Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The effect of risk framing on support for restrictive government policy regarding the COVID-19 outbreak

  • Kirill Chmel ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kirill Chmel, Aigul Klimova, Nikita Savin

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    kchmel@hse.ru

    Affiliations Ronald F. Inglehart Laboratory for Comparative Social Research, HSE University, Moscow, Russian Federation, Department of Integrated Communications, Faculty of Communications, Media, and Design, HSE University, Moscow, Russian Federation

  • Aigul Klimova ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kirill Chmel, Aigul Klimova, Nikita Savin

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Ronald F. Inglehart Laboratory for Comparative Social Research, HSE University, Moscow, Russian Federation, Department of Sociology, Faculty of Social Sciences, HSE University, Moscow, Russian Federation

  • Nikita Savin

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kirill Chmel, Aigul Klimova, Nikita Savin

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Sociology, Faculty of Social Sciences, HSE University, Moscow, Russian Federation, Politics & Psychology Research Laboratory, HSE University, Moscow, Russian Federation

Correction

5 Jul 2022: Chmel K, Klimova A, Savin N (2022) Correction: The effect of risk framing on support for restrictive government policy regarding the COVID-19 outbreak. PLOS ONE 17(7): e0271142. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0271142 View correction

Abstract

This confirmatory research investigates the influence of risk framing of COVID-19 on support for restrictive government policy based on two web survey experiments in Russia. Using 2x2 factorial design, we estimated two main effects–factors of risk severity (low vs. high) and object at risk (individual losses vs. losses to others). First, focusing on higher risks had a positive effect on support for the government’s restrictive policy. Second, focusing on the losses for others did not produce stronger support for the restrictive policy compared to focusing on personal losses. However, we found a positive moderation effect of such prosocial values as universalism and benevolence. We found that those with prosocial values had a stronger positive effect in the “losses for others” condition and were more willing to support government restrictive policy when others were included. The effects found in our experimental study reveal both positive and negative aspects in risk communication during the pandemic, which may have a great and long-term impact on trust, attitudes, and behavior.

Introduction

As COVID-19 turns into a pandemic, a political debate is simultaneously raging about whether autocracies or democracies are better at fighting epidemics [1]. Media pundits and global health officials praise draconian security measures imposed by the Chinese government to prevent the spread of COVID-19 [2, 3], and severely criticize the Swedish government for being excessively lax and soft about containing the virus [4]. But how many people would rather ’stay at home’ and keep a safe ’social distance’ instead of reaping the benefits of limitless freedom which most of them enjoyed before the COVID-19 outbreak? As of March 2020, approximately 75% of the world population, says Gallup International Association [5]. Indeed, according to a host of public opinion polls, the percentage of those willing to sacrifice some of their human rights to stave off the spread of coronavirus varies from 32% in Japan to 95% in Austria. In Russia, where the first cases had just been registered to that moment, there were 60% [5]. About a year later, in December 2020 the world on average became less willing to sacrifice rights to prevent the spread of the virus—the percentage dropped from 75% to 70%. At the same time, the statistics in Russia dramatically changed, since citizens were hardly willing to sacrifice their rights. Only 39% of Russian citizens were positive about their rights being trampled in order to resolve the public health crisis [6].

One of the possible explanations of willingness to sacrifice some human rights is rooted in the idea of risk perception as a driving force for decision-making. Research argues that risk perception drives support for security policies that infringe on civil liberties. Particularly, willingness to trade off civil liberties for security increases in the aftermath of exogenous shocks including, probably, public health outbreaks. The term, ‘When in danger, turn right’, from Karwowski et al. [7], demonstrates how the COVID-19 threat promotes social conservatism and support for right-wing candidates. Nonetheless, the perception of risk does not develop on its own, so media coverage, for instance, may introduce a disjuncture between perceptions of personal risk and objective estimates of population incidence [8].

Indeed, framing of a message, which is selection and emphasis on some aspects of a message, can have a greater impact on attitudes and behavior than the actual context [9]. Recent studies address the effects of different framings on following the protective measures during the pandemic. For instance, in line with the research on gain-loss framing, scholars found that gain-framed messages are more effective in promoting self-care behaviors [1012]. At the same time, other scholars either got null results [13], or found that framing effects are observed if conditioned by such individual characteristics as political ideology or socio-demographics [14, 15].

Risk communication is critical to managing public health outbreaks because of deep uncertainty and lack of issue localization [16]. Risk messages presented to citizens openly and timely aim to rectify the knowledge gap in understanding an epidemiological crisis and adjust the public’s behavior to cope with the risk proactively [17]. However, not all messages have the same effect on citizens’ behavior. For instance, while the World Health Organization (WHO) proclaimed the importance of being ‘supportive’ and ‘careful’ towards others [18], the Swedish strategy to manage COVID-19 has been largely based on the personal responsibility of the citizens who receive daily information about, and individually targeted instructions for, self-protection. Which strategy—to protect yourself or to take care of others—works better then?

Political effects of high and low risk framing

The psychometric paradigm in risk perception research suggests that the main risk categorizations are the level of dread people feel about the risk and the familiarity with the risk [19, 20]. The main factor was found to be the dread risk. The higher the score on this factor, the more individuals are willing to support any restrictive measures that can reduce this risk [19]. This paradigm emphasizes the importance of affective responses that individuals have towards potential risk. They coined the term ‘affect heuristic’ to describe these affective responses [20, 21]. Heuristics are biases whereby respondents use only part of the information with which they are provided [22]. Researchers found that the higher the level of dread people feel about the risks, especially if the risks are new and they are frequently discussed in media, the higher the perceived risks are [23].

Higher perceived risks are the main source of enforcing authoritarian attitudes. Social threats increase right-wing authoritarianism [2426]. The trade-off between civil rights and a high threat to society drives up the willingness to sacrifice rights to reinforce social order [27]. This can be found in risk situations where there is a threat to some particular groups or society overall in the aftermath of pandemics [28], violent crimes, political or economic crises [29], terrorist attacks [30], natural disasters [31], or climate change [32]. Overall, individuals tend to preserve collective security at the expense of freedom, autonomy, and rights, when there are high perceived risks to society.

We rely on the literature that emphasizes the psychological mechanisms behind framing effects and the literature which shows the effect of framing on a number of attitudes [33, 34]. Frames provide some meanings to the events, selecting certain aspects of the perceived reality and making them more salient in communication [35]. They make some opinions available for retrieval and accessible while being exposed to them. Following the distinction, which is made by Chong and Druckman [33], between issue and equivalence framing, we use the former approach to increase the ecological validity of research. We promote high-risk and low-risk as different considerations, since they are usually communicated by politicians. Despite their logical similarity they may not be necessarily perceived as a trade-off without public discussion. As a result, we expect to find that the high-risk framing of COVID-19 would increase support for restrictive government policy compared to low-risk framing.

  1. H1: High-risk framing will have a stronger effect on the support for restrictive government policy compared to low-risk framing.

Who is the main object at risk?

