Figures
Abstract
Bottlenose dolphins are a behaviorally complex, social species that display a variety of social behaviors. Because of this, it is important for zoological facilities to strive to ensure animals display species-appropriate levels of social behavior. The current study is part of the multi-institutional study entitled “Towards understanding the welfare of cetaceans in zoos and aquariums” commonly referred to as the Cetacean Welfare Study. All participating facilities were accredited by the Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums and/or the Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Behavioral data were collected on 47 bottlenose dolphins representing two subspecies, Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops aduncus, at 25 facilities. The social behaviors of group related activity (group active) as well as interacting with conspecifics (interact with conspecific) were examined for their relationships to both animal management factors and habitat characteristics. The behavioral state of group active and the rate of interact with conspecific were both positively related to the frequency of receiving new forms of environmental enrichment. Both were inversely related to the random scheduling of environmental enrichment. Additional results suggested interact with conspecific was inversely related with daytime spatial experience and that males displayed group active more than females. Overall, the results suggested that animal management techniques such as the type and timing of enrichment may be more important to enhance social behavior than habitat characteristics or the size of the habitat. Information gained from this study can help facilities with bottlenose dolphins manage their enrichment programs in relation to social behaviors.
Citation: Miller LJ, Lauderdale LK, Mellen JD, Walsh MT, Granger DA (2021) Assessment of animal management and habitat characteristics associated with social behavior in bottlenose dolphins across zoological facilities. PLoS ONE 16(8): e0253732. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253732
Editor: Fabienne Delfour, Laboratoire d’Ethologie Experimentale et Comparee, FRANCE
Received: June 26, 2020; Accepted: June 12, 2021; Published: August 30, 2021
Copyright: © 2021 Miller et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: In order to protect the identification of the facilities and animals included in this study, findings require data access restrictions at the individual-level. All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: The work was funded by a National Leadership Grant (MG-30-17-0006-17) from the Institute of Museum and Library Services (www.imls.gov) to the Chicago Zoological Society. Additional financial support was provided to the Chicago Zoological Society for the present work by the Indianapolis Zoo, The Seas® Epcot® Walt Disney World® Resort, Dolphin Island – Resorts World Sentosa, Texas State Aquarium, Loro Parque and Loro Parque Fundación, SeaWorld Busch Gardens Conservation Fund and the Chicago Zoological Society Women’s Board. Chicago Zoological Society provided financial support in the form of a salary for LM, University of California Irvine provided financial support for DG salary and University of Florida provided financial support for MW salary through Clearwater Marine Aquarium and Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission state funding. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the 'author contributions' section. The Indianapolis Zoo, The Seas® Epcot® Walt Disney World® Resort, Dolphin Island – Resorts World Sentosa, Texas State Aquarium, Loro Parque, and SeaWorld Parks also contributed to data collection as each of these facilities, as well as animals at the facilities, were involved in the study. The funders had no other role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have read the journal’s policy, and the authors of the study have the following competing interests to declare: LM and LL are affiliated with the Chicago Zoological Society – Brookfield Zoo (an AZA and AMMPA accredited zoo), and Chicago Zoological Society provides a salary for LM. Before the study, JM was previously affiliated with Disney’s Animal Kingdom (an AZA accredited zoo). The Seas® Epcot® Walt Disney World® Resort, Dolphin Island – Resorts World Sentosa, and Loro Parque and Loro Parque Fundación provided funding to the Chicago Zoological Society for this study. This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials. There are no patents, products in development or marketed products associated with this research to declare.
Introduction
Common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) are a behaviorally complex, social species that live in a fission-fusion society. These highly dynamic societies can change in both composition and structure throughout the day [1]. While typically found in groups ranging from a few to a dozen in coastal and estuarine habitats, larger groups can be found in open water [1–3]. Given their fission-fusion society, it is not surprising that they can spend anywhere between 4% and 31% of their time socializing [4–8]. In two of the most studied populations, Sarasota Bay Florida and Shark Bay Australia, females typically socialize with other females in their groups [9–11]. Male bottlenose dolphins on the other hand usually form strong bonds with other males in order to form an alliance [9,10,12]. These social behaviors and bonds appear to be important to these species.
In the wild there are many benefits to group living for bottlenose dolphins including protection from predators and cooperative hunting [13,14]. However, dolphins under professional care receive a readily available high-quality diet and are not exposed to potential predators but likely receive other benefits from group living [15]. Research has shown that strong social bonds can have adaptive consequences. For example, humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) with stable and strong relationships have the highest reproductive success [16]. For many primate species, strong social bonds are associated with males siring more offspring [17], and females having higher reproductive success and longer lifespans [18–20]. Similarly, strong bonds between bottlenose dolphins also result in higher reproductive success [21,22]. It is likely that these social relationships buffer the deleterious effects of stress and thus provide adaptive value. Determining animal care and management factors that lead to positive social relationships may enhance the welfare of animals under professional care [23,24].
