between daily negative affect and volume of alcohol consumed

Analysis of laboratory studies.

Fourteen studies (1100 participants) were included in this meta-analysis. Analysis revealed a significant post-mood induction increase in amount of alcohol consumed by participants that was small-to-medium effect size, d = .28, 95% CI [.11, .44], t = 3.351, p = .004. Heterogeneity between the studies was significant, I2 = 47.7%, Q (13) = 24.87, p = .024.

Main analysis.

A total of 127 effect sizes were extracted from 53 studies (8355 participants). Correlations between negative affect and consumption ranged from -.33 to .70. The pooled correlation coefficient for our data was r = .09, 95% CI [.03, .14], t (48.5) = 3.32, p = .002. As per I2 and τ2 indexes, I2 = 70.02%, whereas τ2 = .02.

Publication bias.

According to Egger’s test, there was a publication bias (t = 3.53, p < .01). Selection model also demonstrated publication bias, χ2 (3) = 25.67, p < .001. After adjusting for publication bias, the relationship between negative affect and drinking volume was still positive and significant, r = .17, 95% CI [.07, .26], p = .004.

P-curve analysis indicated that evidential value is present, and that evidential value is not absent or inadequate (see Fig 2). This means that p-curve estimates that there is a “true” effect size underlying finding, and that the results are unlikely to be the product of publication bias and p-hacking alone. When correcting for selective reporting, the power of tests included in the meta-analysis was 69% (see Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 2. P-curve plot for studies on negative affect and alcohol consumption.

Note: The observed p-curve includes 20 statistically significant (p < .05) results, of which 15 are p < .025. There were 107 additional results entered but excluded from p-curve because they were p > .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Estimated power of meta-analysis on negative affect and drinking volume.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.g003

Meta-regression.

Several moderators were examined: year of publication, country, study design (laboratory vs field), study quality, alcohol measure, whether studies examined distinct emotions or averaged them, and whether study considered affective state to be a continuum or not. Alcohol measure was a significant moderator, as studies that looked at number of drinks as an outcome produced significantly lower effect sizes than studies that used other measures. On the other hand, analysis demonstrated that studies that looked at number of units as an outcome produced higher effect sizes, however, since the degrees of freedom were lower than four, this estimate could not be trusted. Similarly, while analysis showed that studies that treated affect as a continuum (rather than separate entities) and that were published in China and France demonstrated lower effect sizes, the degrees of freedom were lower than four, hence this estimate could not be trusted. Non-significant predictors were omitted from the final model. See Table 2 for a summary of moderator analysis results.

thumbnail
Table 2. Moderators of the relationship between negative affect and alcohol consumption volume.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.t002

Association between daily positive affect and volume of alcohol consumed

A total of 50 effect sizes were extracted from 35 studies (6384 participants). Correlations between negative affect and consumption ranged from -.19 to .96. The pooled correlation coefficient for our data was r = .17, 95% CI [.04, .30], t (34) = 2.70, p = .011. As per I2 and τ2 indexes, I2 = 98.29%, whereas τ2 = .31.

Publication bias.

According to Egger’s test, there was a publication bias (t = 3.01, p = .006). Selection model also demonstrated publication bias, χ2 (1) = 36.35, p < .001. After adjusting for publication bias, the relationship between negative affect and drinking volume was still positive and significant, r = .52, 95% CI [.35, .66], p < .001.

P-curve analysis indicated that evidential value is present, and that evidential value is not absent or inadequate (see Fig 4). This means that P-curve estimates that there is a “true” effect size underlying finding, and that the results are unlikely to be the product of publication bias and p-hacking alone. When correcting for selective reporting, the power of tests included into meta-analysis was 96% (see Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 4. P-curve plot for studies on positive affect and alcohol consumption.

Note: The observed p-curve includes 11 statistically significant (p < .05) results, of which 9 are p < .025. There were 39 additional results entered but excluded from p-curve because they were p > .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Estimated power of meta-analysis on positive affect and drinking volume.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.g005

Meta-regression.

Several moderators were examined: year of publication, country, study quality, study design (laboratory vs field), alcohol measure, whether studies examined distinct emotions or averaged them, and whether study considered affective state to be a continuum or not. While measuring amount consumed in units was a significant predictor of higher effect sizes in the initial model, this was not significant anymore when the model was reduced. Year published was a significant moderator—with later years, effect sizes decreased, t (48) = -2.93, p = .005. None of the other moderaors were significant.

Comparing the results of meta-analyses of negative and positive affect

Cohen’s q statistics was calculated by comparing the results for negative affect (r = .09) and positive affect (r = .17). The obtained q value was .08, indicating that there is no significant difference between the effect sizes of two coefficients.

Discussion

With the aim of examining the extent to which alcohol consumption can be explained theoretically by accounts which posit that people drink to enhance positive or to overcome negative affective states, meta-analyses of eligible non-clinical research outputs spanning 46 years were performed. Findings can be summarized as follows. First, both elevated negative and increased positive affect were associated with increased alcohol consumption volume, although the effect sizes were small. This may indicate that the mixed findings to date may be due to a predominance of underpowered individual studies in this field of research. Second, we did not find that affect measure used impacted the nature of the results observed within studies. Third, for negative affect, studies that used number of drinks as the alcohol consumption measure found lower effect sizes than research that used other metrics (i.e., number of units, amount in milliliters, number of sips, number of drinks). Other moderators were also examined in exploratory analyses (e.g., country), though the only significant moderator of effect sizes for positive affect was year published, pointing to a tendency for effect sizes to decline over time.