The question of ‘object at risk’ is one of the main categorizations which has an effect on risk perception [36]. COVID-19 is widely recognized to pose a threat to all age groups. However, according to statistics and media, as well as according to restrictive policy regarding particular groups of population, older people are particularly vulnerable, thus they are central to the COVID-19 risk [37]. The politicians and media justified restrictive measures by emphasizing that people themselves can be risk objects, which poses a threat to others, especially for those who are at higher risk. That produced the international ‘Stay home. Save lives’ media campaign. The discourse framed the restrictive COVID-19 measures as a way to save the lives of others. The WHO promoted not only protecting oneself from getting sick but others as well [18] along with being ‘supportive’ and ‘careful’ towards others.

Though economic perspective focuses on self-interest, a number of researchers stress the importance of prosocial behavior. Research shows that higher responsibility for oneself and for others regarding risky decisions, decreases risk willingness and risky behavior [38, 39]. There is evidence that people tend to be more risk averse when they make a choice for others, rather than for themselves [40]. Indicating that vaccination is a prosocial behavior, which results not only in direct protection of the vaccinated but also protection of unvaccinated individuals through herd immunity, increases the willingness to be vaccinated [41, 42]. Individuals tend to be more risk-averse and cautious when their risky behavior can have a negative effect on others [43, 44], or are more willing to take risks in order to help others [45].

In words of Mary Douglas [46], during the COVID-19 pandemic, everyone is described as potentially ‘contaminated’, ‘polluted’, and dangerous to others as everyone can be the asymptomatic carrier of COVID-19 without knowing it, as the symptoms can take up to 14 days to manifest themselves. As a result, everyone can be accused of being a threat to others, and society overall, and punished for not following the rules of self-isolation and staying at home, as breaking the rules can be dangerous to the health of others and society.

Recent research on the effect of prosocial messages on COVID-19 prevention behaviors showed some mixed evidence. While some experimental studies found almost no difference between self-focused and prosocial framing on the willingness to self-isolate and wash hands more often [47, 48], other studies found that prosocial framing indeed increased such prevention behavior as social distancing [4951] and wearing masks [52]. Some other experimental studies found no effect of prosocial framing on clickthrough rates while delivering advertisements on Facebook, compared to self-focused framing [53] and comprehension of the information regarding recommended behaviors [54]. In spite of this mixed evidence, we suggest that the framing which highlights losses to others would increase support for restrictive government policy compared to the condition in which we highlight only personal losses.

  1. H2: The object at risk which focuses on losses to others will have a stronger effect on the support for restrictive government policy compared to the personal losses framing.

Prosocial values as moderators of framing effects

Following the argument made by Chong and Druckman [33], we suggest that the effect of frames on attitudes is moderated by personal values. We assumed that the frame affects only particular types of individuals with strong prosocial attitudes. It was found in previous literature that such variables as values [55, 56] or personality traits [57] could explain the variation in attitudes towards prosocial behavior like moral decision-making or altruism. According to the norm activation theory, different frames would activate particular personal values depending on the individual’s cognitive structure of these values [55]. As a result, the ‘losses to others’ frame may have an effect only if this is in line with personal values. Some studies showed that higher empathy increases willingness to self-isolate and maintain social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic [58, 59].

We would apply the Schwartz theory of basic human values and his approach to the measurement of values [60]. Schwartz suggested that there are ten basic human values across cultures. Two of them are in the self-transcendence direction (i.e., prosocial values): benevolence and universalism [61]. Both values suggest that individuals are concerned with the welfare and interests of others, which basically means the transcendence of selfish interests. While benevolence enhances the welfare of in-group members, universalism enhances the welfare of all people beyond the in-group. This theory and approach to the measurement of values showed quite a high validity across many cultures and is used in such cross-cultural studies as the European Social Survey [62]. Some authors argue that such self-transcendent values should have a positive effect on a compliance with COVID-19 restrictive government policy though no empirical evidence has been shown [63]. The mechanism behind it is based on the prioritization of the interests of others at some personal cost. Therefore, we suggest that the ‘losses to others’ framing has a stronger effect on individuals with prosocial values, i.e. with stronger benevolence and universalism values.

  1. H2a: The effect of the object at risk, which focuses on losses to others, will have a stronger effect on the support for restrictive government policy compared to the personal losses framing for the individuals with strong prosocial attitudes.

The context of the study: COVID-19 in Russia

Two experiments were conducted in Russia. Experiment 1 was conducted during the so-called first wave of COVID-19. On March 28, 2020 –the day when Experiment 1 started–the confirmed number of cases in Russia was 1,264. Four people had died from COVID-19 up to that day. On April 24, 2020 when Experiment 1 was finished, a total of 68,622 cases were confirmed, and the COVID-19 death toll reached 615. On March 25, 2020 the President, Vladimir Putin, declared a non-working week in all Russian regions from March 28 to April 5, 2020 [64], which was later extended till April 30 [65], and then till May 11, 2020 [66]. He also entrusted regional authorities full powers to adopt restrictive measures depending on the number of cases in a region [65]. For instance, Moscow, Saint-Petersburg and a number of other Russian regions were placed under lockdown on March 30, 2020 by the decision of local authorities. People were allowed to go outside for medical care purposes, shopping for food and medication, and going to work if remote work was not an option. Besides the obligatory closure of schools, universities, gyms, swimming pools, shopping malls and hair salons, a number of regions including Moscow and Saint-Petersburg launched a digital pass system in April to allow residents to leave their homes for essential reasons as well as a smartphone app to monitor coronavirus patients’ movement in self-isolation [67].

Experiment 2 was conducted during the so-called second wave of COVID-19. As compared with the first wave, the situation with COVID-19 in Russia had largely changed. On November 13, 2020 when Experiment 2 started, the confirmed number of cases in Russia was 1,880,551 and the COVID-19 death toll reached 32,443. During Experiment 2 no lockdown was in place in Russia, as well as there were no non-working weeks. The system of digital passes, which was abolished in all Russian regions by June 9, was no longer used. However, in November 2020 a series of restrictive measures, which were lifted in summer, had been reintroduced across Russian regions to contain the spread of the coronavirus. Nevertheless, such measures as mandatory wearing of face masks and gloves, prohibition of mass events and mass gatherings, reduced capacity of theaters, cinemas, and restaurants, and maintaining social distance had never been lifted from the times of the first wave. The head of Russian Federal Service for Surveillance on Consumer Rights Protection and Human Wellbeing (Rospotrebnadzor), Anna Popova, called for restrictive measures in the regions with the highest numbers of the COVID-19 active cases; and Russian Prime Minister, Mikhail Mishustin, supported this idea [68]. Following this order, for instance, in Moscow, such measures as distance learning and remote work were introduced.

By November 2020 two vaccines against COVID-19 were officially registered in Russia. On 11 August 2020, Russian president Vladimir Putin announced the official approval of the Sputnik-V vaccine. Two month later, on 14 October 2020 another vaccine, EpiVacCorona, was officially registered. However, according to Russian public opinion polls, the vaccine did not make citizens less fearful of the virus. While about 57% of Russians were afraid of getting sick with COVID-19 during the first wave in March 2020, in October 2020 the percentage increased up to 64% [69].

