Figures
Abstract
Aim
Preterm birth(<37 gestational weeks) is associated with numerous adversities, however, data on positive developmental outcomes remain limited. We examined if preterm and term born(≥37 gestational weeks) adults differ in dispositional optimism/pessimism, a personality trait associated with health and wellbeing. We assessed if birth weight z-score, neurosensory impairments and parental education modified the outcome.
Methods
We systematically searched PubMed and Web of Science for cohort or case-control studies(born ≥ 1970) with data on gestational age and optimism/pessimism reported using the Life-Orientation-Test-Revised in adulthood(≥18 years). The three identified studies(Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults; Arvo Ylppö Longitudinal Study; Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children) provided data for the two-step random-effects linear regression Individual-Participant-Data meta-analysis.
Results
Preterm and term borns did not differ on optimism(p = 0.76). Preterms scored higher on pessimism than term borns(Mean difference = 0.35, 95%Confidence Interval 0.36, 0.60, p = 0.007), although not after full adjustment. Preterm born participants, but not term born participants, with higher birth weight z-score, had higher optimism scores (0.30 raw score units per standard deviation increase, 95% CI 0.10, 0.49, p = 0.003); preterm vs term x birth weight z-score interaction p = 0.004).
Citation: Robinson RK, Heinonen K, Girchenko P, Lahti-Pulkkinen M, Kajantie E, Hovi P, et al. (2021) Optimism in adults born preterm: Systematic review and individual-participant-data meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 16(11): e0259463. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0259463
Editor: Frank T. Spradley, University of Mississippi Medical Center, UNITED STATES
Received: June 21, 2021; Accepted: October 19, 2021; Published: November 18, 2021
Copyright: © 2021 Robinson et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: Data cannot be shared publicly because of it contains personal medical information. Data are available upon application from the University of Bristol (ALSPAC), and the University of Helsinki Developmental Psychology Research Group(HeSVA, AYLS) for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Appropriate data agreements and fees apply.
Funding: This project has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant agreement No 733280 for RECAP. The PremLife project is financially supported by the NORFACE Joint Research Programme on Dynamics of Inequality Across the Life-course, which is co-funded by the European Commission through Horizon 2020 under grant agreement No 724363 (Warwick University No. 462.16.100 and the University of Helsinki No 462.16.101). Funding also comes from the Academy of Finland (No 315690, 323910, 1284859, 12848591, 1312670, 1324596) and the Grant JUG 14 by the Federal Government of .Germany Ministry of Science and Technology (BMBF). For ALSPAC, funding was received from the UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and they will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf); This research was specifically funded by the Wellcome Trust MRC No 092731, Lifelong Health and Wellbeing (LLHW) via the MRC No G1001357. The Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults has in addition been supported by Foundation for Cardiovascular Research(Sydäntutkimussäätiö), Foundation for Pediatric Research(Lastentautien Tutkimussäätiö), Juho Vainio Foundation(Juha Vainion Säätiö), Novo Nordisk Foundation(Novo Nordisk Fonden), Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation(Signe ja Ane Gyllenberg Säätiö), Sigrid Juselius Foundation(Sigrid Juselius Säätiö) and Yrjö Jahnsson Foundation(Yrjö Jahnsson Säätiö). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Worldwide approximately 15 million preterm (<37 gestational weeks) births occur annually, equating to 1 in every 10 deliveries [1]. A major cause of global perinatal mortality and morbidity [2], preterm birth also increases the long term risks of physical [3, 4], mental [5], socioemotional [6], and cognitive [7] problems as well as socioeconomic-related adversities [8–11].
While evidence continues to mount on the negative outcomes following preterm birth, data remains limited on positive developmental outcomes—leaving a disproportionate picture as not all preterms experience adversities. One positive developmental outcome may be dispositional optimism [12, 13]. Individuals high on optimism hold generally positive expectations regarding their future life outcomes, whereas individuals high on pessimism hold negative ones [14]. Previous studies associate higher optimism with better physical health [15], exceptional longevity [16], and decreased risk of mortality [17, 18]. A comprehensive meta-analysis found a significant positive association between overall optimism/pessimism with aggregate positive physical health outcome measures [19]. Optimism has also been associated with lower rates of binge-eating [20]), better health-related quality of life [21], more positive daily mood [22, 23] and less suicidal ideation [24]. Furthermore, studies also indicate that optimists may recover faster or cope better with both physical and emotional stress [25, 26].