The social behavior of bottlenose dolphins under professional care has many similarities to the behavior of their wild counterparts [25]. For example, many of the affiliative behaviors such as pectoral fin touching are often seen under multiple populations in professional care [25,26]. However, some differences exist between wild and professionally managed dolphins due to habitat and animal management characteristics impacting social behavior. One study demonstrated that by changing habitat from a closed environment to an open “ocean pen” environment there was a decrease in social interaction for bottlenose dolphins [27]. The total size of the open environment was approximately five times greater than the capacity of the closed environment which were divided into three versus four areas respectively. The resulting change in size could account for the decrease observed, or the change could be due to dolphins in the open environment spending more time exploring the environment that would likely have other forms of wildlife.
Alternatively, participation in interaction programs resulted in an increase in common dolphins (Delphinus delphis) touching in a non-aggressive manner [28]. Additional studies have examined how interaction programs impacted the social behavior of bottlenose dolphins finding no differences comparing before and after the programs [29–31]. Similarly, there was no difference observed in social behavior before and after dolphin presentations [30]. These results suggest that these types of educational programs have a neutral or possibly a positive effect on dolphin social behavior.
Adding to the importance of social relationships for bottlenose dolphins, previous research has shown that when dolphins are given the choice between enrichment objects and social interactions, the latter was frequently selected [32]. This is not to imply that environmental enrichment is not important for dolphins, but to highlight the importance of social behavior for these species. In fact, multiple studies have shown that providing enrichment in the form of cognitive challenges increased social behavior [33,34]. However, those studies also resulted in the animals receiving a food reward making it is difficult to determine the primary factor responsible for impacting social behavior.
The goal of the current study was to examine factors associated with higher levels of social behavior for bottlenose dolphins under professional care. Specifically, environmental enrichment, animal training, and exhibit characteristics were examined to identify their relationship with social behavior for common bottlenose dolphins and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins.
Methods
Ethics statement
The project was reviewed and approved by veterinary and animal care staff at each zoological facility. The U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee also reviewed and approved this research project under proposal #123–2017.
Subjects and facilities
Focal animals for the project included common bottlenose dolphins and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins. Facilities caring for the focal subspecies that were accredited by either the Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums (AMMPA) or Association of Zoos and Aquariums (AZA) were invited to participate. The current study is one component of a larger study entitled “Towards Understanding the Welfare of Cetaceans in Zoos and Aquariums”. We used a semi-random sample design to select two animals from each facility accounting for sex and age. This was done to have a sample representative of the variability in dolphin age and sex across facilities. Data were collected on 86 dolphins from 40 facilities during two different five-week periods. Data were collected on six dolphins during the first five-week period but not the second five-week period. Data from eight dolphins were collected during the second five-week period that were not part of the first five-week period. Data were also collected on two dolphins that changed facilities between the two five-week periods. S4 Appendix 1 in S4 File lists all dolphins that were part of the study including sex, age, and total minutes visible.
Data collection
Data were collected during two five-week periods between July and November of 2018 and from January through April of 2019. During each of the five-week periods data were collected by videotaping focal animals three times a week. Videotaping occurred during one of three time periods: morning (8:00–11:00), mid-day (11:00–14:00), and afternoon (14:00–17:00). Animal care staff or interns at each facility were requested to videotape the animals to ensure accurate identification of the animals. Table 1 displays the counterbalanced recording schedule for all facilities across dolphins, day, and time periods. Animal care staff and interns were allowed to videotape at any point within the assigned time period. Videographers were instructed to not film within 20 minutes before or following a dolphin presentation, interaction program, training session, or research session. Before data collection began at each facility, the videographer spent a week (based on the schedule) standing in the filming location to habituate the dolphins to the person filming. They were also informed not to interact (eye contact or engaging with) with any of the animals while recording. The name of the dolphin, exhibit code, time, date, identification number, and observation numbers were written on a piece of paper and held in frame before each observation began. Observations were 25 minutes in length resulting in a total of 375 minutes of data during each five-week data collection period of the study. A Fuji Film XP120 waterproof video camera with a polarizing filter was used reduce glare. To ensure data were comparable across facilities, recording was done from above water because not all facilities had underwater viewing.