Experiment 1

Participants

Current undergraduate or graduate students of the HSE University were eligible to take part in the experiment, except for students of Political Science and Sociology departments. The consent was obtained in a written form. We invited students via their group emails, which are used for communication between lecturers and students. As a result, according to AAPOR [70] standards, response rates cannot be computed. The number of completed interviews was 762 (N = 762). Completed interviews were determined as those which had more than 80% of the essential questions answered [70].

No course credits were provided for the survey participation and students signed a consent form in which they were told they were free to withdraw from participation at any time they wanted. As an incentive for survey completion, we offered participation in a lottery in which students could win the smart home device, Yandex. Station (the price of around $150 U.S.). The break-off rate was 46% (N = 760), about half of the breakoffs were at the introduction page. Some individuals who reported that they are not students of the HSE University were screened out (N = 118). On average, it took 23 minutes to complete the survey (M = 23.12, SD = 12.69). The baseline characteristics of the final sample are given in S1 File.

Although we used a convenience sample, the use of such samples does not appear to consistently generate false negatives, false positives, or inaccurate effect sizes [71]. Kühberger [34] also finds that the behavior of student participants does not significantly differ from the behavior of non-student participants. However, we cannot estimate conditional average treatment effects (CATEs) using a convenience sample of students for the following reason. We did not expect to observe large variation in values in a convenience sample; there is some evidence that the differences in prosocial values among students are relatively small [72]. Since Mullinix et al. [71] suggest that estimation of CATEs in the experimental studies is problematic when there is lack of variance of the moderator, especially among convenience samples of students, we did not test hypothesis H2a in Experiment 1.

Experimental design

The experimental design was approved by the Council of Peers at Ronald F. Inglehart Laboratory for Comparative Social Research (№ER-2020-01). It is confirmed that the proposed research project conforms to ethical standards in modern social sciences. We designed a web-based experiment with a 2 (risk severity: high vs. low) X 2 (object at risk: individual losses vs. losses to others) factorial design. The subjects were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions: (1) High-risk X Individual losses, (2) High-risk X Losses to others, (3) Low-risk X Individual losses, (4) High-risk X Losses to others. The randomization process was carried out at the individual level and was conducted within the flow of the survey. No restrictions were placed on randomization, and we did not employ blocking. To ensure the effectiveness of randomization we checked for the covariate balance and put these results in S2 File. The p-values of joint orthogonality tests indicate that the group differences are insignificant, except for the probability of COVID-19 infection in the manipulation of the risk severity factor (for the details of statistical analysis, see S2 File). The data collection started on March 28, 2020 and finished on April 24, 2020. As is recommended in Reporting Guidelines for Experimental Research, the CONSORT flow diagram is provided in S3 File.

Materials

The vignettes were structured as the set of rubrics with essentially similar content, yet different framing. The information on the pandemic, scale of the issue, probability of infection / recovery, medical treatment, long-term negative consequences for personal health (yes / no), incubation period, and advice on how to protect yourself / take care of others, were described in texts. The statistics and information in the vignettes were taken from official websites such as the World Health Organization, Russian Federal Service for Surveillance on Consumer Rights Protection and Human Wellbeing, and Russian media sources, in March-April 2020. The length of text varied from 214 to 265 words. Vignettes can be found in Table 1. To ensure that the independent variable had effectively been manipulated and the participants understood risk framing in the way we wanted them to, we used three manipulation checks. Further details of manipulation checks are given in S8 File.

Outcome measures and covariates

We had three dependent variables mainly used to ensure the robustness. Descriptive statistics of the dependent variable measures and pre-treatment covariates, which are described in S4 File, are given in Table 2. First, we asked the participants ‘How willing are you to sacrifice some of your rights if this helps prevent the spread of coronavirus in Russia?’, and measured their willingness to sacrifice rights on a 5-point scale (1 –‘not at all willing’; 5 –‘fully willing’).

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of dependent variables and pre-treatment covariates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t002

Second, we provided the participants with a list of 22 restrictive government policies which either had already been adopted by the Russian government or were being discussed in order to prevent the further spread of COVID-19. The complete list of security measures is given in S5 File. So, we asked the participants ‘To what extent do you support the following measures to prevent the spread of coronavirus in Russia’ and measured their level of support on a 5-point scale from 1 –‘do not support at all’ to 5 –‘fully support’. We then added up the points from respondents’ answers and used the resulting sum as the measure of participants’ support for restrictive government policy (Cronbach’s alpha, α = 0.91; [0.90; 0.92]).

Third, we asked the participants ‘To what extent do you support the introduction of criminal liability for violation of the quarantine in Russia?’ and measured their support for criminal liability on a 5-point scale (1 –‘do not support at all’; 5 –‘fully support’). This question appeared separately from other questions on restrictive government policies, since the intention of the Russian government to adopt it was harshly criticized in various media sources. Many people found this measure too severe and violating human rights, which resulted in public outrage on social media.

Results

Group means comparisons are summarized in Table 3 and Fig 1. As expected, we found statistically significant differences in the support for restrictive government policy (F(3, 762) = 4.68, p < 0.01) and support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (F(3, 762) = 2.72, p < 0.05) between four treatment conditions. Though we did not find evidence of significant differences in means of the willingness to sacrifice rights (F(3, 762) = 1.82, p = 0.142) between experimental conditions, the effect of risk framing was proven to be statistically significant in 2 of 3 measures for the dependent variable. Pairwise comparisons of experimental conditions and be found in S6 File.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

From left to right, the group means with 95% error bars in a) the willingness to sacrifice rights, b) the support for restrictive government policy, and c) the support for criminal liability for quarantine violation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.g001

thumbnail
Table 3. Group means of experimental conditions in a completely randomized 2x2 factorial design.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t003

To estimate average treatment effects of the main factors, we ran t-tests. We found strong support of H1 for all three measures of the dependent variable. The differences between means of participants’ willingness to sacrifice rights in high-risk (M = 3.41, SD = 0.92) and low-risk (M = 3.26, SD = 0.96) conditions are statistically significant (t(760) = 2.222, p < 0.05), so the ATE of risk severity on the willingness to sacrifice rights is 0.15 ([0.02; 0.29]) on a 5-point scale, Cohen’s d = 0.16. The same is true for the measure of support for restrictive government policy. The high-risk group mean (M = 94.72, SD = 11.97) and the low-risk group mean (M = 91.23, SD = 14) are significantly different (t(760) = 3.707, p < 0.001), meaning that the ATE of risk severity on the support for restrictive government policy is 3.49 ([1.64; 5.34]), Cohen’s d = 0.27. Regarding the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation, we also found a statistically significant difference (t(760) = 2.805, p < 0.01) between the high-risk (M = 3.00, SD = 1.31) and low-risk groups (M = 2.73, SD = 1.34), so the ATE is 0.27 ([0.08; 0.46]) on a 5-point scale, Cohen’s d = 0.20.