The relationship between daily affect and alcohol consumption volume: Theoretical implications

The results of our meta-analysis indicate that both negative and positive affective states over the course of a day are associated with increased consumption volume in non-clinical populations. This temporal positive association is consistent with affect regulation models of alcohol consumption (self-medication hypothesis, [3]; tension-reduction theory, [11]; stress-response dampening theory, [12]; negative reinforcement model of alcohol use, [4]; positive reinforcement theory of alcohol use, [26]). Our analyses build on the meta-analysis by Bresin et al. [2], which found that laboratory negative mood induction is associated with increased consumption, and extend this work in two ways. First, we demonstrate that the association between increased negative affect and alcohol consumption occurs in field as well as laboratory studies, thereby helping to overcome concerns regarding the ecological validity of laboratory-based work (although laboratory studies yielded higher effect sizes for negative affect). Second, we also found that positive affect, not previously considered meta-analytically, was also associated with elevated same day alcohol consumption.

The finding that both negative and positive affect are associated with increased alcohol consumption raises questions about whether it is necessary to retain both negative and positive reinforcement models of alcohol use, or whether a more parsimonious theoretical account of the mood-alcohol nexus may be possible. There are two reasons for entertaining this thought. First, the effect sizes for negative and positive affect were similar (.09 difference), and, when compared statistically using the q denominator [79], were not significantly different from each other. Therefore, increases of affect (i.e., affect intensity, [142,143]) may play a more determinant role than affect valence (i.e., pleasantness) in explaining the relationship between mood and consumption. Second, as indicated by the moderator analysis, whether distinct emotions were examined did not appear to impact the affect-alcohol relationship. Our analyses, in this way, indicate that specific emotions are not differently associated with consumption, but the intensity of these emotions is. For example, it did not make a difference whether happiness or sadness were examined as predictors of alcohol consumption; rather how strongly happiness or sadness were experienced appeared to be important.

What emerges from these findings is that it may be useful to subsume existing affect regulation models, which posit that alcohol consumption is driven by a desire to alleviate or heighten particular affective states, with an account that emphasises affect intensity: it may be that particular affective states per se are less important in explaining increased alcohol use than the regulation of their intensity. This model would suggest that, on a given drinking day, alcohol consumption is likely to be elevated in individuals whose mood is more intense (or whose affect is less ‘flat’). Such an approach does not contradict the notion that negative and positive affect are distinct entities. Instead, it asserts that the contents of emotions are of less importance in explaining alcohol consumption than the intensity with which these are experienced. The proposed account is consistent with the notion that people can experience negative and positive affect at the same time [57], and postulates that both may simultaneously shape alcohol consumption.

This way of thinking about drivers of alcohol consumption may have wider theoretical implications. For example, when considering the drinking motives literature [144,145]—which is also characterised by inconsistent and mixed finding (although see meta-analysis by [146])—it may be possible to think about distinct emotional drinking motive categories (enhancement and coping, [147]) in terms of ‘affect regulation’. In this way, Littlefield et al. found that drinkers who consume alcohol for either coping or enhancement motives do not form two distinct groups [14], suggesting that these motives may be best viewed as dimensional variables that covary such that individuals who are high in one internal motive tend to be high in the other motive. This is not to say that negative and positive affect (or coping / enhancement motives) predict all forms of alcohol consumption in the same way. Nevertheless, based on the results of our meta-analyses, these factors may similarly be associated with daily drinking volume on a drinking day. Future research could fruitfully investigate whether affect intensity is associated with other variables of interest such as drinking onset or craving.

Methodologically, an affect intensity regulation hypothesis of alcohol consumption suggests that studies could focus on affect intensity instead of overall levels of affect. This could involve asking participants to report the extent to which they feel the intensity of their negative and positive mood rather than asking them multiple questions about distinct emotions. Approaches requiring participants to only express the intensity of their overall negative and positive mood could lead to decreased participant burden and increased compliance in studies, which is particularly relevant to real-time research designs. While more research (in clinical populations) is required, current findings suggest that prevention and intervention efforts might usefully target overall levels of affect rather than focusing on affect valence. As such, providing individuals with alternative strategies for improving affect regulation may be of particular benefit.

Moderator analyses

Moving on from possible theoretical implications of current findings, it is worthwhile to briefly consider significant moderators of the affect-alcohol consumption relationship. For negative affect, we found that studies that used number of drinks as an outcome tended to generate lower effect sizes than research that used other measures of drinking behavior such as the amount consumed in milliliters. For positive affect, year of publication was a significant moderator pointing to a tendency for effect sizes to decline over time. This is perhaps an indication that the field of alcohol research is not immune to the well-documented decline effect [148] whereby effect sizes can decrease over time for a variety of possible reasons that include false positive results, overestimation of effect sizes, under-specification of the conditions of the study, or genuinely decreasing effect sizes [149]. In the current context, it is also possible that advances in methodology and statistical analysis may have contributed to a more accurate effect size detection with the passage of time.