As previously noted, the cameras were fixed with polarizing filters to reduce the glare. However, for some outdoor facilities the glare was too intense, and dolphins were not visible for long periods of time. In addition, for some of the open ocean facilities animals were also not visible for long periods of time due to turbidity changes from particulate, algae, or plankton. In order to ensure reliable and valid data, for each dolphin, data were analyzed for either the first or second five-week data collection period, using a criterion of at least 240 minutes of visibility. The criterion of 240 minutes was chosen as it would have required at least 10 observations over the five-week period and the animals would have been required to be visible for the majority of the total possible time filmed. Dolphins that were visible less than 240 minutes during both the first and second five-week data collection period were dropped from further analysis. Dolphins with more than 240 minutes total time visible during both five-week data collection periods had the second period dropped from further analysis. The second data collection period was dropped in this scenario even when meeting the 240-minute criteria due to statistical analysis excluding rows with missing data. Finally, dolphins with more than 240 minutes for only the first five-week data collection period were retained for analysis. A single five-week data collection period was selected because dolphins without data in both five-week periods would have been excluded entirely when building the generalized estimating equation (GEE) models, further reducing the sample size. The decision resulted in the largest possible sample size to investigate variability across facilities rather than within individual dolphins. A chi-square test of significance was used to ensure resulting sample size was not significantly different from original sample based on sex or habitat type and an independent t-test was used to ensure there were no differences based on age.
Independent variables
Independent variables were selected to examine a variety of animal care and management factors as well as exhibit characteristics that may be associated with social behavior. These variables were created from a survey that was sent to each facility to detail their habitat characteristics, environmental enrichment program, and animal training routine for the dolphins examined in this study. The full list of independent variables can be found in Table 2 along with definitions. Any independent variables that were calculated from the management survey and not direct data are detailed by [41].
Statistical analysis
All descriptive statistics for independent and dependent variables can be found in Supplementary Document S1 in S1 File. All statistical models were examined using GEE given the non-normal distribution of the data in SPSS Version 27. GEE benefits analysis when variables are not normally distributed as they do not require transformations which makes interpretation of results easier [42,43]. Individual dolphin was the unit of analysis for all models while controlling for facility as a within-subject variable and any significant demographic variables (age and sex). Step one of building the models required examining univariate relationships and only variables with p < 0.15 were used to develop and examine the multivariate models [44,45]. Final models were selected based on the best quasi-likelihood under the independence model criterion (QIC) and significance for independent variables [44,46]. The final models that were considered with significant independent variables and the lowest QIC values are in Supplementary Document S2 in S2 File.
Results
Based on the minimum criteria of being visible for greater than 240 minutes, the final sample size included 47 dolphins from 25 accredited zoological facilities. Dolphins ranged from 4 to 47 years of age (average 19.68 ± 12.39 SD). The resulting sample size was not statistically different from the original sample size based on sex (χ2(1, N = 133) = 0.01, p > 0.05), age (t (131) = -1.105, p > 0.05), or habitat type (χ2(1, N = 133) = 3.016, p > 0.05). There was a total of 43 common bottlenose dolphins (91.3%) and 4 Indo-pacific bottlenose dolphins (8.7%) with 28 males and 19 females. Across all individuals throughout the five-week period, group active ranged from 0.00 to 33.79 percent of visible scans while interact with conspecific ranged from a rate of 0.00 to 0.72 per minute visible. Table 3 highlights the rates of aggressive behavior observed throughout the study.
Following univariate analysis (Tables 4 and 5), eight variables were considered for multivariate models for group active and eight variables were considered for multivariate models for interact with conspecific. Descriptive statistics for all independent variables considered for multivariate models are listed in Tables 6 and 7.
Multivariate models for group active were examined using sex and age (demographic), enrichment diversity index, enrichment schedule, and frequency of new enrichment (environmental enrichment), training duration (animal training), habitat type, and social management (habitat characteristics). Multivariate models for interact with conspecific were examined using sex (demographic), enrichment diversity index, enrichment schedule, and frequency of new enrichment (environmental enrichment), maximum number of interaction guests (animal training), day time spatial experience, length of habitat, and social management (habitat characteristics). The final multivariate model for group active included sex, enrichment schedule, and frequency of new enrichment (Table 8). Female dolphins displayed group active less when compared to male dolphins (β = -0.036, p = 0.017). Dolphins that were provided enrichment on a random schedule displayed group active less than animals provided enrichment on a predictable schedule (β = -0.053, p = 0.002). Additionally, dolphins that were provided new forms of enrichment twice per year or monthly/weekly displayed group active more than animals provided new forms of enrichment annually or less frequently (twice per year: β = 0.065, p < 0.001; monthly/weekly: β = 0.055, p = 0.002). The final multivariate model for interact with conspecific included enrichment schedule, frequency of new enrichment, and day time spatial experience (Table 9). Similar to group active, dolphins that were provided enrichment on a random schedule interact with conspecific less than animals provided enrichment on a predictable schedule (β = -0.078, p = 0.025). Also similar to group active, dolphins that were provided new forms of enrichment twice per year or monthly/weekly interact with conspecific more than animals provided new forms of enrichment annually or less frequently (twice per year: β = 0.115, p = 0.002; monthly/weekly: β = 0.118, p = 0.003). Finally, there was an inverse relationship found between interact with conspecific and daytime spatial experience β = -0.057, p = 0.045).