OLS regression models (see Table 4; see also S7 File) confirm the results of t-tests. Overall, these results prove that the high-risk framing has a stronger effect compared to the low-risk framing on willingness to sacrifice rights (β = 0.152, p < 0.05), the support for restrictive measures (β = 3.489, p < 0.01), and the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (β = 0.269, p < 0.01).

thumbnail
Table 4. OLS regression models estimates of main effects and interactions; pre-treatment covariates as controls are not included.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t004

In contrast, we did not find any evidence of H2. We found that the difference in willingness to sacrifice rights between the ‘individual losses’ (M = 3.36, SD = 0.95) and the ‘losses to others’ (M = 3.31, SD = 0.94) groups is not statistically significant (t(760) = 0.708, p = 0.479). There is also no evidence that the support for restrictive government policy is any different (t(760) = 0.080, p = 0.937) for those who were exposed to personal losses framing (M = 93.09, SD = 13.32) in comparison with those who were shown the object at risk, which indicates losses to others (M = 93.01, SD = 12.87). The support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation was not proved to be appreciably different (t(760) = 0.274, p = 0.784) between the ‘individual losses’ (M = 2.86, SD = 1.36) and the ‘losses to others’ (M = 2.88, SD = 1.30) conditions. Therefore, there are no grounds for accepting the second hypothesis. We conclude that the object at risk which indicates losses to others does not have a stronger effect on the support for restrictive government policy compared to the personal losses framing. OLS regression models (see Table 4; see also S7 File) confirm the results of t-tests; there are no statistically significant effects of ‘losses to others’ frame compared to ‘individual losses’ on willingness to sacrifice rights (β = -0.05 p = 0.465), the support for restrictive measures (β = -0.109, p = 0.908), and the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (β = 0.024, p = 0.804).

We also estimated interaction effects between main experimental factors. Models 2, 4, and 6 in Table 4 demonstrate that there are no statistically significant interaction effects between risk severity factor and object at risk, neither on willingness to sacrifice rights (β = -0.000, p = 0.999), nor on the support for restrictive government policy (β = 1.074, p = 0.569), nor on the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (β = 0.097, p = 0.612).

Interestingly enough, we found that the participants who were exposed to the ‘low-risk’ framing (M = 2.91, SD = 0.59) were less convinced of the credibility of the information (t(760) = -4.938, p < 0.001) than those who were in the ‘high-risk group’ (M = 3.1, SD = 0.49) (see S8 File for more details). We found that the CATEs (conditional average treatment effects) of risk severity framing increase for those who perceived the information as credible (see S8 File).

Experiment 2

Participants

To calculate the CATEs and effect sizes among the general population, we conducted the second study during the so-called ‘second wave of COVID-19’ using a volunteer online access panel, managed by Online Market Intelligence (OMI) in Russia. The panel has ISO 20252 certification. The consent was obtained in a written form. The number of completed interviews was 1,570. The participation rate [70] was 5%. The break-off rate was 9.6% (N = 187). Some respondents were screened out (N = 7) or started completing the survey when some quotas were full (N = 181). There were some nationally representative quotas on gender, age, federal district, and level of education.

On average, it took 29 minutes to complete the survey (M = 28.6; SD = 20.41). We have excluded from the analysis those respondents who showed low data quality, which is extremely quick reading of the vignettes and straight lining in grid questions [73]. Overall, we included 1,438 respondents (N = 1,438). About 55% were females. The mean age was 46 (M = 45.65, SD = 14.08). Other baseline characteristics of the final sample are given in S1 File.

Experimental design

The experimental design was approved by the Council of Peers at Ronald F. Inglehart Laboratory for Comparative Social Research (№ER-2020-02). It is confirmed that the proposed research project conforms to ethical standards in modern social sciences. The design was similar to Study 1, with a 2 (risk severity: high vs. low) X 2 (object at risk: individual losses vs. losses to others) factorial design. The data collection started on November 13, 2020 and finished on November 19, 2020. To ensure the effectiveness of randomization we checked for the covariate balance and put these results in S2 File. The p-values of joint orthogonality tests indicate that the group differences are insignificant, except for higher education, in the manipulation of the object at risk factor (for the details of statistical analysis, see S2 File). The CONSORT flow diagram is provided in S3 File.

Materials

Since this was the second wave of COVID-19 pandemic we changed the wording of vignettes. We excluded some basic information which was quite new at the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., about the pandemic overall, spread of the issue, incubation period), but was common knowledge at the beginning of the second wave—10 months after the pandemic was declared. As a result, the number of words has been substantially decreased compared to Experiment 1. The length of text varied from 79 to 117 words. Moreover, to the moment of the second Experiment, the statistics and media coverage had changed since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic. Previous research has shown that the proportion of news frames has changed during different time periods depending on if it was pre-crisis, lockdown or recovery period of COVID-19 pandemic [7476]. Following the idea of agenda-setting effects [77] we have updated some relevant and excluded some outdated information in order to make vignettes more habitual for respondents. We described the scale of the issue / recovery, medical treatment, and the advice on how to protect yourself / take care of others. The statistics and information in the vignettes were taken from Russian Federal Service for Surveillance on Consumer Rights Protection and Human Wellbeing, and Russian media sources in November 2020. Vignettes can be found in Table 5. We used the same manipulation checks. Further details of manipulation checks are given in S8 File.

Outcome measures and covariates

We had the same three dependent variables used in Experiment 1. Descriptive statistics of the dependent variable measures and pre-treatment covariates, which are described in S4 File, used in the second experiment are given in Table 6. However, we have slightly changed the list of restrictive government policies to prevent the further spread of COVID-19, since most of the measures used in the first study were no longer relevant to the context. Similar to the first experiment, we added up the points from respondents’ answers and used the resulting sum as the measure of participants’ support for restrictive government policy (Cronbach’s alpha, α = 0.91; [0.90; 0.92]). The list of 10 policies included in Experiment 2 is given in S5 File. We used the 21-item short version of Scwartz’s portrait values questionnaire [60] to measure prosocial values and test H2a. The focus of our experimental study was the self-transcendence direction, in particular, benevolence and universalism. The value scores were centered to measure the priority given to each of the value types as suggested by Schwartz [78].

thumbnail
Table 6. Descriptive statistics of dependent variables and pre-treatment covariates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t006

Results

Group means comparisons are summarized in Table 7 and Fig 2. ANOVA showed statistically significant differences between four treatment conditions in the willingness to sacrifice rights (F(3, 1434) = 2.94, p < 0.05, see Table 7, Fig 2), but no differences in the support for restrictive government policies (F(3, 1434) = 0.61, p = 0.608) and criminal liability for quarantine violation (F(3, 1434) = 0.54, p = 0.655). Hence, the effect of risk framing was proven to be statistically significant in 1 of 3 measures of the dependent variable. Pairwise comparisons of experimental conditions and be found in S6 File.

thumbnail
Fig 2.