Limitations and further research

The results of the current meta-analyses need to be considered in light of a number of limitations. First, the original correlation coefficients were not always available, and were extracted from standardised beta weights [65] which were converted from d, obtained from unstandardized beta weights [66], or F-values [67], using an online effect size calculator [Lenhard & Lenhard, Unpublished]. Furthermore, as r can only be obtained from standardised beta coefficient when it is between -.05 and .05, one study had to be excluded from analyses, while another study was omitted as it did not report any statistics from which effect sizes could be calculated. As such, the current meta-analysis for negative affect could not exhaustively represent all published data, although sensitivity analyses conducted indicated that there would have been no significant differences if it had been possible to use the excluded studies in the analyses. Second, while we separated the meta-analyses based on affect valence, we did not account for difference in affect arousal and how this could potentially impact the relationship between mood and consumption.

It is also important to note that our meta-analyses were concerned with drinking volume. That is, we examined whether intra-day affect influenced the amount of alcohol consumed on the day and future research could therefore usefully examine other variables of interest such as drinking onset, likelihood, blood alcohol concentration, or alcohol cravings. Similarly, further studies may consider examining the differences in the relationship between daily affect and heavy drinking (versus any drinking, as was examined in the present review). Moreover, this review only focused on intra-day consumption. While this allowed us to examine the association between state affect, further examination of trait affect (i.e., tendency to experience particular affective states) could help answer the question how longer-term affective states may be associated with alcohol consumption (e.g., [150]). Furthermore, the current meta-analyses focused on affective states in general without looking at potential differences between mood and emotional state. While this decision was made because there is an overlap between these constructs and, for this reason, many studies use these terms interchangeably, further meta-analytical studies may wish to look at conceivably different effects of mood versus emotional state on alcohol consumption.

We also recommend that future research should routinely report the direct relationship between mood and consumption, and include correlation coefficients between all variables of interest. More generally, there is also a need for studies to be adequately powered and to conduct longitudinal investigations given the dominance of cross-sectional work in this area. As outlined, future research may also benefit from utilising affect intensity as a primary outcome variable as this could help reduce participant burden. Furthermore, since most of the studies on the topic were conducted in USA, additional research in other national contexts, which may differ with regards to the sociocultural positioning of alcohol [151], is advised.

Conclusion

Overall, results of the present meta-analyses converge to suggest that both positive and negative affective states are associated with elevated daily alcohol consumption volume in non-clinical populations. While in apparent support of both positive and negative reinforcement models, present findings thereby suggest that greater insights into the relationship between mood and alcohol may be garnered through a more parsimonious focus on the intensity of emotional experiences (i.e., aggregate intensity of both negative and positive affect) rather than on valence. Consistent with idea that facets of positive and negative affect may be experienced simultaneously it therefore appears possible to posit an affect intensity regulation hypothesis. According to this, the intensity (rather than valence) of people’s affective states on a given drinking day is associated with increased consumption of alcohol. While future research is required to test this theory, it is evident that much remains to be uncovered with regards to the mood-alcohol nexus, and that this endeavour will continue to exercise philosophers, writers, and scientists for some time to come.

Supporting information

S1 File. S1, S2—Unpussblished references.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.s002

(DOCX)