Rationale
To our knowledge, no study has explored if preterm and term born adults differ in dispositional optimism and pessimism. Hence, we systematically reviewed the literature and explored in an individual participant meta-analysis (IPD-MA), if young adults born preterm and at term differed in self-reported dispositional optimism and pessimism. Additionally, we also examined if the differences between preterm and term born adults in optimism and pessimism varied by birth weight z-score, neurosensory impairment, and parental education, because they have been identified as effect modifiers in previous literature on preterm birth [27, 28]. As preterm young adults have been shown to display adversities in health and wellbeing, we hypothesized that preterms would display lower optimism and higher pessimism than term borns. We also predicted that optimism would decrease and that pessimism would increase according to the degree of prematurity. However, importantly, as not all preterms experience adversities, we hypothesized that preterms born with higher birth weight z-scores, without neurosensory impairments and with higher parental education would display higher optimism and lower pessimism.
Materials and methods
Eligibility criteria
Cohort and longitudinal case-control studies with published or unpublished data beginning at birth or before, which had measured optimism/pessimism using the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R) in young adulthood (18–30 years), and had any gestational age data. Study populations born before 1970 were excluded due to the major changes in neonatal medicine.
Identification of studies: Search & information sources.
The original electronic search was performed in PubMed and Web of Science (inception to 2019) and an updated search was performed in June 2021 in PubMed, Web of Science, ProQuest and EBSCO/Host. The full search strategies are provided in S1 File. The study also collaborated with three major international preterm birth study networks where the availability of unpublished data could, also be assessed; PremLife [29], RECAP [30], APIC [3, 31–33], encompassing a wide breadth of preterm birth, long-term cohort studies in Europe, the Americas, New Zealand and Australia.
Selection of studies.
Study selection from the electronic searches occurred in three rounds; 1) Title review of all results for relevance, 2) Abstract review of identified studies for inclusion/exclusion criteria, and 3) Full-text review for final inclusion (Fig 1). For unpublished data, the preterm birth cohort network studies were reviewed for follow-up time points in adulthood, followed by data dictionary review to determine final inclusion based on same inclusion criteria set out for published studies. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) study website contains details of all the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool [34]. All other data dictionaries were provided by partners from the preterm birth study networks.
Ethical considerations.
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committee on human experimentation with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All study protocols were approved by the respective ethics committee of each participating institution and informed consents were obtained from all participants and their parents/guardian.
Data collection
We included all studies that met the inclusion criteria: ALSPAC [35, 36], the Arvo Ylppö Longitudinal Study(AYLS) [38] and the Helsinki Study of Very low birth weight Adults (HeSVA) [4, 28, 41, 39–51). Data dictionary review identified all necessary variables from each included study. All included studies received an itemized variable level data request with the proposed analysis plan. All missing and non-standard values (e.g. -9999, -11) were recoded as system missing. Data harmonization was performed in SPSS(RR) and was reviewed in duplicate (PG).
Specification of outcomes and effect measures
All measures for exposure, outcomes and covariates and moderators were pre-specified at the onset of the study. All included studies provided the requested LOT-R, gestational age, birth weight, perinatal and adult follow-up variables outlined below.
Optimism and pessimism outcome measurements.
Item level data for the participants’ LOT-R [14] measuring dispositional optimism/pessimism were requested and received from each cohort. Participants responded to optimism and pessimism items on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “strongly disagree” (0) to “strongly agree” (4). Positively worded items (n = 3) (e.g. ‘In uncertain times, I usually expect the best.’) were summed to calculate the optimism score and the negatively worded items (n = 3) to pessimism score (e.g. ‘I rarely count on good things to happen to me.’) [14]. The study analysed optimism and pessimism as empirically different constructs [35].
After harmonizing the LOT-R items, optimism and pessimism scales were scored as described previously [14]. In the case of only one missing value, the mean of the two available items replaced the missing value. Otherwise, the score was coded as missing. One participant lacked a pessimism scale score due to missing values, and hence we excluded this participant from the pessimism analyses.
Gestational age exposure measurements.