From left to right, the group means with 95% error bars in a) the willingness to sacrifice rights, b) the support for restrictive government policy, and c) the support for criminal liability for quarantine violation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.g002

thumbnail
Table 7. Group means of experimental conditions in a completely randomized 2x2 factorial design.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t007

Compared to Experiment 1, in Experiment 2 we found support of H1 for one measure of the dependent variable only. T-tests showed significant differences in the willingness to sacrifice rights between the low-risk (M = 2.88, SD = 1.13) and high-risk (M = 3.04, SD = 1.15) conditions (t(1436) = -2.631, p < 0.01). Hence, the ATE of risk severity on the willingness to sacrifice rights is 0.16 ([0.04; 0.28]) on a 5-point scale, Cohen’s d = 0.14. However, no other significant differences—either in the support for restrictive government policies (t(1436) = -1.317, p = 0.188) or in the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (t(1436) = -0.329, p = 0.742)—were found in the two other measures.

Similarly to Experiment 1 no statistically significant differences—not in the willingness to sacrifice rights (t(1436) = -0.809, p = 0.418), not in the support for restrictive government policies (t(1436) = 0.273, p = 0.785), not in the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (t(1436) = -0.540, p = 0.589)—were found between ‘individual losses’ and ‘losses to others’ conditions. So, again there are no grounds for accepting the second hypothesis. We conclude that the object at risk which indicates losses to others does not have a stronger effect on the support for restrictive government policy compared to the personal losses framing.

OLS regression models (see Table 8; see also S7 File) confirm the results of t-tests; the influence of risk severity on the willingness to sacrifice rights is the only main effect which is statistically significant (β = 0.157, p < 0.01). We also estimated interaction effects between main experimental factors. Models 2, 4, and 6 in Table 8 demonstrate that there are no statistically significant interaction effects between risk severity factor and object at risk, neither on willingness to sacrifice rights (β = 0.138, p = 0.250), nor on the support for restrictive government policy (β = -0.085, p = 0.932), nor on the support for criminal liability for the quarantine violation (β = -0.151, p = 0.268).

thumbnail
Table 8. OLS regression models estimates of main effects and interactions; pre-treatment covariates as controls are not included.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t008

Now we proceed with the empirical test of the hypothesis H2a to see if the effect of the ’object at risk’ framing on the support for restrictive government policy is different for the individuals with strong prosocial attitudes. In OLS models (see Table 9) we found a statistically significant interaction effect of the ‘losses to others’ framing and the Schwartz’s value ‘benevolence’, which is defined by the preservation and strengthening of others’ wellbeing [74], in willingness to sacrifice rights (β = 0.158, p < 0.05, Table 9). At 10% significance level, we also found that there is a statistically significant interaction effect of the ‘losses to others’ framing and the Schwartz’s value ‘universalism’—understanding, appreciation, tolerance, and protection for the welfare of all people—on support for restrictive government policies (β = 1.374, p < 0.1). There were no statistically significant moderating effects of either ‘benevolence’ (β = 0.031, p = 0.576) or ‘universalism’ (β = -0.09, p = 0.392) on the support for criminal liability for quarantine violation.

thumbnail
Table 9. OLS regression models estimates of CATEs using values as moderators; pre-treatment covariates as controls are not included.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t009

Finally, similar to the results of Experiment 1, we found that the participants who were exposed to the ‘low-risk’ framing (M = 2.76, SD = 0.65) were less convinced of the credibility of the information (t(1436) = -4.936, p < 0.001) than those who were in the ‘high-risk group’ (M = 2.92, SD = 0.64); see S8 File for more details. We found that the CATEs of risk severity framing are also higher for those who perceive the treatment information as credible (see S8 File). In other words, Experiment 2 demonstrates empirical evidence of H1 for all three measures of attitudes towards restrictive government policy, but this effect of high vs. low risks is observed only for those who perceive information as credible (see main effects and interactions in Table 10). On the contrary, in Experiment 1 we found that the framing effect was consistent among all respondents, though it was also conditioned by perceived information credibility.

thumbnail
Table 10. Conditional average treatment effects of high-risk vs. low-risk groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.t010

Discussion

There are three major findings in this study. First, focusing on higher risks has a positive effect on the support for the government restrictive policy. We found some evidence for the first hypothesis H1, i.e., that high-risk framing caused a higher willingness to sacrifice rights, support for government restrictive measures, and criminal liability. That is in line with the literature on both risk perception of infections and the perception of societal risks which can be a threat to society and social order overall [2426]. This is also in consistency with health-related behavior theories (e.g., protection motivation theory, health belief model) and the research results which show that those who evaluate health risks as high are more willing to comply with self-protective measures [79]. Our finding also mirrors earlier studies of pandemic impact on social attitudes and behavior. The Ebola outbreak, for instance, was found to produce a stronger support for restrictive policies [80].

At the same time, it should be noted that in terms of effect sizes the differences were small and not always statistically significant. Cohen’s d was up to 0.27 in Experiment 1 and up to 0.16 in Experiment 2. Overall, the participants in both experiments strongly approved a number of restrictive policies. Small effect sizes might result from the fact that the overall level of anxiety and emotional fear (‘affect heuristic’ [21]) of COVID-19 is very high. Significantly more respondents showed a lower level of credibility to the vignettes in the low-risk condition in both experiments. The effect sizes were higher among those who found the information in the vignettes more credible: Cohen’s d was up to 0.34 in Experiment 1 and up to 0.19 in Experiment 2. This result was anticipated, since previous research has shown that individuals seem to perceive lower risk estimates as less credible and consider such information as less trustworthy, especially in health communication [81]. In addition, our result is consistent with the so-called negativity bias effect in processing information [82]. The high-risk condition can be also called a strong frame as it is more compelling for people [33]. That is also elicited by the substantial overestimation of the COVID-19 mortality and infection rates by the respondents in both experiments. The mean mortality rate was evaluated as 11% in Experiment 1 and 35% in Experiment 2. The mean infection rate was evaluated as 31% in Experiment 1 and 56% in Experiment 2. Due to the availability heuristic, when the information about the number of deaths and newly infected people is reported on a daily basis, this increased both infection and mortality rates. This is in accordance with the literature that shows an increase in risk evaluation if the issue is salient for people and if mass media reports the risks on a regular basis [23]. Indeed, the higher individuals evaluated the infection and fatality rates, the more they supported government restrictive policy.

Second, focusing on the losses for others did not produce a stronger support for the restrictive policy compared to focusing on personal losses. This is in line with the papers which found no difference between self-focused and prosocial framing during the COVID-19 pandemic [47, 48, 53, 54]. We found no evidence that prosocial responsibility acts like a Trojan horse for willingness to sacrifice rights and an acceptance of privacy violation [83]. This effect is in line with the idea that people are less inclined to sacrifice for others in a state of uncertainty [84], but contradicts the opposing point of view that people tend to sacrifice if they are exposed to worst-case scenarios [85]. Alongside health and death issues, pandemics might also impact a conservative shift and security demands for oneself [86].