Discussion
It is important to understand factors that are associated with species-appropriate levels of social behavior for bottlenose dolphins under professional care. In the current study, the behaviors of group active and interact with conspecific were used to explore factors that are associated with social behavior. Given the low levels of aggression observed throughout the study, the factors associated with group active and interact with conspecific are associated with primarily affiliative behavior in bottlenose dolphins. Additionally, previous research has suggested that tail slaps, one of the behaviors listed as aggressive in the current study, may not always be a form of aggression similar to an animal breaching [47]. While female dolphins displayed group active less than males, and day time spatial experience was inversely related to interact with conspecific, the majority of significant predictors were variables associated with environmental enrichment. These included both the timing of enrichment as well as the frequency with which animals were provided with new forms of environmental enrichment.
In the current study, the timing of environmental enrichment refers to the randomness in temporal predictability. Dolphins on a more predictable schedule were more likely to display both group active and interact with conspecific compared to dolphins provided enrichment on a random schedule. Previous research on the predictability of receiving environmental enrichment for animals under professional care is scarce. More research has been conducted on the temporal predictability of receiving food, another positive experience in the care of animals. A review of the research on predictability of favorable events suggests that a random temporal or unpredictable schedule would be best if cues as to when food would be provided were predictable [48]. However, those authors note that this has been studied across few taxonomic groups and could differ depending on the species. The level of predictability in receiving food in relation to activity levels and behavioral diversity was observed in fennec fox (Vulpes zerda) [49]. Results suggested that moderate levels of temporal predictability optimized both activity levels and behavioral diversity when compared to 100% predictable or 100% unpredictable food availability. This would suggest the semi-random schedule of enrichment might be optimal for dolphins, but was not the case in the current study for social behavior. Instead, a more predictable schedule was associated with higher levels of both group active and interact with conspecific. Additionally, a random schedule of enrichment also related to lower energy expenditure in bottlenose dolphins [50], which may be accounted for by the lower rates of group active and interact with conspecific observed in the current study. Moving forward, animal care specialists that care for bottlenose dolphins can explore moderate levels of randomization for enrichment to enhance social behavior, but a completely random schedule would not be advisable. Additionally, a recent study suggested that providing enrichment directly following training sessions may not be appropriate given the high levels of synchronous swimming observed [23]. This type of behavior has been demonstrated as beneficial for dolphin’s social bonds and welfare and providing enrichment could potentially interfere with these important behaviors [51,52]. If providing enrichment on a completely predictable schedule, it would be important for facilities to determine the best possible time based on the behavior of those specific animals.
In contrast, providing new forms of enrichment on weekly/monthly or twice annual schedule was positively associated with both group active and interact with conspecific when compared to providing it yearly or less frequently. Previous research has shown that the frequency of providing new forms of enrichment can be just as important as the type of enrichment provided [53]. For example, pigs quickly habituated to objects existing within their enclosure, but exploratory behavior was maintained by consistently replacing old items with new items [54]. Recently, the frequency of providing new enrichment has also been shown to positively relate to energy expenditure, distance traveled, and behavioral diversity, all likely demonstrating positive welfare [50,55]. Given the intelligence of bottlenose dolphins [56], providing new forms of enrichment on a regular basis would likely be beneficial and promote positive welfare. Previous research has shown that dolphins display anticipatory behavior before receiving enrichment, and the amount of anticipatory behavior correlates with the duration of use of the enrichment [57]. This provides further support of the importance of environmental enrichment as anticipatory behavior can be an indicator of motivation [58].