Gestational age measured via ultrasound or date of the mother’s last menstrual period was obtained from medical records. Preterm birth was defined as ≤36+6 (weeks+days) of gestational age and term birth as ≥37+0 gestational age. We further categorized preterm birth to Early Preterm(<32+0, n = 193), Moderate Preterm (32+0 to 33+6, n = 57), and Late Preterm (34+0 to 36+6, n = 275) births.
Covariates and moderators.
The variables considered as covariates included participant’s sex (women/men), as well as maternal mode of delivery(vaginal/cesarean section), smoking during pregnancy(yes/no), age at childbirth(years) and preeclampsia(yes/no), which were taken from medical records for all studies. The variables considered both as covariates and moderators included birth weight z-score, neurosensory impairment and parental education. Education level variables were harmonized according to the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) [36] and dichotomized into secondary education or less and tertiary education. Parental education was reported by the mother in childhood (ALSPAC, AYLS) and by the participant in adulthood(HeSVA) [36]. Birth weight was obtained from medical records and we calculated the birth weight z-score using INTERGROWTH-21 [37], the World Health Organization (WHO) international standardized growth curves which adjust for sex and gestational age. Neurosensory impairments were defined as yes if the participant had at least one of the following: cerebral palsy, intellectual disability (IQ<70), severe visual impairment or severe auditory deficit (n = 192) [4, 38–40]. Neurosensory impairments were assessed at the childhood clinical follow-ups in all three studies, with exception of IQ, which was assessed in adulthood in HeSVA [41].
Statistical analysis
Synthesis method.
To examine if preterm and term born young adults differed in optimism/pessimism and if optimism/pessimism varied according to the degree of prematurity, we employed a two-stage random-effects IPD meta-analysis. We fitted the linear regression models to the data from each cohort and then derived the random inverse-variance weighted pooled effects accounting for between-study heterogeneity tested using the Cochran’s Q statistic and quantified by the I2 value. Low heterogeneity was defined as an I2 value of 0% to 25%, moderate heterogeneity as an I2 of 25% to 75%, and high heterogeneity as an I2 of 75% to 100%.
Applying the same analytic approach, we examined if participant’s birth weight z-score, neurosensory impairments or parental education modified the developmental outcomes of those born preterm and at term. This was studied by including interaction terms of preterm birth vs term birth x birth weight z-score, preterm birth vs term birth x yes vs no neurosensory impairment and preterm birth vs term birth x parental education into the regression equations following the main effects of these variables.
We present the findings as adjusted for sex and for those covariates that were associated with dispositional optimism/pessimism. In preterm and term comparisons, we also conducted sensitivity analyses excluding participants with neurosensory impairment.
The study utilized Stata 15(StataCorp. 2017. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.).
Power analyses.
With alpha level 0.05 and 80% power we were able to detect differences between preterm born and term born adults and detect interactions that in effect size were small (N = 525 preterm adults and N = 4547 term born adults; Cohen’s d = 0.12 for the main effects and 0.48 for the interactions).
Results
Included studies
Study selection and IPD obtained.
The electronic search of PubMed and Web of Science yielded 165 studies. Hand searching of grey literature and the preterm birth cohort networks yielded another 23 possible studies. The updated search yielded an addition 100 studies. After title and abstract review, 5 published studies underwent full-text review. None of the published studies from the electronic search met the inclusion criteria. Of the preterm birth networks’ cohorts with unpublished data, 3 studies met inclusion criteria (Fig 1). After contacting all three studies, each study provided the requested IPD for the analyses. The approved research proposal and data request for ALSPAC, #B3202 from the University of Bristol for the PremLife project can be found online [42]. Data collection for the ALSPAC cohort utilized the REDCap software [43, 44].
Quality of evidence assessment & IPD data integrity.