References

  1. 1. Bukowski C. Women. USA: HarperCollins Publishers; 1978. 632 p.
  2. 2. Bresin K, Mekawi Y, Verona E. The Effect of Laboratory Manipulations of Negative Affect on Alcohol Craving and Use: A Meta-analysis. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2018 Sep;32(6):617–27.
  3. 3. Khantzian E J. The Self-Medication Hypothesis of Substance Use Disorders: A Reconsideration and Recent Applications: Harvard Review of Psychiatry: Vol 4, No 5. Taylor & Francis Online. 1996 [cited 2020 Dec 3]. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.3109/10673229709030550.
  4. 4. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 2004 Jan;111(1):33–51. pmid:14756584
  5. 5. Appleton A, James R, Larsen J. The Association between Mental Wellbeing, Levels of Harmful Drinking, and Drinking Motivations: A Cross-Sectional Study of the UK Adult Population. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018 Jul;15(7):1333.
  6. 6. Bolton JM, Robinson J, Sareen J. Self-medication of mood disorders with alcohol and drugs in the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Affect Disord. 2009 Jun 1;115(3):367–75. pmid:19004504
  7. 7. Cho SB, Su J, Kuo SI-C, Bucholz KK, Chan G, Edenberg HJ, et al. Positive and negative reinforcement are differentially associated with alcohol consumption as a function of alcohol dependence. Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33(1):58–68. pmid:30667237
  8. 8. Holt LJ, Armeli S, Tennen H, Austad CS, Raskin SA, Fallahi CR, et al. A person-centered approach to understanding negative reinforcement drinking among first year college students. Addict Behav. 2013 Dec 1;38(12):2937–44. pmid:24064193
  9. 9. Shadur JM, Hussong AM, Haroon M. Negative affect variability and adolescent self-medication: The role of the peer context. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015 Nov;34(6):571–80. pmid:25867550
  10. 10. Stewart SH, Morris E, Mellings T, Komar J. Relations of social anxiety variables to drinking motives, drinking quantity and frequency, and alcohol-related problems in undergraduates. J Ment Health. 2006 Jan 1;15(6):671–82.
  11. 11. Conger J. Reinforcement theory and the dynamics of alcoholism. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1956;17:296–305. pmid:13336262
  12. 12. Sher K, Levenson R. Risk for alcoholism and individual differences in the stress-response-dampening effect of alcohol. 1982 [cited 2021 Mar 30]; /paper/Risk-for-alcoholism-and-individual-differences-in-Sher-Levenson/32ec3b4e27cc27041a41575b41654f914f720b25.
  13. 13. Greeley J, Oei T. Alcohol and tension reduction. In: Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism, 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press; 1999. p. 14–53. (The Guilford substance abuse series).
  14. 14. Swendsen JD, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G, Willard A, Hromi A. Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test of the self-medication hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(2):198–204. pmid:10895557
  15. 15. Field M, Quigley M. Mild stress increases attentional bias in social drinkers who drink to cope: A replication and extension.—PsycNET. 2009 [cited 2021 Apr 9]. https://doi.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fa0017090. pmid:19803630
  16. 16. Hepworth R, Mogg K, Brignell C, Bradley BP. Negative mood increases selective attention to food cues and subjective appetite. Appetite. 2010 Feb 1;54(1):134–42. pmid:19815043
  17. 17. Higgins R, Marlatt G. Effects of anxiety arousal on the consumption of alcohol by alcoholics and social drinkers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1973;41(3):426–33. pmid:4803274
  18. 18. Marlatt GA, Kosturn CF, Lang AR. Provocation to anger and opportunity for retaliation as determinants of alcohol consumption in social drinkers. J Abnorm Psychol. 1975;84(6):652–9. pmid:1194526
  19. 19. Moskal D, Maisto SA, De Vita M, Ditre JW. Effects of experimental pain induction on alcohol urge, intention to consume alcohol, and alcohol demand. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;26(1):65–76. pmid:29323505
  20. 20. Fairlie AM, Cadigan JM, Patrick ME, Larimer ME, Lee CM. Unplanned Heavy Episodic and High-Intensity Drinking: Daily-Level Associations With Mood, Context, and Negative Consequences. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2019;80(3):331–9. pmid:31250798
  21. 21. Bresin K, Fairbairn CE. The Association Between Negative and Positive Affect and Alcohol Use: An Ambulatory Study. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2019;80(6):614–22. pmid:31790351
  22. 22. Breslin FC, O’Keeffe , Burrell L, Ratliff-Crain J, Baum A. The effects of stress and coping on daily alcohol use in women. Addict Behav. 1995;20(2):141–7. pmid:7484308
  23. 23. de Leon AND, Dvorak RD, Kramer MP, Peterson R, Pinto DA, Leary AV, et al. Daily Patterns of Emotional Functioning on Drinking and Nondrinking Days. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2020;44(12):2598–610. pmid:33206419
  24. 24. Dvorak RD, Stevenson BL, Kilwein TM, Sargent EM, Dunn ME, Leary AV, et al. Tension Reduction and Affect Regulation: An Examination of Mood Indices on Drinking and Non-Drinking Days among University Student Drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;26(4):377–90. pmid:29985019
  25. 25. Simons JS, Dvorak RD, Batien BD, Wray TB. Event-level associations between affect, alcohol intoxication, and acute dependence symptoms: Effects of urgency, self-control, and drinking experience. Addict Behav. 2010;35(12):1045–53. pmid:20685044
  26. 26. de Wit H, Phan L. Positive reinforcement theories of drug use. In: Substance abuse and emotion. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2010. p. 43–60.
  27. 27. Howard AL, Patrick ME, Maggs JL. College student affect and heavy drinking: Variable associations across days, semesters, and people. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29(2):430–43.