The final relationships observed with group active and interact with conspecific included day time spatial experience and sex. Day time spatial experience was inversely related to interact with conspecific suggesting that dolphins interact more in smaller habitats. This relationship could potentially be explained by density of individuals, or having the dolphins in a larger habitat, decreasing the likelihood of interaction. Previous research has shown similar results, with bottlenose dolphins displaying less social behavior in larger habitats [27]. However, this change in social behavior could also be due to animals in the larger habitats spending more time exploring the environment in the open “ocean pen” that could have other forms of wildlife. We would not recommend creating smaller habitats to encourage appropriate levels of social behavior. Instead, it would be more important to focus on building strong social groups and bonds through the relationships observed in the current study with environmental enrichment.
Finally, females were less likely to display group active when compared to males. This could possibly be explained by the fact that male dolphins in the wild create complex lifelong alliances and affiliative social behavior would help in forming and maintaining those relationships [59,60]. An additional explanation could be that we only examined one form of swimming pattern in group active, and it is quite possible that if synchronous or group swimming had been included, the females would have been observed at higher levels. Previous research has determined this type of synchronous swimming is an important behavior for developing social bonds in dolphins [51,52]. Future research should also examine how habitat characteristics, environmental enrichment, and training programs impact other forms of social behavior. Given the current findings and fact that group composition was not examined in the current study, there are few recommendations we could make based on the relationship observed with sex.
It is clear that facilities and their staff that care for bottlenose dolphins can utilize enrichment strategies to enhance species-appropriate levels of social behavior such as group active or interact with conspecific. Previous research has found that cognitive enrichment [33] and other forms of environmental enrichment [61] increase levels of social play. However, the former study noted an increase in social play specifically when dolphins acquired a gelatin ball, so play could be a result of the puzzle or simply having a new consumable object. Similarly, engaging in a problem-solving task as a form of environmental enrichment increased social behavior for a group of bottlenose dolphins at another facility, but the animals also received a food reward as part of the puzzle [34]. However, another study which utilized a collaborative cognitive challenge with bottlenose dolphins found no impact on social behavior [62]. With the recommendation to find new ways of providing new forms of enrichment on a continuous basis, cognitive enrichment may be one way to facilitate being creative with an enrichment program. However, staff would be encouraged to examine the impact of any new enrichment for the animals under their professional care to ensure it is having a positive effect on their welfare.
Ultimately, ensuring that bottlenose dolphins under professional care have the ability to socialize at species-appropriate levels is important. Previous research has shown that dolphins that display more social affiliative behavior judge ambiguous cues more optimistically [51]. Ensuring that animals are in a positive emotional state is critical to the welfare of bottlenose dolphins under professional care. Moving forward, animal care staff can utilize aspects of enrichment programs found to relate to social behavior.
One of the limitations of the current study was the decrease in subjects as a result of issues with visibility. However, this is still the largest multi-institutional study on cetacean welfare. Additionally, we were unable to control for subspecies in the current analysis given the small number of Tursiops aduncus. However, given their close relatedness we do not believe this to be a significant limitation. The significant and non-significant relationships observed can help drive animal management decisions regarding the social behavior of bottlenose dolphins under professional care. Future research should examine additional factors related to ensuring species-appropriate levels of social behavior given the highly complex social nature of the species. Given the importance of social behavior, this in turn can help ensure that dolphins are experiencing positive welfare and can continue to inspire people to become engaged in conservation activities [63].
Supporting information
S1 File. Descriptive statistics Miller et al social behavior.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253732.s001
(XLSX)
S2 File. Model selection Miller et al social behavior.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253732.s002
(XLSX)
S3 File. Striking image Miller et al social behavior.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253732.s003
(TIFF)
S4 File. Appendix1 Miller et al social behavior.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253732.s004
(XLSX)
Acknowledgments
We would like to start by thanking Joy Mench and Melinda Conners for their significant efforts at the beginning of the project assisting with project design and review of methods. We would also like to thank Jocelyn Woods for all of her efforts in behavioral coding, data entry and data processing. Thank you to the interns for their efforts coding videos and sorting samples. In addition, special thanks to the Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums and the Association of Zoos and Aquariums for support throughout the duration of the project. We would also like to thank Sarah Breen-Bartecki and Bill Zeigler for their continued support and Rita Stacey and the Seven Seas staff for all of their original efforts. Finally, a sincere thank you to the people at each of the following facilities for participating in this study: Cabo Dolphins, Chicago Zoological Society–Brookfield Zoo, Delphinus Playa Mujeres, Delphinus Puerto Morelos, Delphinus Punta Cancún, Delphinus Riviera Maya, Delphinus Xcaret, Delphinus Xel-Há, Dolphin Adventure, Dolphin Discovery Akumal, Dolphin Discovery Cozumel, Dolphin Discovery Dreams, Dolphin Discovery Isla Mujeres, Dolphin Discovery Mahahual, Dolphin Discovery Maroma, Dolphin Discovery Moon Palace, Dolphin Discovery Ocho Rios, Dolphin Discovery Riviera Maya, Dolphin Island–Resorts World Sentosa, Dolphin Quest Hawaii, Dolphin Quest Oahu, Dolphin Quest Bermuda, Georgia Aquarium/Marineland Dolphin Adventure, Gulf World Marine Park, Gulfarium Marine Adventure Park, Indianapolis Zoo, Interactive Aquarium Cancun, John G. Shedd Aquarium, Lisbon Zoo Portugal, Loro Parque and Loro Parque Fundación, Mystic Aquarium, National Aquarium, Ocean Park Corporation, Sea Life Park Hawaii, Sea World Parks, Texas State Aquarium, The Mirage Dolphin Habitat and The Mirage Casino-Hotel LLC, The Seas® Epcot® Walt Disney World® Resort, and the U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program.