As no published studies met the inclusion criteria, the quality of previously published evidence could not be assessed. To evaluate IPD data integrity, the datasets received from each cohort were checked for abnormalities, missing data, out-of-range, inconsistent items or for harmonization issues. No issues were identified when reviewing the IPD provided from any of the three cohort studies. We assessed the risk of bias in the IPD in duplicate (RR, KH) on the cohort level, using a modified version of the criteria set out by Crawford and colleagues [51] (S1 Table). Although common in adult follow ups of birth cohort studies, attrition was high, particularly in ALSPAC. We assessed the characteristics of participants lost to follow-up compared to those included in our analyses (S2 Table). Young adults who provided optimism/pessimism data in young adulthood in ALSPAC and AYLS were born more often at term, had higher birth weight z-scores, less often had neurosensory impairment, their mothers were older at childbirth, and their parents had higher education compared to those who did not provide optimism/pessimism data. In AYLS, non-participants were more often born via caesarean section than participants. In ALSPAC and HeSVA, participants’ mothers smoked less often during pregnancy and in all cohorts the participants were more often women. See S2 Table for detailed attrition data.
Characteristics of the study population
The IPD-MA study population comprised three birth cohorts: ALSPAC; Born 1990–92, UK [35, 36], AYLS; Born 1985–86, Finland [37, 38], and the Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults (HeSVA; Born 1978–85, Finland) [4, 28, 41, 39–51]. Table 1 describes the characteristics of the study population by cohort. The combined study population (n = 5072) included 525 participants who were born preterm between 1978–1992 in Finland and the United Kingdom. All three cohorts have been described in detail in previous publications [35–37, 40].
Among the combined datasets, mothers of preterm born participants were more likely than mothers of term borns to be primiparous, to have delivered via caesarean section, and to have had a multiple pregnancy or pre-eclampsia (Table 1). Fig 2 shows associations between the covariates and optimism (Panel A) and pessimism (Panel B). Men in comparison to women, and participants without neurosensory impairments, whose mothers did not smoke during pregnancy and whose parents had higher educational attainment, scored higher on optimism and lower on pessimism. Pessimism scores were also lower for those born to older mothers.
Optimism/Pessimism in preterm and term born young adults.
In the pooled IPD-MA adjusted for participant’s sex, preterm born and term born young adults did not differ on optimism (Fig 3), but preterm born young adults scored higher on pessimism (Fig 3). Excluding 192 individuals with neurosensory impairments from the analyses did not alter the findings (optimism: Mean difference (MD) = -0.02 raw score units [95%CI -0.26, 0.22], p = 0.88; pessimism: MD = 0.29 raw score units [95%CI 0.04, 0.55], p = 0.03). After further adjustments for those covariates that were significantly associated with optimism/pessimism, namely maternal age at delivery, parental education and maternal smoking during pregnancy, the difference in pessimism between preterm born and term born adults became non-significant (MD = 0.17 raw score units [95%CI -0.11, 0.46], p = 0.23).
Degree of preterm birth was not associated with optimism (p>0.47 for unadjusted and adjusted models), but by each category decrease in preterm from term birth, pessimism scores increased by 0.14 raw score units [(95%CI 0.01, 0.27, p = 0.04). This increase in pessimism, became non-significant after covariate adjustment (p = 0.26).
Birth weight z-score, neurosensory impairments and parental education in preterm and term born adults.
In the pooled IPD-MA adjusted for participants’ sex, preterm born adults with higher birth weight z-score scored higher on optimism, while birth weight z-score was not associated with optimism in individuals born at term (Fig 4; p = 0.004 for preterm birth vs term birth x birth weight z-score interaction). The association between birth weight z-score and optimism among preterm born adults remained significant after covariate adjustments (Fig 4). There were no such interactions on pessimism, and the interactions between preterm birth vs term birth x yes vs no neurosensory impairment and preterm birth vs term birth x parental education on optimism and pessimism were not significant (p-values>0.66 for interactions).
Discussion
Young adults born preterm and at term report no differences on optimism. This is in contrast to what we expected. However, we also found that preterm born adults scored higher on pessimism, although this difference was not significant after adjustments for covariates. A similar finding was observed according to the degree of prematurity in categorical gestational age groups. These findings help illuminate the similarities between preterm born and term born adults. Albeit surprisingly, given that preterm born individuals more often report neurocognitive, socioemotional and physical and mental health adversities [5, 10], which have individually been linked to lower optimism and higher pessimism [19, 21–24]. The lack of differences, however, may reflect that the adulthood follow-ups captured preterm born and term born young adults who fared better and would report less physical and mental health adversities than those who did not participate. While drop-out may offer some insight, all cohorts included in these analyses have individually reported that preterm birth to be associated with neurocognitive, socioemotional, and physical and mental health adversities in young adulthood [47, 52, 53].