Third, though focusing on the losses for others did not produce a stronger support for restrictive policy, we found a positive moderation effect of such prosocial values as universalism and benevolence. We found that those with prosocial values had a stronger positive effect in the “losses for others” frame and were more willing to support restrictive policy when others were included. This is in line with the prediction of Wolf et al. paper though they showed no empirical evidence of the claim [63]. It seems that proclaiming the importance of being ‘supportive’ and ‘careful’ towards others by WHO, during the pandemic, may increase the support for restrictive policy by some individuals whose values are activated when others are included as those who can be harmed. This is in accordance with the literature that links prosocial values with prosocial risk taking in which there may be a risk for others [45]. This is also in line with the literature on the effect of frames which showed a moderator effect of values. Chong and Druckman [33] emphasize that people’s preferences are a function of personal values and the strength of competing frames on the issue. As a result, some frames should be in consistency with personal values to have an effect on attitudes in a competitive environment.

The effects found in our experimental study reveal both positive and negative aspects in risk communication during the pandemic, which may have a great and long-term impact on trust, attitudes, and behavior. The major positive aspect is the efficiency of risk communication for the awareness of risks and its recognition. Higher perceived risks result in a more risk-averse behavior and higher willingness to undertake protective measures [87].

However, at the same time there might be some negative consequences of this risk communication. First, higher risk perception can undermine social trust, political trust, and trust in scientific experts, if protective measures bring negative consequences to the population or the government is not able to handle the issue [88]. According to the protection motivation theory and health belief model, the perceived effectiveness of recommended measures has an effect on the willingness to follow precautionary actions. If perceived effectiveness is quite low, this can bring a decrease of political trust in government similar to what has happened in Europe after the H1N1 pandemic in 2009 [89]. It was also shown that forced social distance and the social changes caused by the Spanish flu had a long-term negative effect on social trust [90]. Second, a greater loyalty to discretionary power of the executives may seem similar to Slovic’s outlook on the consequences of nuclear risks for democracy [91]. While risk communication produces a high-risk perception of COVID-19, it can be efficient in promoting anti-democratic ideas and messages or the denigration of minorities. Similarly, while including others as those who can be harmed, it can be efficient in promoting support for anti-democratic policies among those who have prosocial values. Thus, there might be growing support for restrictive policy worldwide.

The recent papers published in the Lancet, which argue that ‘we need to pay attention to how authoritarian forces shape our frame of mind’ during the pandemic [92], are a wake-up call for risk communication research. Nevertheless, risk framing and its effects on support for restrictive government policy is critical not just to understand the coercive apparatus of authoritarian government, but to evaluate countries’ ability to conduct risk communication in shaping people’s risk perceptions and instructing them to adopt certain preventive measures, such as social distancing and self-isolation. Overall, we stress the importance of the further exploration of risk communication during the COVID-19 pandemic in different cultures and different population groups, as this has tremendous long-term consequences for all countries.

There are several limitations in this study. First, we cannot make generalizations about Russia since a non-probability sample was used in both experiments. Second, we cannot fully extrapolate our findings to other countries. Both findings can be culturally specific [37]. Russia is a developing country with a certain socio-cultural background and cultural values. Cross-cultural studies should be conducted to explore the differences in risk perception, and the effect of different risk framing on risk perception and the support for restrictive policy. Third, the wording of the vignettes was different in our experiments which make our conclusion about the effect sizes limited, since the change in effect sizes can be due to different wording, but also due to different time points and different survey populations. The effect sizes can also vary in further experiments depending on COVID-19 risk dynamics and media coverage of COVID-19.

In spite of the limitations, our findings confirm the great political importance of risk communication and risk literacy in the time of the pandemic. Gigerenzer [93] shows that risk education and the improvement of risk literacy with regards to staying healthy, should be the focus of institutions, as acting politicians can make ill-advised decisions in the time of crises and pandemics, when long-term consequences of protective measures cannot be examined.

Supporting information

S1 File. Eligibility and baseline sample characteristics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.s001

(DOCX)

S4 File. Selection and measurement of pre-treatment covariates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.s004

(DOCX)

S5 File. DV measurement: support for restrictive government policy.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.s005

(DOCX)

S9 File. References used in supporting information.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258132.s009

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

This paper is dedicated to our mothers. We thank them for all their endless love they gave to us. We miss you a lot. May you all rest in peace.