References
- 1.
Connor RC, Wells RS, Mann JA, Read AJ. The bottlenose dolphin: social relationships in a fission-fusion society. In: Mann J, Conner RC, Tyack P, Whitehead H, editors. Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2000; pp. 91–126.
- 2.
Scott MD, Chivers SJ. Distribution and herd structure of bottlenose dolphins in the eastern topical pacific ocean. In: Leatherwood S, Reeves RR, editors. The bottlenose dolphin. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.; 1990; pp. 387–402.
- 3. Shane SH, Wells RS, Würsig B. Ecology, behavior and social organization of the bottlenose dolphin: a review. Mar Mamm Sci. 1986;2: 34–63.
- 4. Lear RJ, Bryden MM. A study of the bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus, in eastern Australian waters. Canberra: Australian National Parks and Wildlife Service; 1980.
- 5. Möller LM, Harcourt RG. Social dynamics and activity patterns of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Jervis Bay, southeastern Australia. Proc Linn Soc N S W. 1998;120: 181–189.
- 6. Scarpaci C, Bigger SW, Saville TA, Nugegoda D. The bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus in the southern end of Port Phillip Bay: behavioural characteristics in spring and summer. Vic Nat. 2000;117: 4–9.
- 7.
Shane SH. Comparison of bottlenose dolphin behavior in Texas and Florida, with a critique of methods for studying dolphin behavior. In: Leatherwood S, Reeves RR, editors. The bottlenose dolphin. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.; 1990; pp. 541–558.
- 8. Galezo AA, Foroughirad V, Krzyszczyk E, Frère CH, Mann J. Juvenile social dynamics reflect adult reproductive strategies in bottlenose dolphins. Behav Ecol. 2020;31: 1159–71.
- 9.
Wells RS, Scott MD, Irvine AB. The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins. In: Genoways HH, editor. Current mammalogy. Boston: Springer; 1987; pp. 247–305.
- 10.
Wells RS. The role of long-term study in understanding the social structure of a bottlenose dolphin community. In: Pryor K, Norris KS, editors. Dolphin societies: discoveries and puzzles. Berkley: University of California Press; 1991; pp. 199–225.
- 11. Smolker RA, Richards AF, Connor RC, Pepper JW. Sex differences in patterns of association among Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins. Behav. 1992;123: 38–69.
- 12.
Connor RC, Smolker RA, Richards AF. Dolphin alliances and coalitions. In: Harcourt AH, De Waal FBM, editors. Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992; pp. 414–443.
- 13.
Wells RS. The social ecology of inshore odontocetes. In: Herman LM, editor. Cetacean behavior: mechanisms and functions. New York: Wiley; 1980; pp. 263–317.
- 14. Gazda SK, Connor RC, Edgar RK, Cox F. A division of labour with role specialization in group–hunting bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off Cedar Key, Florida. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;272: 135–140. pmid:15695203
- 15. Brando S, Broom DM, Acasuso-Rivero C, Clark F. Optimal marine mammal welfare under human care: Current efforts and future directions. Behav Processes. 2018;156: 16–36. pmid:28927965
- 16. Ramp C, Hagen W, Palsbøll P, Bérubé M, Sears R. Age-related multi-year associations in female humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;64: 1563–1576.