However, in line with our hypothesis, we found that individuals born preterm with higher birth weight z-score had more optimistic life orientations. This finding aligns with previous literature where individuals born preterm and at Very Low Birth Weight(VLBW, <1500g), but whose birth weight was appropriate for gestational age(AGA; birth weight z-score within plus or minus 2 standard deviations or between the 10th and 90th percentile) report less often a depression diagnosis, use less often antidepressant medication, score lower on depressive symptoms [40] and report less problems in executive functioning [38] than individuals born preterm with VLBW and whose birth weight was small for gestational age (SGA; birth weight z-score less than minus 2 standard deviations or less than the 10th percentile). Furthermore, young adults born very preterm who were not born with VLBW have better neurodevelopmental and physical health outcomes than preterms born with VLBW [10]. Notably, family environment may also play a role, as fathers of individuals born AGA VLBW preterm reported more supportive parenting than fathers of individuals born SGA VLBW preterm [47]. More supportive parenting has been related to higher optimism [54] in offspring. Hence, higher birthweight in preterm individuals may foster developmental trajectories and environments that promote more optimistic life orientations. Whether the more optimistic life orientation of the higher birthweight preterm born adult reflects their better mental and physical health or is associated with higher resilience in the face of adversity remains unknown.
Although we found that in the entire sample, those without neurosensory impairments and those with higher parental education had higher optimism and lower pessimism scores, these associations did not differ between preterm born and term born peers. Higher optimism and lower pessimism scores in those without neurosensory impairments is logical and suggests that the adversities imposed by neurosensory impairments are independent gestational age. In addition, previous studies have shown that higher parental education is associated with higher optimism and lower pessimism [54].
Strengths & limitations
Our systematic review suggests that this is the first study to assess optimistic and pessimistic life orientations in preterm born and term born young adults using IPD meta-analysis with longitudinal cohort data from pregnancy into adulthood. The use of a singular outcome measure in all studies (LOT-R), reduces heterogeneity in study’s findings. The inclusion criteria that all cohorts included were from the modern neonatal medicine era, reduces confounding from different neonatal medicine practices, and curtails data attrition as more participants survived into adulthood. The collective statistical power of the IPD-MA, provides the largest assessment t of the optimism/pessimism in preterm born individuals to date. Study limitations include follow-up data attrition limiting the generalizability of the findings, as preterm born and term born individuals that did not participate in the follow-up may differ. While we were able to account for important covariates, we could not adjust for age at follow-up, as ALSPAC data precludes the sharing of birth dates and hence, calculation of age. However, all ALSPAC participants were followed-up at the age of 23 and the overall age range in the cohorts was 19 to 27.1 years.
Conclusions and implications
Our IPD-MA showed that preterm born and term born young adults do not differ in dispositional optimism. Important childhood, maternal and familial covariates explained the difference observed in the higher pessimism scores. However, preterm born adults with higher birth weight z-scores were more optimistic than lower birth weight preterm born adults, suggesting that higher birth weights in preterm born infants may foster developmental trajectories that promote more optimistic life orientations. To better reflect the reality of preterm born individuals, positive developmental outcomes following preterm birth warrant further investigation.
Supporting information
S2 Table. Maternal and birth characteristics of young adults who did provide optimism/pessimism data in comparison to young adults who did not provide optimism/pessimism data in young adulthood.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0259463.s003
(PDF)
Acknowledgments
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in the ALSPAC, AYLS, and HeSVA studies, the health care providers and research assistants for their help in recruiting them, and the entirety of the teams involved, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, research assistants, volunteers, managers, receptionists, physicians, psychologists, midwives and nurses.
References
- 1. Chawanpaiboon S, Vogel JP, Moller AB, Lumbiganon P, Petzold M, Hogan D, et al. Global, regional, and national estimates of levels of preterm birth in 2014: a systematic review and modelling analysis. Lancet Glob Health. 2019 Jan 1;7(1):e37–46. pmid:30389451
- 2. Liu L, Johnson HL, Cousens S, Perin J, Scott S, Lawn JE, et al. Global, regional, and national causes of child mortality: an updated systematic analysis for 2010 with time trends since 2000. Lancet. 2012 Jun;379(9832):2151–61.