  28. 28. Weiss NH, Forkus SR, Contractor AA, Schick MR. Difficulties regulating positive emotions and alcohol and drug misuse: A path analysis. Addict Behav. 2018;84:45–52. pmid:29625262
  29. 29. Stein KD, Goldman MS, Boca FKD. The Influence of Alcohol Expectancy Priming and Mood Manipulation on Subsequent Alcohol Consumption. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(1):106–15. pmid:10740941
  30. 30. VanderVeen JD, Plawecki MH, Millward JB, Hays J, Kareken DA, O’Connor S, et al. Negative urgency, mood induction, and alcohol seeking behaviors. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;165:151–8. pmid:27291583
  31. 31. Holzhauer CG, Wemm SE, Wulfert E, Cao Z (Tim). Fluctuations in progesterone moderate the relationship between daily mood and alcohol use in young adult women. Addict Behav. 2020;101:106146. pmid:31639640
  32. 32. Monk RL, Qureshi A, Heim D. An examination of the extent to which mood and context are associated with real-time alcohol consumption. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2020;208. Available from: https://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-85079050841&doi=10.1016%2fj.drugalcdep.2020.107880&partnerID=40&md5=b1c9153d4d5fc50146f778d2d82cf21d. pmid:32004997
  33. 33. Dvorak RD, Simons JS. Daily associations between anxiety and alcohol use: Variation by sustained attention, set shifting, and gender. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(4):969–79. pmid:25180552
  34. 34. Green D, Goldman S, Salovey P. Measurement Error Masks Bipolarity in Affect Ratings. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:1029–41. pmid:8326466
  35. 35. Russell J. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(6):1161–78.
  36. 36. Matthews G, Jones DM, Chamberlain AG. Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist. Br J Psychol. 1990;81(1):17–42.
  37. 37. Russell JA, Weiss A, Mendelsohn GA. Affect Grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;57(3):493–502.
  38. 38. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063–70. pmid:3397865
  39. 39. Stern RA, Arruda JE, Hooper CR, Wolfner GD, Morey CE. Visual analogue mood scales to measure internal mood state in neurologically impaired patients: Description and initial validity evidence. Aphasiology. 1997;11(1):59–71.
  40. 40. Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The Experience of Emotion. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58(1):373–403. pmid:17002554
  41. 41. Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS. The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17(3):715–34. pmid:16262989
  42. 42. Ekman P. Emotions revealed. BMJ. 2004;328(Suppl S5):0405184.
  43. 43. Lench HC, Flores SA, Bench SW. Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: a meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(5):834–55. pmid:21766999
  44. 44. Duif M, Thewissen V, Wouters S, Lechner L, Jacobs N. Affective Instability and Alcohol Consumption: Ecological Momentary Assessment in an Adult Sample. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2019;80(4):441–7. pmid:31495381
  45. 45. Richardson CME, Hoene THM, Rigatti HL. Self-critical perfectionism and daily drinking to cope with negative emotional experiences among college students. Personal Individ Differ. 2020;156:109773.
  46. 46. Stamates AL, Linden-Carmichael AN, Preonas PD, Lau-Barraco C. Testing daily associations between impulsivity, affect, and alcohol outcomes: a pilot study. Addict Res Theory. 2019;27(3):242–8. pmid:31191189
  47. 47. O’Donnell R, Richardson B, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M, Liknaitzky P, Arulkadacham L, Dvorak R, et al. Ecological momentary assessment of drinking in young adults: An investigation into social context, affect and motives. Addict Behav. 2019;98:106019. pmid:31247534
  48. 48. Rohsenow DJ, Smith RE, Johnson S. Stress management training as a prevention program for heavy social drinkers: Cognitions, affect, drinking, and individual differences. Addict Behav. 1985;10(1):45–54. pmid:3890473
  49. 49. Feldman LA. Distinguishing Depression and Anxiety in Self-Report: Evidence from Confirmatory Factor Analysis on Nonclinical and Clinical Samples. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1993;61(4):631–8. pmid:8370858
  50. 50. Tsai JL. Ideal Affect: Cultural Causes and Behavioral Consequences. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2(3):242–59. pmid:26151968
  51. 51. Kuntsche E, Bruno R. Moody booze: Introducing the special section on affect regulation and substance use. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;34(6):569–70. pmid:26494198
  52. 52. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological Momentary Assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4(1):1–32.
  53. 53. Larsen , Engels RCME, Granic I, Overbeek G. An Experimental Study on Imitation of Alcohol Consumption in Same-Sex Dyads. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44(3):250–5. pmid:19240054
  54. 54. Spijkerman R, Larsen H, Gibbons FX, Engels RCME. Students’ Drinker Prototypes and Alcohol Use in a Naturalistic Setting. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;34(1):64–71. pmid:19860805
  55. 55. Quigley BM, Collins RL. The modeling of alcohol consumption: A meta-analytic review. J Stud Alcohol. 1999;60(1):90–8. pmid:10096313
  56. 56. Jones A, Button E, Rose AK, Robinson E, Christiansen P, Di Lemma L, et al. The ad-libitum alcohol ‘taste test’: secondary analyses of potential confounds and construct validity. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233(5):917–24. pmid:26680342
  57. 57. Diener E, Emmons RA. The independence of positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984 Nov;47(5):1105–17. pmid:6520704
  58. 58. Larsen , McGraw A, Cacioppo J. Can People Feel Happy and Sad at the Same Time? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81(4):684–96. pmid:11642354