References

  1. 1. Ang YY. When COVID-19 Meets centralized, personalized power. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4:445–447. pmid:32273583
  2. 2. Cyranoski D. What China’s coronavirus response can teach the rest of the world. Nature. 2020;579(7800):479–480. pmid:32203360
  3. 3. Kupferschmidt K, Cohen J. China’s aggressive measures have slowed the coronavirus. they may not work in other countries. Science. 2020.
  4. 4. Connolly P. Is softly, softly Sweden heading for catastrophe? In: The Daily Mail. 2020 March 31 [cited 29 April 2020]. Available from: https://www.dailymail.co.uk/debate/article-8173691/Is-softly-softly-Sweden-heading-catastrophe.html.
  5. 5. International Gallup. Snap poll on Cov19 in 28 countries by Gallup International Association. In: Gallup International [Internet]. March 2020 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: https://www.gallup-international.com/fileadmin/user_upload/surveys/2020/GIA_SnapPoll_2020_COVID_Tables_final.pdf.
  6. 6. International Gallup. Almost a year with pandemic: People around globe still mobilized against the threat, with hope for vaccines. Yet potential problems become more significant. In: Gallup International [Internet]. 2021 January 22 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: https://www.gallup-international.bg/en/44307/a-year-of-global-coronavirus-pandemic-global-survey-vaccines-attitudes.
  7. 7. Karwowski M, Kowal M, Groyecka A, Białek M, Lebuda I, Sorokowska A, et al. (2020). When in danger, turn right: COVID-19 threat promotes social conservatism and right-wing presidential candidates. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. 2020 [cited January 2021]. Available from: https://psyarxiv.com/pjfhs.
  8. 8. Young ME, King N, Harper S, Humphreys KR. The influence of popular media on perceptions of personal and population risk in possible disease outbreaks. Health Risk Soc. 2013;15(1):103−114.
  9. 9. Ogbodo JN, Onwe EC, Chukwu J, Nwasum CH, Nwakpu ES, Nwankwo SU, et al. Communicating health crisis: a content analysis of global media framing of COVID-19. Health Promot Perspect. 2020;10(3):257–269. pmid:32802763
  10. 10. Gantiva C, Jiménez-Leal W, Urriago-Rayo J. Framing messages to deal with the COVID-19 crisis: The role of loss/gain frames and content. Front Psychol. 2021;12:568212. pmid:33584464
  11. 11. Hameleers M. Prospect theory in times of a pandemic: the effects of gain versus loss framing on policy preferences and emotional responses during the 2020 coronavirus outbreak–evidence from the US and the Netherlands. Mass Commun Soc. 2021;24(4):479–499.
  12. 12. Olmastroni F., Guidi M., Martini S., Isernia P. Framing effects on the COVID-19 see-saw. Swiss Political Science Review. 2021.
  13. 13. Sanders M, Stockdale E, Hume S, John P. Loss aversion fails to replicate in the coronavirus pandemic: Evidence from an online experiment. Econ Lett. 2021;199:109433. pmid:32836579
  14. 14. Steffen J., Cheng J. The Influence of gain-loss framing and its interaction with political ideology on social distancing and mask wearing compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic. 2021 [cited March 28 2021]. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. Available from: pmid:34341651
  15. 15. Xu P, Cheng J. Individual differences in social distancing and mask-wearing in the pandemic of COVID-19: The role of need for cognition, self-control and risk attitude. Pers Individ Dif. 2021;175:110706. pmid:33551529
  16. 16. Kenis P, Schol LG, Kraaij‐Dirkzwager MM, Timen A. Appropriate governance responses to infectious disease threats: Developing working hypotheses. Risk, Hazards & Crisis in Public Policy. 2019;10(3):275–293.
  17. 17. Frewer LJ. The public and effective risk communication. Toxicology Letters. 2004;149(1–3):391–397. pmid:15093286
  18. 18. WHO: World Health Organization (2020). Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) advice for the public. In: WHO [Internet]. Update: 2020 April 29 [cited 29 April 2020]. Available from: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/advice-for-public.
  19. 19. Slovic P. Perception of risk. Science. 1987;236(4799):280–285. pmid:3563507
  20. 20. Slovic P, Finucane M, Peters E, MacGregor D. Rational actors or rational fools: implications of affect heuristic for behavioral economics. J Socio Econ. 2002;31(4):338–339.
  21. 21. Finucane ML, Alhakami AS, Slovic P, Johnson SM. The affect heuristic in the judgement of risks and benefits. J Behav Decis Mak. 2000;13(1):1–17.
  22. 22. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185(4157):1124–1131. pmid:17835457
  23. 23. Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S. Facts and fears: Understanding perceived risk. In: Schwing RC, Albers WA Jr, editors. Societal risk assessment: How safe is safe enough? New York: Plenum Press; 1980; pp.181–214.
  24. 24. Doty RM, Peterson B, Winter DG. Threat and authoritarianism in the United States, 1978–1987. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61(4):629–640. pmid:1960654
  25. 25. Feldman S, Stenner K. Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Polit Psychol. 1997;18(4):741–770.
  26. 26. Jugert P, Duckitt J. A motivational model of authoritarianism: Integrating personal and situational determinants. Polit Psychol. 2009;30(5):693–719.
  27. 27. Viscusi WK, Zeckhauser R. Sacrificing civil liberties to reduce terrorism risks. J Risk Uncertain. 2003;26(2):99–120.
  28. 28. Prati G, Pietrantoni L. Knowledge, risk perceptions, and xenophobic attitudes: evidence from Italy during the Ebola outbreak. Risk Anal. 2016;36(10):2000–2010. pmid:26916803
  29. 29. Duckitt J, Fisher K. The impact of social threat on world view and ideological attitudes. Polit Psychol. 2003;24(1):199–222.
  30. 30. Davis DW, Silver BD. Civil liberties vs. security: Public opinion in the context of the terrorist attacks on America. Am J Pol Sci. 2004;48(1):28–46.
  31. 31. Carlin RE, Love GJ, Zechmeister EJ. Natural disaster and democratic legitimacy: the public opinion consequences of Chile’s 2010 earthquake and tsunami. Polit Res Q. 2014;67(1):3–15.
  32. 32. Fritsche I, Cohrs JC, Kessler T, Bauer J. Global warming is breeding social conflict: the subtle impact of climate change threat on authoritarian tendencies. J Environ Psychol. 2012;32(1):1–10.
  33. 33. Chong D, Druckman JN. Framing theory. Annu Rev Polit Sci. 2007;10:103–126.
  34. 34. Kühberger A. The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1998;75(1):23–55. pmid:9719656
  35. 35. Entman R. Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. J Commun. 1993;43(4):51–58.
  36. 36. Boholm Å, Corvallec H. A relational theory of risk. J Risk Res. 2011;14(2):175–190.
  37. 37. Brown P. Studying COVID-19 in light of critical approaches to risk and uncertainty: research pathways, conceptual tools, and some magic from Mary Douglas. Health Risk Soc. 2020; 22(1):1–14.
  38. 38. Bolton GE, Ockenfels A, Stauf J. Social responsibility promotes conservative risk behavior. Eur Econ Rev. 2015;74:109–127.
  39. 39. Rothman AJ, Salovey P, Turvey C, Fishkin SA. Attributions of responsibility and persuasion: increasing mammography utilization among women over 40 with an internally oriented message. Health Psychol. 1993;12(1):39–47. pmid:8462498
  40. 40. Atanasov P. Risk preferences in choices for self and others: Meta analysis and research directions. SSRN [Preprint]. 2015 [cited 19 May 2021]. Available from: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1682569
  41. 41. Betsch C, Böhm R, Korn L. Inviting free-riders or appealing to prosocial behavior? Game-theoretical reflections on communicating herd immunity in vaccine advocacy. Health Psychol. 2013;32(9):978–985. pmid:24001248
  42. 42. Vietri JT, Li M, Galvani AP, Chapman GB. Vaccinating to help ourselves and others. Med Decis Making. 2012;32(3):447–458. pmid:22127841
  43. 43. Heinrich T, Mayrhofer T. Higher-order risk preferences in social settings: An experimental analysis. Exp Econ. 2018;21(2):434–456. pmid:29720890
  44. 44. Pahlke J, Strasser S, Vieider FM. Responsibility effects in decision making under risk. J Risk Uncertain. 2015;51:125–146.
  45. 45. Do KT, Moreira JF, Telzer EH. But is helping you worth the risk? Defining prosocial risk taking in adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;25:260–271. pmid:28063823
  46. 46. Douglas M. Purity and danger: An analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul; 1966.