- 3. Hovi P, Vohr B, Ment LR, Doyle LW, McGarvey L, Morrison KM, et al. Blood Pressure in Young Adults Born at Very Low Birth Weight: Adults Born Preterm International Collaboration. Hypertension. 2016 Oct;68(4):880–7. pmid:27572149
- 4. Hovi P, Andersson S, Eriksson JG, Järvenpää A-L, Strang-Karlsson S, Mäkitie O, et al. Glucose Regulation in Young Adults with Very Low Birth Weight. N Engl J Med. 2007; May 17;356(20):2053–63. pmid:17507704
- 5. Robinson R, Lahti-Pulkkinen M, Heinonen K, Schintzlein D, Voit F, Girchenko P, et al. Mental Health Outcomes of Adults Born Very Preterm or with Very Low Birth Weight: A systematic review. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2020; Jun;25(3):101113. pmid:32402835
- 6. Mendonça M, Bilgin A, Wolke D. Association of Preterm Birth and Low Birth Weight with Romantic Partnership, Sexual Intercourse, and Parenthood in Adulthood: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2(7):e196961. pmid:31298716
- 7. Brydges CR, Landes JK, Reid CL, Campbell C, French N, Anderson M. Cognitive outcomes in children and adolescents born very preterm: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018; May;60(5):452–468. pmid:29453812
- 8. Lindström K, Winbladh B, Haglund B, Hjern A. Preterm infants as young adults: A Swedish national cohort study. Pediatrics. 2007 Jan 1;120(1):70–7. pmid:17606563
- 9. Mathiasen R, Hansen BM, Nybo Anderson A-M, Greisen G. Socio-economic achievements of individuals born very preterm at the age of 27 to 29 years: A nationwide cohort study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009; 51(11), 901–908. pmid:19459908
- 10. Raju TNK, Buist AS, Blaisdell CJ, Moxey-Mims M, Saigal S. Adults born preterm: a review of general health and system-specific outcomes. Acta Paediatr. 2017;106(9):1409–37. pmid:28419544
- 11. Wolke D, Johnson S, Mendonça M. The Life Course Consequences of Very Preterm Birth. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. 2019; 1(1), 69–92.
- 12. Scheier MF, Carver CS. Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 1985;4(3):219–47. pmid:4029106
- 13. Rasmussen HN, Scheier MF, Greenhouse JB. Optimism and Physical Health: A Meta-analytic Review. Ann Behav Med. 2009 Jun 27;37(3):239–56. pmid:19711142
- 14. Scheier M. F., Carver C. S., & Bridges MW. LIFE ORIENTATION TEST–Revised (LOT-R). J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;, 67, 1063–1078. pmid:7815302
- 15. Scheier MF, Carver CS. Dispositional optimism and physical health: A long look back, a quick look forward. Am Psychol. 2018 Dec;73(9):1082–1094. pmid:30525784
- 16. Lee LO, James P, Zevon ES, Kim ES, Trudel-Fitzgerald C, Spiro A, et al. Optimism is associated with exceptional longevity in 2 epidemiologic cohorts of men and women. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019 Sep 10;116(37):18357–18362. pmid:31451635
- 17. Kim ES, Hagan KA, Grodstein F, DeMeo DL, De Vivo I, Kubzansky LD. Optimism and Cause-Specific Mortality: A Prospective Cohort Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2017 Jan 1;185(1):21–9. pmid:27927621
- 18. Giltay EJ, Kamphuis MH, Kalmijn S, Zitman FG, Kromhout D. Dispositional Optimism and the Risk of Cardiovascular Death. Arch Intern Med. 2006 Feb 27;166(4):431–6. pmid:16505263