  59. 59. Ekkekakis P. Affect, mood, and emotion. In: Measurement in sport and exercise psychology. Champaign, IL, US: Human Kinetics; 2012. p. 321–32.
  60. 60. Page MJ, Moher D, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n160. pmid:33781993
  61. 61. APA. Reporting Standards for Research in Psychology. Am Psychol. 2008;63(9):839–51. pmid:19086746
  62. 62. APA. Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, Sixth Editio. https://apastyle.apa.org/products/4200066.
  63. 63. Kmet LM, Cook LS, Lee RC. Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary Research Papers from a Variety of Fields. ERA. 2004 [cited 2021 May 19]. https://era.library.ualberta.ca/items/48b9b989-c221-4df6-9e35-af782082280e.
  64. 64. Thalheimer W, Cook S. How to calculate effect sizes from published research: A simplified methodology. Work-Learning Research. 2002;1:1–9.
  65. 65. Peterson RA, Brown SP. On the Use of Beta Coefficients in Meta-Analysis. J Appl Psychol. 2005;90(1):175–81. pmid:15641898
  66. 66. Feingold A. A Regression Framework for Effect Size Assessments in Longitudinal Modeling of Group Differences. Rev Gen Psychol J Div 1 Am Psychol Assoc. 2013 Mar 1;17:111–21. pmid:23956615
  67. 67. Lipsey MW, Wilson D. Practical Meta-Analysis. 1st edition. Thousand Oaks, Calif: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2000. 264 p.
  68. 68. Baird HM, Webb TL, Sirois FM, Gibson-Miller J. Understanding the effects of time perspective: A meta-analysis testing a self-regulatory framework. Psychol Bull. 2021;147(3):233–67. pmid:33180512
  69. 69. RStudio | Open source & professional software for data science teams. [Internet]. N.D. [cited 2021 Jan 29]. https://rstudio.com/.
  70. 70. Balduzzi S, Rücker G, Schwarzer G. How to perform a meta-analysis with R: a practical tutorial. Evid Based Ment Health. 2019;22(4):153–60. pmid:31563865
  71. 71. Hedges LV, Tipton E, Johnson MC. Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Res Synth Methods. 2010;1(1):39–65. pmid:26056092
  72. 72. Fisher Z, Tipton E. robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. 2015.
  73. 73. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–34. pmid:9310563
  74. 74. JASP team. JASP. 2020.
  75. 75. Bartoš F, Maier M, Wagenmakers E-J. Adjusting for Publication Bias in JASP—Selection Models and Robust Bayesian Meta-Analysis. PsyArXiv; 2020 [cited 2021 Sep 20]. https://psyarxiv.com/75bqn/.
  76. 76. Simonsohn U, Nelson LD, Simmons JP. P-curve: A key to the file-drawer. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143(2):534–47. pmid:23855496
  77. 77. Harrer M, Cuijpers P, Furukawa TA, Ebert DD. Doing Meta-Analysis with R: A Hands-On Guide. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2021. 500 p.
  78. 78. Huizenga H m, Visser I, Dolan C. Testing overall and moderator effects in random effects meta-regression. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2011;64:1–19. pmid:21506942
  79. 79. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Routledge; 1988. 579 p.
  80. 80. Austin HRT, Notebaert L, Wiers RW, Salemink E, MacLeod C. Potions for Emotions: Do self-reported individual differences in negative-emotional drinking predict alcohol consumption in the laboratory following exposure to a negative experience? Addict Behav Rep. 2020;11:100243. pmid:32467832
  81. 81. Cyders MA, Zapolski TCB, Combs JL, Settles RF, Fillmore MT, Smith GT. Experimental Effect of Positive Urgency on Negative Outcomes From Risk Taking and on Increased Alcohol Consumption. Psychol Addict Behav. 2010;24(3):367–75. pmid:20853921
  82. 82. de Castro JM. Social, circadian, nutritional, and subjective correlates of the spontaneous pattern of moderate alcohol intake of normal humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;35(4):923–31. pmid:2345766
  83. 83. Dinc L, Cooper AJ. Positive affective states and alcohol consumption: The moderating role of trait positive urgency. Addict Behav. 2015;47:17–21. pmid:25863003
  84. 84. Gabel PC, Noel NE, Keane TM, Lisman SA. Effects of sexual versus fear arousal on alcohol consumption in college males. Behav Res Ther. 1980;18(6):519–26. pmid:7447874
  85. 85. Gautreau C, Sherry S, Battista S, Goldstein A, Stewart S. Enhancement motives moderate the relationship between high-arousal positive moods and drinking quantity: Evidence from a 22-day experience sampling study. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;6(34):595–602. pmid:25735401
  86. 86. Larsen R, Diener E. Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. Rev Personal Soc Psychol Emot. 1992;13.
  87. 87. Liu S, Wang MO, Zhan Y, Shi J. Daily work stress and alcohol use: testing the cross-level moderation effects of neuroticism and job involvement. Pers Psychol. 2009;62(3):575–9.
  88. 88. Gable SL, Reis HT, Elliot AJ. Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(6):1135–49. pmid:10870914
  89. 89. Mohr CD, Arpin S, McCabe CT. Daily affect variability and context-specific alcohol consumption. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;34(6):581–7. pmid:25735819
  90. 90. Mohr CD, Brannan D, Wendt S, Jacobs L, Wright R, Wang M. Daily mood–drinking slopes as predictors: A new take on drinking motives and related outcomes. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(4):944–55. pmid:23647154
  91. 91. Peacock A, Cash C, Bruno R, Ferguson SG. Day-by-day variation in affect, arousal and alcohol consumption in young adults. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;34(6):588–94. pmid:25588504
  92. 92. Pihl RO, Yankofsky L. Alcohol consumption in male social drinkers as a function of situationally induced depressive affect and anxiety. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1979;65(3):251–7. pmid:117496
  93. 93. Zuckerman M, Lubin B. The Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist (MAACL). 1965; https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/The-Multiple-Affect-Adjective-Checklist-(MAACL)-Zuckerman-Lubin/c94a8b1f3c4ba0907399357518069a48d677d9b8.