- 17. Schülke O, Bhagavatula J, Vigilant L, Ostner J. Social bonds enhance reproductive success in male macaques. Curr Biol. 2010;20: 2207–2210. pmid:21093261
- 18. Brent LJ, Heilbronner SR, Horvath JE, Gonzalez-Martinez J, Ruiz-Lambides A, Robinson AG, et al. Genetic origins of social networks in rhesus macaques. Sci Rep. 2013;3: 1042. pmid:23304433
- 19. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science. 2003; 302: 1231–1234. pmid:14615543
- 20. Archie EA, Tung J, Clark M, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Social affiliation matters: both same-sex and opposite-sex relationships predict survival in wild female baboons. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;281: 20141261. pmid:25209936
- 21. Frère CH, Krützen M, Mann J, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB. Social and genetic interactions drive fitness variation in a free-living dolphin population. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107: 19949–19954. pmid:21041638
- 22.
Wells RS. Reproduction in wild bottlenose dolphins: overview of patterns observed during a long-term study. In: Duffield D, Robeck TR, editors. The Bottlenose Dolphin Breeding Workshop. Silver Springs, MD: Association of Zoos and Aquariums, Marine Mammal Taxon Advisory Group; 2000; pp. 57–73.
- 23. Clegg IL, Rödel HG, Cellier M, Vink D, Michaud I, Mercera B, et al. Schedule of human-controlled periods structures bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) behavior in their free-time. J Comp Psychol. 2017;131: 214–224. pmid:28358547
- 24. Serres A, Hao Y, Wang D. Body contacts and social interactions in captive odontocetes are influenced by the context: An implication for welfare assessment. Animals. 2020;10: 924. pmid:32466514
- 25. Dudzinski KM, Gregg JD, Paulos RD, Kuczaj SA II. A comparison of pectoral fin contact behaviour for three distinct dolphin populations. Behav Processes. 2010;84: 559–567. pmid:20176094
- 26. Dudzinski KM, Gregg J, Melillo-Sweeting K, Seay B, Levengood A, Kuczaj SA II. Tactile contact exchanges between dolphins: self-rubbing versus inter-individual contact in three species from three geographies. Int J Comp Psychol. 2012;25: 21–43.
- 27. Ruiz CU, Sánchez A, Maldonado FG. Social and individual behavior of a group of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in open and closed facilities. Vet Méx. 2009;40: 381–387.
- 28. Kyngdon DJ, Minot EO, Stafford KJ. Behavioural responses of captive common dolphins Delphinus delphis to a ‘Swim-with-Dolphin’programme. App Anim Behav Sci. 2003;81: 163–170.
- 29. Brando S, Kooistra N, Hosey G. Pre and post session behaviour of captive bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus involved in “Swim-with-Dolphin” events. J Zoo Aquar Res. 2019;7: 195–202.
- 30. Miller LJ, Mellen J, Greer T, Kuczaj SA. The effects of education programmes on Atlantic bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) behaviour. Anim Welf. 2011;20: 159–172.
- 31. Trone M, Kuczaj S, Solangi M. Does participation in Dolphin–Human Interaction Programs affect bottlenose dolphin behaviour? Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2005;93: 363–374.
- 32. Delfour F, Helen B. Assessing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Zoo Biol. 2012;31: 137–150. pmid:22083895
- 33. Clark FE, Davies SL, Madigan AW, Warner AJ, Kuczaj SA. Cognitive enrichment for bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Evaluation of a novel underwater maze device. Zoo Biol. 2013;32: 608–619. pmid:24018985
- 34. Lauderdale LK, Miller LJ. Efficacy of an interactive apparatus as environmental enrichment for common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Anim Welf. 2020;29: 379–386.
- 35.
Shane SH. Behavior and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin at Sanibel Island, Florida. In: Leatherwood S, Reeves RR, editors. The Bottlenose Dolphin. San Diego: Academic Press; 1990. pp. 245–265.
- 36.
Defran RH, Pryor K. The behavior and training of cetaceans in captivity. In: Herman LM, editor. Cetacean behavior: Mechanisms and functions. New York: Wiley; 1980. pp. 319–362.
- 37. Mann J, Smuts B. Behavioral development in wild bottlenose dolphin newborns (Tursiops sp.). Behav, 1999;136: 529–566.
- 38. Samuels A, Gifford T. A quantitative assessment of dominance relations among bottlenose dolphins. Mar Mamm Sci. 1997;13: 70–99.
- 39. Tavolga MC, Essapian FS. The behavior of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): mating, pregnancy, parturition, and mother-infant behavior. Zoologica. 1957;42: 11–31.
- 40. Wursig B, Wursig M. Day and night of the dolphin. Nat Hist. 1979;88: 60–67.
- 41. Lauderdale LK, Walsh MT, Mellon JD, Granger DA, Miller LJ. Habitat characteristics, environmental enrichment, and training of common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus). Plos One. 2021.