- 19. Scheier MF, Swanson JD, Barlow MA, Greenhouse JB, Wrosch C, Tindle HA. Optimism Versus Pessimism as Predictors of Physical Health: A Comprehensive Reanalysis of Dispositional Optimism Research. Am Psychol. 2021 Apr;76(3):529–548. pmid:32969677
- 20. Mason TB, Lewis RJ. Examining social support, rumination, and optimism in relation to binge eating among Caucasian and African–American college women. Eat Weight Disord. 2017 Dec 29;22(4):693–8. pmid:27357464
- 21. Francescato D, Pezzuti L, Mebane M, Tomai M, Benedetti M, Moro A. Dispositional characteristics, relational well-being and perceived life satisfaction and empowerment of elders. Aging Ment Health. 2017 Oct;21(10):1052–1057. pmid:27248667
- 22. Schweizer K, Beck-Seyffer A, Schneider R. Cognitive Bias of Optimism and its Influence on Psychological Well-Being. Psychol Rep. 1999 Apr;84(2):627–36. pmid:10335077
- 23. Kleiman EM, Chiara AM, Liu RT, Jager-Hyman SG, Choi JY, Alloy LB. Optimism and well-being: a prospective multi-method and multi-dimensional examination of optimism as a resilience factor following the occurrence of stressful life events. Cog Emot. 2017 Feb 17;31(2):269–83. pmid:26558316
- 24. Hirsch JK, Conner KR, Duberstein PR. Optimism and Suicide Ideation Among Young Adult College Students. Arch Suicide Res. 2007;11(2):177–85. pmid:17453695
- 25. Kivimäki M, Vahtera J, Elovainio M, Helenius H, Singh-Manoux A, Pentti J. Optimism and Pessimism as Predictors of Change in Health After Death or Onset of Severe Illness in Family. Health Psychol. 2005;24(4):413–21. pmid:16045377
- 26. Shifren K, Anzaldi K. Optimism, well-being, depressive symptoms, and perceived physical health: a study among Stroke survivors. Psychol Health Med. 2018 Jan;23(1):46–57. pmid:28475351
- 27. Eves R, Mendonça M, Baumann N, Ni Y, Darlow BA, Horwood J, et al. Association of Very Preterm Birth or Very Low Birth Weight With Intelligence in Adulthood: An Individual Participant Data Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021 Aug 1;175(8):e211058. pmid:34047752
- 28. Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Hovi P, Järvenpää AL, et al. Depression in young adults with very low birth weight: The Helsinki study of very low-birth-weight adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008 Mar;65(3):290–6. pmid:18316675
- 29.
Premlife Project [Internet]. [cited 2019 Aug 5]. Available from: https://warwick.ac.uk/fac/sci/psych/research/lifespan/premlife/
- 30.
RECAP preterm [Internet]. [cited 2019 Aug 5]. Available from: https://recap-preterm.eu/
- 31.
APIC [Internet]. [cited 2019 Aug 5]. Available from: http://www.apic-preterm.org
- 32. Pyhälä R, Wolford E, Kautiainen H, Andersson S, Bartmann P, Baumann N, et al. Self-Reported Mental Health Problems Among Adults Born Preterm: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2017; Apr;139(4):e20162690. pmid:28283612
- 33. Doyle LW, Andersson S, Bush A, Cheong JLY, Clemm H, Evensen KAI, et al. Expiratory airflow in late adolescence and early adulthood in individuals born very preterm or with very low birthweight compared with controls born at term or with normal birthweight: a meta-analysis of individual participant data Lancet Respir Med. 2019 Aug;7(8):677–686. pmid:31078498
- 34.