  47. 47. Heffner J, Vives ML, FeldmanHall O. Emotional responses to prosocial messages increase willingness to self-isolate during the COVID-19 pandemic. Pers Individ Dif. 2020;170:110420. pmid:33082614
  48. 48. Jordan J, Yoeli E, Rand D. Don’t get it or don’t spread it? Comparing self-interested versus prosocial motivations for COVID-19 prevention behaviors. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. 2020 [cited 25 May 2021]. Available from:
  49. 49. Ceylan M, Hayran C. Message framing effects on individuals’ social distancing and helping behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychol. 2021;12:579164. pmid:33828501
  50. 50. Lunn PD, Timmons S, Barjaková M, Belton CA, Julienne H, Lavin C. Motivating social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: An online experiment. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020;265:113478. pmid:33162198
  51. 51. Pfattheicher S, Nockur L, Böhm R, Sassenrath C, Petersen MB. The emotional path to action: Empathy promotes physical distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol Sci. 2020;31(11):1363–1373. pmid:32993455
  52. 52. Capraro V, Barcelo H. The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. J Behav Econ Pol. 2020;4(S2):45–55.
  53. 53. Banker S, Park J. Evaluating prosocial COVID-19 messaging frames: Evidence from a field study on Facebook. Judgm Decis Mak. 2020;15(6):1037–1043.
  54. 54. Bilancini E, Boncinelli L, Capraro V, Celadin T, Di Paolo R. The effect of norm-based messages on reading and understanding COVID-19 pandemic response governmental rules. J Behav Econ Pol. 2020;4(S):45–55.
  55. 55. Schwartz SH. Normative influences on altruism. In: Berkowitz L., editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press. 1977;10:221–279
  56. 56. Schwartz SH, Howard J. Internalized values as motivators of altruism. In: Staub E, Bar-Tal D, Karylowski J, Reykowski J, editors. Development and maintenance of prosocial behavior. New York: Plenum; 1984; pp.229–255.
  57. 57. Hilbig BE, Glöckner A, Zettler I. Personality and prosocial behavior: Linking basic traits and social value orientations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2014;107(3):529–539. pmid:25019254
  58. 58. Christner N, Sticker RM, Söldner L, Mammen M, Paulus M. (2020). Prevention for oneself or others? Psychological and social factors that explain social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Health Psychol. 2020. Online first. pmid:33302730
  59. 59. Petrocchi S, Bernardi S, Malacrida R, Traber R, Gabutti L, Grignoli N. Affective empathy predicts self-isolation behaviour acceptance during coronavirus risk exposure. Sci Rep. 2021;11:10153. pmid:33980946
  60. 60. Schmitt MJ, Schwartz S, Steyer R, Schmitt T. Measurement models for the Schwartz values. Eur J Psychol Assess. 1993;9(2):107–121.
  61. 61. Schwartz SH. An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture. 2012;2(1).
  62. 62. Bilsky W, Janik M, Schwartz S. The structural organization of human values—Evidence from three rounds of the European Social Survey (ESS). J Cross Cult Psychol. 2011;42(5):759–776.
  63. 63. Wolf L, Haddock G, Manstead A, Maio G. The importance of (shared) human values for containing the COVID-19 pandemic. Br J Soc Psychol. 2020;59:618–627. pmid:32572981
  64. 64. Kremlin. Address to the Nation. In: Kremlin [Internet]. 2020a March 25 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: http://en.kremlin.ru/events/president/news/63061.
  65. 65. Kremlin. Address to the Nation. In: Kremlin [Internet]. 2020b April 2 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: http://en.kremlin.ru/events/president/news/63133.
  66. 66. Kremlin. Meeting with regional heads on countering the spread of the coronavirus. In: Kremlin [Internet]. 2020c April 28 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: http://en.kremlin.ru/events/president/news/63288.
  67. 67. Rudnitsky J, Khrennikov I. Moscow tightens lockdown with digital permits as virus spreads. In: Bloomberg [Internet]. 2020 April 10 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2020-04-10/moscow-tightens-lockdown-with-permit-system-as-virus-spreads.
  68. 68. Russian Government. Meeting of the Government Coordination Council to control the incidence of novel coronavirus infection in the Russian Federation. In: Russian Government [Internet]. 2020 9 November [cited 26 June 2021]. Available from: http://government.ru/en/news/40801.
  69. 69. Levada-center. Coronavirus: Fears and measures. In: Levada-center [Internet]. 2020 November 2 [cited 28 May 2021]. Available from: https://www.levada.ru/2020/11/02/koronavirus-strahi-i-mery.
  70. 70. AAPOR: The American Association for Public Opinion Research. Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys. 9th edn. Amsterdam: AAPOR; 2016
  71. 71. Mullinix KJ, Leeper TJ, Druckman JN, Freese J. The generalizability of survey experiments. J. Exp. Political Sci. 2015;2(2):109–138.
  72. 72. Hofmann-Towfigh N. Do students’ values change in different types of schools? J Moral Educ. 2007;36(4):453–473.
  73. 73. Zhang C, Conrad F. Speeding in web surveys: The tendency to answer very fast and its association with straightlining. Surv Res Methods. 2014;8(2):127−135.
  74. 74. Gozzi N, Tizzani M, Starnini M, Ciulla F, Paolotti D, Panisson A, et al. Collective response to the media coverage of COVID-19 pandemic on Reddit and Wikipedia. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(10):e21597. pmid:32960775
  75. 75. Poirier W, Ouellet C, Rancourt M-A, Béchard J, Dufresne Y. (Un)covering the COVID-19 pandemic: framing analysis of the crisis in Canada. Canadian Journal of Political Science. 2020;53:365–371.
  76. 76. Yu J, Lu Y, Muñoz-Justicia J. Analyzing Spanish news frames on Twitter during COVID-19—a network study of El País and El Mundo. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(15):5414. pmid:32731359
  77. 77. McCombs M, Shaw D. The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public Opin. Q. 1972;36(2):176–187.
  78. 78. Schwartz SH. Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1992;25:1–65.
  79. 79. de Bruin WB, Saw H-W, Goldman DP. Political polarization in US residents’ COVID-19 risk perceptions, policy preferences, and protective behaviors. J Risk Uncertain. 2020;61:177–194. pmid:33223612
  80. 80. Stürmer S, Rohmann A, Mazziotta A, Siem B, Barbarino M-L. Fear of infection or justification of social exclusion? The Symbolic exploitation of the Ebola epidemic. Polit Psychol. 2017;38:499–513.
  81. 81. Trumbo CW, McComas KA. The function of credibility in information processing for risk perception. Risk Anal. 2003;23(2):343–353, pmid:12731818
  82. 82. Peeters G, Czapinski J. Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 1990;1:33–60.
  83. 83. Kokkoris MD, Kamleitner B. Would you sacrifice your privacy to protect public health? Prosocial responsibility in a pandemic paves the way for digital surveillance. Front Psychol. 2020;11:578618. pmid:33071918
  84. 84. Norton M, Weber R. Motivated bayesians: feeling moral while acting egoistically. J Econ Perspect. 2016;30(3):189–212.
  85. 85. Van Bavel JJ, Baicker K, Boggio PS, Carparo V, Cichocka A., Cikara M, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4:460–471. pmid:32355299
  86. 86. Burke BL, Kosloff S, Landau MJ. Death goes to polls: A meta-analysis of mortality salience effects on political attitudes. Polit Psychol. 2013;34(2):183–200.
  87. 87. Jones J, Salathe M. Early assessment of anxiety and behavioural response to novel swine-origin influenza A(H1N1). PLOS ONE. 2009;4:e8032. pmid:19997505
  88. 88. Poortinga W, Bickerstaff K, Langford I, Niewohner J, Pidgeon N. The British 2001 foot and mouth crisis: A comparative study of public risk perceptions, trust and beliefs about government policy in two communities. J Risk Res. 2004;7(1):73–90.
  89. 89. Bangerter A, Krings F, Mouton A, Gilles I, Green E, Clémence A. Longitudinal investigation of public trust in institutions relative to the 2009 H1N1 pandemic in Switzerland. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e49806 pmid:23185444
  90. 90. Aassve A, Alfani G, Gandolfi F, Le Moglie M. Epidemics and trust: the case of the Spanish flu. Health Econ. 2021;30(4):840–857. pmid:33554412
  91. 91. Slovic P. Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk Anal. 1993;13(6):675–682.
  92. 92. Pericàs JM. Authoritarianism and the threat of infectious diseases. Lancet. 2020;395(10230):1111–1112. pmid:32247388
  93. 93. Gigerenzer G. On the supposed evidence for libertarian paternalism. Rev Philos Psychol. 2015;6(3):361–383. pmid:26213590