- 42.
Kowalski J, Tu XM. Modern applied U-statistics. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2008.
- 43.
Tang W, He H, Tu X. Applied categorical and count data analysis. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2012.
- 44. Greco BJ, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Leighty KA, Mellen J, Mason GJ, et al. The days and nights of zoo elephants: using epidemiology to better understand stereotypic behavior of African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in North American zoos. PLoS One. 2016;11: e0144276. pmid:27416071
- 45. Holdgate MR, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Miller LJ, Soltis J, Andrews J, et al. Walking behavior of zoo elephants: associations between GPS-measured daily walking distances and environmental factors, social factors, and welfare indicators. PloS one. 2016;11: e0150331. pmid:27414411
- 46. Pan W. Akaike’s information criterion in generalized estimating equations. Biometrics. 2001;57: 120–125. pmid:11252586
- 47.
Pryor K. Reinforcement training as interspecific communication. In: Schusterman RJ, Thomas JA, Wood FG, Schusterman R, editors. Dolphin cognition and behavior: A comparative approach. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1986. pp. 253–266.
- 48. Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM. Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007;102: 223–245.
- 49. Watters JV, Miller JT, Sullivan TJ. Note on optimizing environmental enrichment: A study of fennec fox and zoo guests. Zoo Biol. 2011;30: 647–654. pmid:22147593
- 50. Lauderdale LK, Shorter KA, Zhang D, Gabaldon J, Mellen JD, Walsh MT, et al. Bottlenose dolphin habitat and animal management factors related to activity and distance traveled in zoos and aquariums. PlosOne, 2021.
- 51. Clegg IL, Rödel HG, Delfour F. Bottlenose dolphins engaging in more social affiliative behaviour judge ambiguous cues more optimistically. Behav Brain Res. 2017;322: 115–122. pmid:28110003
- 52. Dudzinski KM, Ribic CA. Pectoral fin contact as a mechanism for social bonding among dolphins. Anim Behav Cog. 2017;4: 30–48.
- 53. Kuczaj S, Lacinak T, Fad O, Trone M, Solangi M, Ramos J. Keeping environmental enrichment enriching. Int J Comp Psychol. 2002;15: 127–137.
- 54. Mkwanazi MV, Ncobela CN, Kanengoni AT, Chimonyo M. Effects of environmental enrichment on behaviour, physiology and performance of pigs—A review. Asian-Australas J Anim Sci. 2019;32: 1–13. pmid:28728387
- 55. Miller LJ, Lauderdale LK, Mellen JD, Walsh MT, Granger DA. Relationships between animal management and habitat characteristics with two potential indicators of welfare for bottlenose dolphins under professional care. PlosOne, 2021.
- 56.
Herman LM. Exploring the cognitive world of the bottlenosed dolphin. In: Beckoff M, Allen C, Burghardt GM, editors. The cognitive animal. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2002. pp. 275–283.
- 57. Clegg IL, Rödel HG, Boivin X, Delfour F. Looking forward to interacting with their caretakers: dolphins’ anticipatory behaviour indicates motivation to participate in specific events. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2018;202: 85–93.
- 58. Spruijt BM, van den Bos R, Pijlman FT. A concept of welfare based on reward evaluating mechanisms in the brain: anticipatory behaviour as an indicator for the state of reward systems. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001;72: 145–171. pmid:11278033
- 59. Connor RC, Krützen M. Male dolphin alliances in Shark Bay: changing perspectives in a 30-year study. Anim Behavi. 2015;103: 223–235.
- 60.
Wells RS. Social structure and life history of bottlenose dolphins near Sarasota Bay, Florida: insights from four decades and five generations. In: Yamagiwa J, Karczmarski L, editors. Primates and cetaceans: field research and conservation of complex mammalian societies. Tokyo: Springer; 2014. pp. 149–172.
- 61. Serres A, Delfour F. Environmental changes and anthropogenic factors modulate social play in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Zoo Biol. 2017;36: 99–111. pmid:28221675
- 62. Matrai E, Ng AK, Chan MM, Gendron SM, Dudzinski KM. Testing use of a potential cognitive enrichment device by an Indo‐Pacific bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops aduncus). Zoo Biol. 2020; 39:156–167. pmid:32128851
- 63. Miller LJ, Zeigler‐Hill V, Mellen J, Koeppel J, Greer T, Kuczaj S. Dolphin shows and interaction programs: benefits for conservation education?. Zoo Biol. 2013;32: 45–53. pmid:22622768