Explore data and samples | Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children | University of Bristol [Internet]. [cited 2020 Jan 28]. Available from: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/
- 35. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, et al. Cohort profile: The ‘Children of the 90s’-The index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2013;42(1):111–27. pmid:22507743
- 36. Fraser A, Macdonald-wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Davey smith G, et al. Cohort profile: The avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013 Feb;42(1):111–27. pmid:22507743
- 37. Heinonen K, Raikkonen K, Pesonen A-K, Kajantie E, Andersson S, Eriksson JG, et al. Prenatal and Postnatal Growth and Cognitive Abilities at 56 Months of Age: A Longitudinal Study of Infants Born at Term. Pediatrics. 2008 May;121(5):e1325–33. pmid:18450875
- 38. Kumpulainen SM, Heinonen K, Salonen MK, Andersson S, Wolke D, Kajantie E, et al. Childhood cognitive ability and body composition in adulthood. Nutr & Diabetes. 2016 Aug 15; 6:e223. pmid:27525818
- 39. Pyhälä R, Räikkönen K, Pesonen A-K, Heinonen K, Hovi P, Eriksson JG, et al. Behavioral inhibition and behavioral approach in young adults with very low birth weight–The Helsinki study of very low birth weight adults. Pers Individ Dif. 2009 Jan;46(2):106–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.013
- 40. Kajantie E, Hovi P, Raikkonen K, Pesonen A-K, Heinonen K, Jarvenpaa A-L, et al. Young Adults With Very Low Birth Weight: Leaving the Parental Home and Sexual Relationships—Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults. Pediatrics. 2008 Jul;122(1):e62–72. pmid:18595976
- 41. Wehkalampi K, Hovi P, Strang-Karlsson S, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, et al. Reduced body size and shape-related symptoms in young adults born preterm with very low birth weight: Helsinki study of very low birth weight adults. J Pediatr. 2010 Sep;157(3):421–7, 427.e1. pmid:20400106
- 42. Herzberg PY, Glaesmer H, Hoyer J. Separating optimism and pessimism: A robust psychometric analysis of the Revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R). Psychol Assess. 2006 Dec;18(4):433–438. pmid:17154764
- 43.
OECD, European Union UI for S. ISCED 2011 Operational Manual: Guidelines for classifying national education programmes and related qualifications. ISCED 2011 Operational Manual: Guidelines for classifying national education programmes and related qualifications. OECD publishing, Paris; 2015. https://doi.org/https%3A//doi.org/10.1787/9789264228368-en
- 44. Villar J, Ismail LC, Victora CG, Ohuma EO, Bertino E, Altman DG, et al. International standards for newborn weight, length, and head circumference by gestational age and sex: the Newborn Cross-Sectional Study of the INTERGROWTH-21 st Project. Lancet. 2014 Sep 6;384(9946):857–68. pmid:25209487
- 45. Strang-Karlsson S, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Kajantie E, Paavonen EJ, Lahti J, et al. Very low birth weight and behavioral symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood: the Helsinki study of very-low-birth-weight adults. Am J Psychiatry. 2008 Oct;165(10):1345–53. pmid:18628349
- 46. Pesonen AK, Räikkönen K, Heinonen K, Andersson S, Hovi P, Järvenpää AL, et al. Personality of young adults born prematurely: The Helsinki study of very low birth weight adults. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008 Jun;49(6):609–17. pmid:18341548
- 47. Pyhälä R, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Lahti J, Hovi P, et al. Parental bonding after preterm birth: Child and parent perspectives in the Helsinki study of very low birth weight adults. J Pediatr. 2011 Feb;158(2):251–6.e1. pmid:20850763
- 48. Lemola S. B3202—Protective and Resiliency Factors for wellbeing after Preterm Birth A Multi-Cohort Study—06/11/2018, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children [Internet]. University of Bristol, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. 2018 [cited 2018 Nov 6]. Available from: https://proposals.epi.bristol.ac.uk/?q=node/129809
- 49. Harris PA, Taylor R, Minor BL, Elliott V, Fernandez M, O’Neal L, et al. The REDCap consortium: Building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inform. 2019 Jul;95:103208. pmid:31078660
- 50. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)-A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support J Biomed Inform. 2009 Apr;42(2):377–81. pmid:18929686
- 51. Crawford F, Cezard G, Chappell FM, Murray GD, Price JF, Sheikh A, et al. A systematic review and individual patient data meta-analysis of prognostic factors for foot ulceration in people with diabetes: the international research collaboration for the prediction of diabetic foot ulcerations (PODUS). Health Technol Assess. 2015 Jul;19(57):1–210. pmid:26211920
- 52. Odd D, Evans D, Emond AM. Prediction of school outcome after preterm birth: A cohort study. Arch Dis Child. 2019 Apr;104(4):348–353. pmid:30297444
- 53. Pyhala R, Raikkonen K, Feldt K, Andersson S, Hovi P, Eriksson JG, et al. Blood Pressure Responses to Psychosocial Stress in Young Adults With Very Low Birth Weight: Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults. Pediatrics. 2009 Feb;123(2):731–4. pmid:19171645
- 54. Heinonen K, Räikkönen K, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Dispositional optimism: Development over 21 years from the perspectives of perceived temperament and mothering. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;38(2):425–435.