  94. 94. Rohsenow DJ. Control over interpersonal evaluation and alcohol consumption in male social drinkers. Addict Behav. 1982;7(2):113–21. pmid:7102442
  95. 95. Simons JS, Gaher RM, Oliver MNI, Bush JA, Palmer MA. An experience sampling study of associations between affect and alcohol use and problems among college students. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(4):459–69. pmid:16240553
  96. 96. Simons JS, Wills TA, Neal DJ. The many faces of affect: A multilevel model of drinking frequency/quantity and alcohol dependence symptoms among young adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(3):676–94. pmid:24933278
  97. 97. Watson D, Clark LA. The PANAS-X: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule—Expanded Form. University of Iowa; 1994 [cited 2021 Jan 29]. https://iro.uiowa.edu/discovery/fulldisplay/alma9983557488402771/01IOWA_INST:ResearchRepository.
  98. 98. Stasiewicz PR, Lisman SA. Effects of infant cries on alcohol consumption in college males at risk for child abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 1989;13(4):463–70. pmid:2819522
  99. 99. Stevenson BL, Dvorak RD, Kramer MP, Peterson RS, Dunn ME, Leary AV, et al. Within- and between-person associations from mood to alcohol consequences: The mediating role of enhancement and coping drinking motives. J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(8):813–22. pmid:31657596
  100. 100. Sutker. Alcohol use, negative mood states. 1983.
  101. 101. Wardell JD, Read JP, Curtin JJ, Merrill JE. Mood and Implicit Alcohol Expectancy Processes: Predicting Alcohol Consumption in the Laboratory. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36(1):119–29. pmid:21797887
  102. 102. Mohr CD, Tennen H, Temple M, Clark J, Armeli S, Todd M, et al. Moving beyond the keg party: A daily process study of college student drinking motivations. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(4):392–403. pmid:16366811
  103. 103. Hussong AM, Galloway CA, Feagans LA. Coping motives as a moderator of daily mood-drinking covariation. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(3):344–53. pmid:16047523
  104. 104. Todd M, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G. A Daily Diary Validity Test of Drinking to Cope Measures. Psychol Addict Behav. 2003;17(4):303–11. pmid:14640826
  105. 105. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A Global Measure of Perceived Stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385–96. pmid:6668417
  106. 106. Carney MA, Armeli S, Tennen H, Affleck G, O’Neil TP. Positive and negative daily events, perceived stress, and alcohol use: A diary study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68(5):788–98. pmid:11068965
  107. 107. Armeli S, Carney MA, Tennen H, Affleck G, O’Neil TP. Stress and alcohol use: A daily process examination of the stressor–vulnerability model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(5):979–94. pmid:10821203
  108. 108. Stone AA, Neale JM. Effects of severe daily events on mood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984;46(1):137–44. pmid:6694057
  109. 109. Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Day AM. Ecological Momentary Assessment of Acute Alcohol Use Disorder Symptoms: Associations With Mood, Motives, and Use on Planned Drinking Days. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22(4):285–97. pmid:24932896
  110. 110. Thomas SE, Merrill JE, von Hofe J, Magid V. Coping Motives for Drinking Affect Stress Reactivity but Not Alcohol Consumption in a Clinical Laboratory Setting. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2014;75(1):115–23. pmid:24411803
  111. 111. Grant VV, Stewart SH, Mohr CD. Coping-anxiety and coping-depression motives predict different daily mood-drinking relationships. Psychol Addict Behav. 2009;23(2):226–37. pmid:19586139
  112. 112. Steptoe A, Wardle J. Mood and drinking: a naturalistic diary study of alcohol, coffee and tea. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;141(3):315–21.
  113. 113. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. POMS manual. Educational and Industrial Testing Service. 1981.
  114. 114. Hull JG, Young RD. Self-consciousness, self-esteem, and success–failure as determinants of alcohol consumption in male social drinkers. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44(6):1097–109. pmid:6875802
  115. 115. O’Hara RE, Armeli S, Tennen H. College students’ daily-level reasons for not drinking. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2014;33(4):412–9. pmid:24976084
  116. 116. Todd M, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Ball SA, Kranzler HR, et al. Drinking to cope: a comparison of questionnaire and electronic diary reports. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(1):121–9. pmid:15830912
  117. 117. Todd M, Armeli S, Tennen H. Interpersonal problems and negative mood as predictors of within-day time to drinking. Psychol Addict Behav. 2009;23(2):205–15. pmid:19586137
  118. 118. Collins RL, Morsheimer ET, Shiffman S, Paty JA, Gnys M, Papandonatos GD. Ecological momentary assessment in a behavioral drinking moderation training program. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1998;6(3):306–15. pmid:9725114
  119. 119. Ehrenberg E, Armeli S, Howland M, Tennen H. A daily process examination of episode-specific drinking to cope motivation among college students. Addict Behav. 2016;57:69–75. pmid:26894551
  120. 120. Higgins R, Marlatt G. Fear of interpersonal evaluation as a determinant of alcohol consumption in male social drinkers. J Abnorm Psychol. 1975;84(6):644–51. pmid:1194525
  121. 121. Holroyd KA. Effects of social anxiety and social evaluation on beer consumption and social interaction. J Stud Alcohol. 1978;39(5):737–44. pmid:672214
  122. 122. Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Sargent EM, Stevenson BL, Mfon AM. Daily associations between emotional functioning and alcohol involvement: Moderating effects of response inhibition and gender. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;163:S46–53. pmid:27306731
  123. 123. Armeli S, Dehart T, Tennen H, Todd M, Affleck G. Daily Interpersonal Stress and the Stressor–Vulnerability Model of Alcohol Use. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2007;26(8):896–921.
  124. 124. Corcoran KJ, Parker PS. Alcohol expectancy questionnaire tension reduction scale as a predictor of alcohol consumption in a stressful situation. Addict Behav. 1991;16(3):129–37.
  125. 125. Kidorf M, Lang AR. Effects of social anxiety and alcohol expectancies on stress-induced drinking. Psychol Addict Behav. 1999;13(2):134–42.
  126. 126. Tucker CJ. Remote sensing of leaf water content in the near infrared. Remote Sens Environ. 1980;10(1):23–32.
  127. 127. McGrath E, Jones A, Field M. Acute stress increases ad-libitum alcohol consumption in heavy drinkers, but not through impaired inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233(7):1227–34.
  128. 128. Magrys SA, Olmstead MC. Acute Stress Increases Voluntary Consumption of Alcohol in Undergraduates. Alcohol Alcohol. 2015;50(2):213–8. pmid:25557606
  129. 129. Spielberg CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults—PsycNET. 1983 [cited 2021 Sep 20]. https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft06496-000.
  130. 130. Aldridge-Gerry AA, Roesch SC, Villodas F, McCabe C, Leung Q, Da Costa M. Daily Stress and Alcohol Consumption: Modeling Between-Person and Within-Person Ethnic Variation in Coping Behavior. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011; 72(1):125–34. pmid:21138719
  131. 131. Emery NN, Simons JS. The role of affect, emotion management, and attentional bias in young adult drinking: An experience sampling study. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2020;1557–75.
  132. 132. Hamilton HR, Armeli S, Tennen H. Affect and alcohol: The moderating role of episode-specific drinking motives. Addict Behav. 2020;110:106521. pmid:32622025
  133. 133. Mohr CD, Brannan D, Mohr J, Armeli S, Tennen H. Evidence for positive mood buffering among college student drinkers. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008 Sep;34(9):1249–59. pmid:18550862
  134. 134. Schroder KEE, Perrine MW. Covariations of emotional states and alcohol consumption: Evidence from 2 years of daily data collection. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(12):2588–602. pmid:17761376
  135. 135. Waddell JT, Sher KJ, Piasecki TM. Coping motives and negative affect: An ecological study of the antecedents of alcohol craving and alcohol use. Psychol Addict Behav. 2021;35(5):565–76. pmid:33507786
  136. 136. Larsen H, Engels R, Granic I, Huizink A. Does Stress Increase Imitation of Drinking Behavior? An Experimental Study in a (Semi-)Naturalistic Context. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37.
  137. 137. Dieleman GC, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC. Perceived and physiological arousal during a stress task: Can they differentiate between anxiety and depression? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(8):1223–34. pmid:20219286
  138. 138. Sacco P, Burruss K, Smith CA, Kuerbis A, Harrington D, Moore AA, et al. Drinking behavior among older adults at a continuing care retirement community: affective and motivational influences. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19(3):279–89. pmid:25010351
  139. 139. Mackinnon A, Jorm AF, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Rodgers B. A short form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: evaluation of factorial validity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Personal Individ Differ. 1999;27(3):405–16.
  140. 140. Lindgren KP, Ramirez JJ, Wiers RW, Teachman BA, Norris J, Olin CC, et al. Mood selectively moderates the implicit alcohol association-drinking relation in college student heavy episodic drinkers. Psychol Addict Behav. 2018;32(3):338–49. pmid:29771561
  141. 141. Holland RW, de Vries M, Hermsen B, van Knippenberg A. Mood and the attitude–behavior link: The happy act on impulse, the sad think twice. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2012;3(3):356–64.
  142. 142. Diener E, Larsen RJ, Levine S, Emmons RA. Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;48(5):1253–65. pmid:3998989
  143. 143. Larsen RJ, Diener E. Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic: A review. J Res Personal. 1987;21(1):1–39.
  144. 144. Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model.—PsycNET. 1994 [cited 2021 May 20]. /doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2F1040-3590.6.2.117.
  145. 145. Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(5):990–1005. pmid:7473043
  146. 146. Bresin K, Mekawi Y. The “Why” of Drinking Matters: A Meta-Analysis of the Association Between Drinking Motives and Drinking Outcomes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2021;45(1):38–50. pmid:33206387
  147. 147. Littlefield AK, Talley AE, Jackson KM. Coping motives, negative moods, and time-to-drink: Exploring alternative analytic models of coping motives as a moderator of daily mood-drinking covariation. Addict Behav. 2012;37(12):1371–6. pmid:22867813
  148. 148. Schooler JW. Metascience could rescue the ‘replication crisis’. Nat News. 2014 Nov 6;515(7525):9. pmid:25373639
  149. 149. Protzko J, Schooler JW. Decline effects: Types, mechanisms, and personal reflections. In: Psychological science under scrutiny: Recent challenges and proposed solutions. Wiley Blackwell; 2017. p. 85–107.
  150. 150. Treeby M, Bruno R. Shame and guilt-proneness: Divergent implications for problematic alcohol use and drinking to cope with anxiety and depression symptomatology. Personal Individ Differ. 2012;53(5):613–7.
  151. 151. Gordon R, Heim D, MacAskill S. Rethinking drinking cultures: A review of drinking cultures and a reconstructed dimensional approach. Public Health. 2012;126(1):3–11. pmid:22137093